Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2020 Jun 10;9:e57215. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57215

Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition

Bon-Mi Gu 1, Robert Schmidt 2, Joshua D Berke 1,3,
Editors: Daeyeol Lee4, Richard B Ivry5
PMCID: PMC7314538  PMID: 32519952

Abstract

Flexible behavior requires restraint of actions that are no longer appropriate. This behavioral inhibition critically relies on frontal cortex - basal ganglia circuits. Within the basal ganglia, the globus pallidus pars externa (GPe) has been hypothesized to mediate selective proactive inhibition: being prepared to stop a specific action, if needed. Here we investigate population dynamics of rat GPe neurons during preparation-to-stop, stopping, and going. Rats selectively engaged proactive inhibition towards specific actions, as shown by slowed reaction times (RTs). Under proactive inhibition, GPe population activity occupied state-space locations farther from the trajectory followed during normal movement initiation. Furthermore, the state-space locations were predictive of distinct types of errors: failures-to-stop, failures-to-go, and incorrect choices. Slowed RTs on correct proactive trials reflected starting bias towards the alternative action, which was overcome before progressing towards action initiation. Our results demonstrate that rats can exert cognitive control via strategic adjustments to their GPe network state.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

Our capacity for self-restraint is critical for adaptive behavior. Dysfunctions in behavioral inhibition are involved in many human disorders, including drug addiction (Ersche et al., 2012). A standard test of behavioral inhibition is the stop-signal task (Logan et al., 1984; Verbruggen et al., 2019), in which subjects attempt to respond rapidly to a Go cue, but withhold responding if the Go cue is quickly followed by a Stop cue. The stop-signal task has been invaluable for revealing specific cortical-basal ganglia mechanisms involved in both movement initiation (‘Going’; e.g. Hanes and Schall, 1996) and inhibition (‘Stopping’; e.g. Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Eagle et al., 2008). ‘Reactive’ inhibition – making quick use of a Stop cue – appears to involve at least two distinct mechanisms (Schmidt and Berke, 2017): a rapid Pause process mediated via the subthalamic nucleus (STN; Aron and Poldrack, 2006; Schmidt et al., 2013) followed by a Cancel process achieved through pallidostriatal inhibition (Mallet et al., 2016).

Behavioral inhibition can also be ‘proactive’: restraint of actions, in advance of any Stop cue. Proactive inhibition may be particularly relevant to human life (Aron, 2011; Jahanshahi et al., 2015). Whereas reactive inhibition typically involves a global, transient arrest of actions and thoughts (Wessel and Aron, 2017), proactive inhibition can be selectively directed to a particular action (Cai et al., 2011). A key behavioral signature of proactive inhibition is slowing of reaction times (RTs) for that action, when the anticipated Stop cue does not actually occur (e.g. Verbruggen and Logan, 2009; Chikazoe et al., 2009; Zandbelt et al., 2013). This overt behavioral signature presumably relies on covert shifts in information processing, yet the nature of these shifts is unclear. In some studies, fitting of models to behavioral data has suggested that slowed RTs reflect raising of a decision ‘threshold’ (Verbruggen and Logan, 2009; Jahfari et al., 2012), but other studies have found evidence for a slower rate of progression toward threshold instead (Dunovan et al., 2015).

The neural circuit mechanisms by which proactive control is achieved are also not well understood. It has been proposed that proactive inhibition critically depends on the basal ganglia ‘indirect’ pathway via GPe (Aron, 2011; Jahanshahi et al., 2015; Dunovan et al., 2015). Yet clear support for this hypothesis is sparse (Majid et al., 2013). There have been few electrophysiological studies of proactive inhibition at the level of individual neurons (Chen et al., 2010; Pouget et al., 2011; Hardung et al., 2017; Yoshida et al., 2018), and to our knowledge none in GPe. We therefore targeted GPe (often called simply GP in rodents) for investigating neural mechanisms of proactive control.

We also wished to integrate a dynamical systems approach into the study of behavioral inhibition, and the basal ganglia. Analysis of the collective dynamics of motor cortex neurons has provided insights into various aspects of movement control, including how brain networks may prepare actions without prematurely triggering them (Kaufman et al., 2014), and the origins of RT variability (Afshar et al., 2011). We demonstrate below that the analysis of GPe population activity can reveal distinct covert strategies underlying overt manifestations of proactive control.

