Skip to main content
. 2020 Jun 12;11:1041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01041

TABLE 2.

Three emergent levels in the evolution of consciousness, and the new features at each level (adapted from Feinberg and Mallatt, 2019).

Level 1. Life
A. Simplest system that has life is the cell, with bacteria and archaea being the simplest cells
B. First appearance: ∼3.7 billion years ago
C. Emergent structures: macromolecules (proteins, nucleic acids, sugars, lipids), organelles, cells
D. Emergent processes:
 ∙ The strong boundary condition of embodiment: semipermeable membrane encloses cell contents to concentrate the chemical reactions and keep the reaction products from diffusing away (Morowitz, 2002)
 ∙ Information-based organization, directed by DNA/genes, and coded to specify the chemical reactions; the gene-coded “purpose” of Mayr (2004)
 ∙ Metabolism, to convert food to energy (ATP) and make new cellular materials; efficient use of energy and of vital molecules slows entropy (energy waste lost as heat)
 ∙ Self-upkeep and goal-directed properties (Mayr, 2004; Godfrey-Smith, 2019)
 ∙ Growth and self-replication/reproduction
 ∙ Sensitivity and movement
 ∙ Homeostasis: maintaining a constant internal environment in response to changes in the external environment
 ∙ Adaptation to the environment
 ∙ Evolution; natural selection becomes the pruning process that limits the possibilites of evolutionary change and of what features emerge in the system from this level onward (Morowitz, 2002)
E. Adaptive advantage of this emergence: world’s first self-perpetuation of complex systems over time
Level 2. Nervous systems, From Reflexes Through the Level of Simple, Core Brains (Not Conscious)
A. Organisms possessing it: most invertebrate animals; for example, most worms
B. First appearance: ∼ 580 million years ago
C. Emergent structures: multicellular animal body with different cell types including neurons, neural reflex arcs, sensory receptors, motor effectors (muscles, glands); nerve nets, then a consolidation into central and peripheral nervous system; some of the animals have a simple brain with movement-patterning circuits; the sensory receptors are mechano-, chemo- and photoreceptor cells
D. Emergent processes:
 ∙ Speed: neurons transmit signals fast enough to control the actions of a large, multicellular body in response to sensory stimuli
 ∙ Connectivity: reflex arcs and neuron networks coordinate all the parts of a large body
 ∙ Core-brain processes:
  ∘ Control complex reflexes for inner-body homeostasis
  ∘ Basic motor programs and central pattern generators for rhythmic locomotion, feeding, and other stereotyped movements
  ∘ Set the level of arousal
E. Adaptive advantages of this emergence: Sustains a large body that can move far through the environment, following sensory stimuli to find food, safety, and mates
Level 3. Consciousness
A. Organisms possessing it: vertebrates, arthropods, cephalopod molluscs
B. First appearance: 560–520 million years ago
C and D. Emergent structures and processes: the special neurobiological features of consciousness:
 ∙ Neural complexity (more than exists in a simple, core brain)
  ∘ Brain with many neurons (>100,000?)
  ∘ Many subtypes of neurons
 ∙ Elaborated sensory organs
  ∘ Image-forming eyes, receptor organs for touch, hearing, smell
 ∙ Neural hierarchies with neuron-neuron interactions
  ∘ Extensive reciprocal communication in and between the pathways for the different senses
  ∘ Brain has many neural computing modules and networks that are distributed but integrated (separate but highly interconnected), leading to local functional specialization plus global coherence (Nunez, 2016; Mogensen and Overgaard, 2017) (see Figure 3)
  ∘ Synchronized communication by brain-wave oscillations; neural spike trains form representational codes
  ∘ The higher levels allow the complex processing and unity of consciousness
  ∘ Higher brain levels exert more influence on the lower levels such as motor neurons, for increased top-down causality
  ∘ Hierarchies that let consciousness model events a fraction of a second in advance (Clark, 2013; Gershman et al., 2015; Jylkkä and Railo, 2019; Solms, 2019)
 ∙ Pathways that create mapped mental images or affective states
  ∘ Neurons are arranged in topographic sensory maps of the outside world and body structures
  ∘ Valence coding of good and bad, for affective states
  ∘ Feed into premotor brain regions to motivate, choose, and guide movements in space
 ∙ Brain mechanisms for selective attention and arousal
 ∙ Memory, short-term or longer
E. Adaptive advantages of this emergence:
 ∙ Consciousness organizes large amounts of sensory information into a detailed, unified simulation of the world, so the subject can choose the best behavioral responses
  ∘ This is a large, effective, expansion of the basic life-property of sensing the environment and responding
 ∙ With mental maps, one can navigate through space even when no sensory stimuli for guidance are present
 ∙ Consciousness ranks all the sensed stimuli by importance, by assigning affects to them (good, bad), thereby simplifying decisions on how to respond (Cabanac, 1996)
 ∙ Consciousness provides behavioral flexibility: adjusts fast to new stimuli so it deals well with the changing challenges of new environments