Results

Action initiation is slower when a stop cue is expected

We trained rats in a modified version of our stop-signal task (Figure 1A; Leventhal et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2013; Mallet et al., 2016). Freely-moving rats poked their noses into a hole and maintained that position for a variable delay (500–1250 ms) before presentation of one of two Go cues (1 kHz or 4 kHz tone), instructing leftward or rightward movements respectively into an adjacent hole. If initiated rapidly (RT limit <800 ms), correct movements triggered delivery of a sugar pellet reward from a separate food hopper. On some trials the Go cue was quickly followed by a Stop cue (white noise burst), indicating that the rat instead needed to maintain its nose in the starting hole (for a total of 800 ms after Go cue onset) to trigger reward delivery. The delay between Go and Stop cue onsets (100–250 ms) ensured that stopping was sometimes successful and sometimes not. As expected, Failed Stop (error) trials had similar RTs to the faster part of the Go trial RT distribution (Figure 1B). This is consistent with the basic ‘race’ conceptual model of reactive inhibition (Logan et al., 1984): failures-to-stop typically occur when an underlying Go process evolves more quickly than average (Schmidt et al., 2013), and thus wins the race against a separate Stop process.

Figure 1. Reactive and Proactive Behavioral Inhibition.

(A) Left, operant box configuration; right, event sequence for Go and Stop trials. RT, reaction time; MT, movement time; SSD, stop-signal delay; Reward, delivery of a sugar pellet to the food port. (B) Left, distributions of Go and Failed-Stop RTs (on Maybe-Stop trials; shading, S.E.M. across n = 63 sessions). Failed-Stop RTs are similar to the faster part of the Go RT distribution, consistent with the ‘race’ model in which a relatively-fast Go process produces failures to stop. The tail of the Failed-Stop distribution (RT >500 ms) is presumed to reflect trials for which rats successfully responded to the Stop cue, but then failed to maintain holding until reward delivery (see Leventhal et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2013; Mayse et al., 2014). Right, proportions of failed and successful Stop trials after Contra and Ipsi Go cues. Error bars, S.E.M. across n = 63 sessions. (C) Trial start location indicates stop probabilities (locations counterbalanced across rats). In this example configuration recording from left GP, starting from the middle hole indicates the Maybe-stop Contra condition: Go cues instructing rightward movements might be followed by a Stop cue, but Go cues instructing leftward movements will not. (D) Proactive inhibition causes selective RT slowing for the Maybe-Stop direction (two-tail Wilcoxon signed rank tests on median RT for each session: contra cues in Maybe-Stop-contra versus No-Stop, z = 7.7, p=1.15 × 10−14; ipsi cues in Maybe-Stop-contra versus No-Stop, p=0.32). Additionally, under selective proactive inhibition rats were more likely to fail to respond quickly enough (RT limit errors; Wilcoxon signed rank tests, z = 7.2, p=5.41 × 10−13) and to select the wrong choice (uncued action direction; Wilcoxon signed rank tests, z = 7.0, p=2.59 × 10−12). Error bars, S.E.M. across n = 63 sessions. Only trials without a Stop cue are included here. RT limit error = Nose remained in Center port for >800 ms after Go cue onset; MT limit error = movement time between Center Out and Side port entry >500 ms.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Behavioral data for all sessions and for each individual animal.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Proactive slowing of RT is visible in aggregate across all recorded sessions (n = 251 sessions, from six rats), in both left and right directions. Shading indicates SEM across rats. (B) Cumulative density plots of RT for all sessions included in electrophysiology data analysis for each rat, in the same format as Figure 1. Left plots, comparison of Go RT and Stop-fail RT; right plots, selective proactive inhibition for movements contraversive to the recorded neurons.

To probe selective proactive inhibition we used a ‘Maybe-Stop versus No-Stop’ approach (Aron and Verbruggen, 2008). The three possible starting holes were associated with different Stop cue probabilities (Figure 1C): no possibility of Stop cue; 50% probability that a left Go cue (only) will be followed by the Stop cue; or 50% probability that a right Go cue (only) will be followed by the Stop cue. Our index of proactive inhibition was a preferential increase in RT for the Maybe-Stop direction, compared to the No-Stop conditions. Among rats that began learning this task variant, approximately half acquired clear proactive inhibition within 3 months of training (see Materials and methods), and were thus considered eligible for electrode implantation. Here we report behavioral and neural results for six rats for which we were able to obtain high-quality GP recordings as rats engaged proactive control.

We selected for further analysis those behavioral sessions (n = 63) with a significant proactive inhibition effect (i.e. longer RT when a Stop cue might occur; one-tail Wilcoxon rank sum test, p<0.05) and distinct GP single units (n = 376 neurons included). Prior work has shown particular basal ganglia involvement in the control of contraversive orienting-type movements (i.e. directed towards the opposite side; Carli et al., 1985; Isoda and Hikosaka, 2008; Schmidt et al., 2013; Leventhal et al., 2014). We therefore focused on proactive control of movements contraversive (‘contra’) to the recorded cell locations; for example we included a left GPe cell only if the rat demonstrated proactive control for rightward movements during that recording session. For included sessions, median RT for correct contra movements was 251 ms when the Stop cue could not occur (No-Stop), and 385 ms when the Stop cue could occur (Maybe-Stop) but did not. Results from all sessions, and from individual animals, are shown in Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

RT slowing due to proactive inhibition was highly selective to the Maybe-Stop direction (Figure 1D; Figure 1—figure supplement 1; for Maybe-Stop-Contra trials without a Stop cue, median ipsiversive (‘ipsi’) RT was unslowed at 264 ms). The Maybe-Stop condition was also associated with an increase in errors (Figure 1D), in particular not responding quickly enough to the Go cue that might be followed by Stop (RT limit error; RT >800 ms) and making the wrong choice (incorrect action selection). These error types are examined further below.

GP firing rate changes related to movement onset and proactive inhibition

We recorded individual neurons (n = 376) from a wide range of GP locations (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). As expected from prior studies (DeLong, 1971; Brotchie et al., 1991; Gardiner and Kitai, 1992; Turner and Anderson, 1997; Arkadir et al., 2004; Gage et al., 2010; Shin and Sommer, 2010; Schmidt et al., 2013; Yoshida and Tanaka, 2016; Mallet et al., 2016) GP neurons were tonically-active (mean session-wide firing rate, 28 Hz) with diverse, complex changes in firing patterns during task performance (Figure 2A). The majority of GP cells showed strongest firing rate changes (increases or decreases) when activity was aligned relative to movement onset, rather than to the Go or Stop cues (Figure 2C,D; see also Figure 2—figure supplement 1B for ipsi movement trials). Individual neurons showed greater changes for either contra or ipsi movements (Figure 2A,B), but these were about equally represented in the overall population (Figure 2B,E), and the average GP activity was similar for the two movement directions (at least until the movement was already underway; Figure 2B).

Figure 2. Movement-related activity of individual GP neurons.

(A) Four examples of single neurons, showing average firing rates (top) and spike rasters (bottom) aligned on movement onset (Center Out; correct No-Stop trials only). Activity for contra-, ipsi- movements are shown in blue and green respectively. (B) Top, averaged, Z-scored firing of GP cells around Center Out; time points when activity distinguishes movement direction are shown with thicker lines. Shaded band, +- S.E.M across n = 376 neurons. Bottom, fraction of neurons whose firing rate significantly distinguishes movement direction, across time (t-test for each neuron in each 50 ms bin, p<0.05). Higher firing rate for contra-, ipsi- shown in blue, green respectively. Horizontal grey lines indicate thresholds for a significant proportion of neurons (binomial test, p<0.05 without or with multiple-comparisons correction respectively) and bins that exceed these thresholds are filled in color. Many GP cells encoded movement direction even before Center-Out; this is less obvious after averaging. (C) Firing pattern of all GP cells (n = 376) on correct contra trials. Activity is scaled between minimum and maximum firing rate across alignments to Go cue (left), Center Out (middle) and the Stop cue (right). In each column cell order (top-bottom) is sorted using the time of peak deflection from average firing, separately for cells that showed bigger increases (top) or decreases (bottom). (D) GP population activity is more related to movements than cues. Scatter plots show peak deflections in firing rate (Z-scored) for each GP cell, comparing Center Out aligned data to Go cue aligned (top) or Stop cue aligned (bottom). Data included are 500 ms around alignment time. Indicated p-values are from Wilcoxon signed rank tests over the GP population; individual GP cells that showed significant differences are indicated with red points (t test, p<0.05). (E) Scatter plot indicates no overall movement direction bias. Same format, same statistical tests as D, but comparing peak deflections in Center Out aligned firing rate for contra, ipsi movements. (F) Top, comparing average firing between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions. On left, data are aligned on Go cue, including all Maybe-Stop-Contra trials (including both contra- and ipsi-instructing Go cues and Stop trials). On right, data are aligned on Center-Out (and does not include Stop cue trials). Bottom, proportion of neurons whose firing rate is significantly affected by proactive inhibition (same format as B; bins exceeding p<0.05 threshold without multiple comparisons correction are filled in light color, bins exceeding corrected threshold are filled in dark color). Although GP neurons significantly distinguished Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions at multiple time points before the Go cue, there was no single time point at which the proportion of individually-significant neurons became large. (G) Comparison of individual cell activity in Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions, during the 500 ms epoch immediately before the Go cue.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Further details of GP recordings.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Estimated locations of recorded units, within coronal atlas sections (Paxinos, 2006). (B) Firing pattern of all GP cells (n = 376) on ipsi trials, shown in the same format as Figure 2C. (C) Proactive effects on average GP firing. As Figure 2F, but dividing units into those that predominantly increase or decrease firing rate. (D). Duration of significant difference between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions, during the 500 ms before Go cue, for each neuron. Most units show a significant difference at some time, but very few show sustained changes with proactive inhibition. (E). Comparing average GP firing on Correct contra trials and error trials (wrong choices and RT limit errors).

We next examined how the activity of individual GP neurons is affected by proactive inhibition. As rats waited for the (unpredictably-timed) Go cue, average firing was similar between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions (Figure 2F), regardless of whether we examined cells that predominantly increase or decrease activity during movements (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C). We hypothesized that this average activity obscures a sizable GP subpopulation that consistently and persistently ‘encodes’ proactive control as rats wait. To search for this putative subpopulation we used a screening approach (similar to our prior work on reactive stopping; Schmidt et al., 2013; Mallet et al., 2016), comparing the Maybe-Stop-contra and No-Stop conditions. We did find that the fraction of GP cells that fired differently between these conditions was slightly greater than expected by chance (Figure 2F), consistent with GP involvement in proactive control. However, contrary to our hypothesis, we were not able to identify a clear subgroup of individual neurons that strongly and persistently distinguished between conditions (Figure 2G). Rather, proactive control was associated with altered activity in different subsets of GP neurons at various brief moments before the Go cue (Figure 2—figure supplement 1D).

Population trajectories during movement selection and initiation

We next hypothesized that these GP firing rate differences, though subtle and diverse at the single-cell level, are coordinated to produce clear, interpretable changes in population dynamics. To observe these dynamics we began by reducing the dimensionality of population activity (Cunningham and Yu, 2014), using principal component analysis (PCA). For each neuron we included normalized, averaged firing rates for a 500 ms epoch around movement onset (separately for contra and ipsi movements; Figure 3A). We used the first 10 principal components (PCs; Figure 3—figure supplement 1A) to define a 10-dimensional state-space, with GP population activity represented as a single point in this space. For visualization we display the first 3 PCs (which together account for 71% of total population variance; Figure 3B), although statistical analyses used all 10 PCs.

Figure 3. GP dynamics for Going and Stopping.

(A) PCA was performed using averaged, normalized firing rates for each GP cell, in a 500 ms epoch around Center Out for contra and ipsi movements (concatenated). (B) Variance explained by each of the first 10 PCs. (C) GP state-space trajectories for contra and ipsi movements (blue, green) within the first 3 PCs, shown from two different angles. Each small dot along the trajectory is separated by 4 ms. Trajectories begin at a similar mean location at the Go cue (diamonds), and diverge gradually until Center Out (large circles) then rapidly thereafter. ‘Initiation Axis’ joins the average position at Go cue and the average position at Center Out (black asterisk). ‘Selection Axis’ joins the means of each trajectory, colored asterisks. (D) Comparing state-space trajectories for Successful- and Failed-Stop trials. Same format and PCA space as C, but plotting trajectories aligned on the Stop cue (including both contra and ipsi trials). Filled circles indicate epochs of significant Euclidean distance between two trajectories (permutation test on each 4 ms time bin, p<0.05). (E) Permutation tests of whether the state-space positions for Successful- and Failed-Stop trials are significantly different, at either the Go cue (top) or the Stop cue (bottom). Positions are compared either in the 10-D PC space (Euclidean distance) or along the Initiation or Selection Axes. Grey distributions show surrogate data from 10000 random shuffles of trial types. Dark grey, most extreme 5% of distributions (one-tailed for Euclidean, 2-tailed for others). Red vertical lines show observed results (bright red, significant; dark red, n.s.). (F) Distance travelled along Initiation Axis for successful and failed Stop trials, aligned on either Go cue (left) or Stop cue (right). Thicker lines indicate epochs of significant difference between successful and failed Stop trajectories (permutation test on each 4 ms time bin, p<0.05). On Failed stops (only), activity has already evolved substantially by the time of the Stop cue.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Principal Components.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) The first 10 principal components. (B) Relative contributions of each PC to the Initiation and Selection Axes (i,e, the eigenvector of each Axis in the 10-PC space). (C) Weight of each GP neuron on the Initiation and Selection Axis. (D, E) Spatial arrangement of absolute weight values.

Within state space, population activity was very similar for contra and ipsi movements at the Go cue (Figure 3C), and initially evolved in a common direction before progressively separating into distinct trajectories (Video 1). We used the common direction to define an ‘Initiation Axis’, scaled between 0 (mean location at Go cue) and 1 (mean location at movement onset, Center Out). This allows us to quantify progression towards (or away from) movement onset. We used the difference between trajectories to define a ‘Selection Axis’, scaled between −1 (mean of the ipsi trajectory) and +1 (mean of the contra trajectory). This allows us to quantify bias toward one movement direction or the other. Along both Initiation and Selection axes, change was not dominated by a small proportion of GP neurons. Instead, there were smaller contributions from many individual cells located throughout GP (Figure 3—figure supplement 1AC-E).

Video 1. Using movement-related trajectories through state-space to define Initiation, Selection Axes.

Download video file (1.1MB, mp4)

Failed stops reflect earlier evolution of GP activity

We then considered how GP population activity is evolving when Stop cues occur. As noted above, standard race models of reactive stopping (Logan et al., 1984), together with prior data (Schmidt et al., 2013), suggest that failures-to-Stop occur when an underlying Go process evolves more quickly than average, and thus the Stop cue arrives too late. GP population activity was consistent with these ideas (Figure 3D–F). On successful-Stop trials GP activity showed little or no movement before the Stop cue. By contrast, on failed-Stop trials GP activity was in a significantly different state by the time of the Stop cue, having already evolved a substantial distance along the Initiation Axis (Figure 3D; includes both contra- and ipsi-cued trials). Thus, our observations of neural dynamics support hypothesized internal dynamics that determine whether we can react to new information, or are already committed to a course of action.

When stop cues may occur, GP activity starts farther from movement initiation

Conceptually, the slowing of RT with proactive inhibition could reflect any of several distinct underlying changes (Figure 4A), that would manifest in GP dynamics in different ways. If slowing involves mechanisms ‘downstream’ of GP, we might observe no change in the GP population trajectory when aligned on the Go cue (hypothesis 1). Alternatively, the GP might be in a different state at the time the Go cue arrives. In particular, GP activity might start farther away from ‘threshold’ (in dynamical terms, farther from a subspace associated with movement initiation), and thus take longer to get there (hypothesis 2). Finally, proactive inhibition might cause GP activity to evolve differently after Go cue onset. Various, non-mutually-exclusive possibilities include a delayed start (hypothesis 3), slower progress along the same trajectory (hypothesis 4), and/or a threshold that is shifted further away from the starting point (hypothesis 5). Of note, only hypothesis two predicts a change in the trajectory start location at the time of the Go cue (Figure 4A).

Figure 4. Distinct state-space positions at Go cue predict distinct outcomes.

(A) Alternative concepts for proactive inhibition, illustrated using a simplified rise-to-threshold framework (Brown and Heathcote, 2008; Noorani and Carpenter, 2016; Verbruggen and Logan, 2009). (B). Comparison of GP population state between Maybe-Stop-Contra trials (including both contra- and ipsi-instructing Go cues and Stop trials) and No-Stop trials (± 100ms around Go cue; same state-space as Figure 3). Filled circles indicate epochs of significant Euclidean distance between two trajectories (permutation test on each 4 ms time bin, p<0.05). (C). Permutation tests (same format as Figure 3). Just before the Go cue (-100-0ms) the Maybe-Stop state was significantly shifted away from action initiation, and in the ipsi direction. (D) Breakdown of GP state for trials with contra Go cues, by distinct trial outcomes. (E) Quantification of D, comparing evolution of activity along Initiation and Selection Axes on correct contra trials (blue), incorrect action selections (light green) and RT limit errors (brown; failure to initiate movement within 800ms). Thicker lines indicate epochs of significant difference to the Correct trajectory (permutation test on each 4 ms time bin, p<0.05).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Neural population results for individual rats, and corresponding behavior.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

(A) Comparing proactive shifts along Initiation and Selection Axes for all rats together (left) and for individual rats. Rats 2,4 and 6 were grouped together as they had fewer recorded neurons. In left plots, thicker lines indicate epochs of significant difference between two conditions (permutation test on each 4 ms time bin, p<0.05). Note that Rat three had the largest Selection Axis bias towards ipsiversive movements before the Go cue (and a bias towards movement on the Initiation Axis). (B) RT results for the same animal groupings. In all cases there was a greater slowing of contra than ipsi movements, consistent with a selective proactive inhibition effect. However, Rat three showed a speeding of ipsi movements compared to the No-Stop condition, consistent with an ipsiversive bias and no overall movement inhibition.
Figure 4—figure supplement 2. Trial-history dependence.

Figure 4—figure supplement 2.

(A) (Left) On Maybe-Stop trials that followed Stop trials (‘After-Stop’), rats were more likely to succeed in stopping (Wilcoxon signed rank test, z = 2.67, p=0.008) and showed increased RT (Wilcoxon signed rank test, z = 4.46, p=8.02 × 10−6), compared to trials that followed Go trials. (Right) On No-Stop trials that followed error trials (‘After-Error’), rats were more likely to make RT limit errors (Wilcoxon signed rank test, z = 3.03, p=0.002) and showed increased RT (Wilcoxon signed rank test, z = 5.42, p=5.95 × 10−8). (B) Corresponding apparent shifts along the Initiation Axis did not reach significance (permutation tests, analysis epoch: −100–0 ms before Go cue).

We compared GP population activity between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions, immediately before the Go cue (−100 ms - 0 ms; including all trial subtypes). When proactive inhibition was engaged, GP activity occupied a significantly shifted location within state-space (Figure 4B,C). When examined along the Initiation axis (Figure 4C), the direction of this shift was consistent with a longer trajectory required for movements to begin (hypothesis 2). In other words, the brain can restrain actions by placing key circuits into a state from which actions are slower to initiate.

Distinct state-space positions predict distinct types of errors

Proactive inhibition of contra movements also produced a significant shift along the Selection axis before the Go cue, in the direction associated with ipsi movements (Figure 4C). This suggests a preparatory bias against contra movements, when the contra-instructing Go cue may be followed by a Stop cue. To examine how starting position affects behavioral outcome, we examined how state-space location at the Go cue varies with distinct types of errors (Figure 4D). Failures to respond quickly enough to the Go cue (RT limit errors) were associated with starting farther away on the Initiation Axis (Figure 4E). By contrast, incorrect choices (ipsi movements despite contra cue) were associated with starting closer to movement initiation, together with a more-ipsiversive position on the Selection axis at Go cue (Figure 4E; Video 2). Thus, even while the animals are holding still, waiting for the Go cue, GP networks show distinctly-biased internal states that predict distinct subsequent behavioral outcomes.

Video 2. State-space location at the Go cue varies with distinct types of errors.

Download video file (880.9KB, mp4)

Overcoming a selection bias delays movement initiation

The starting ipsiversive bias on the Selection axis when contra actions might have to be cancelled can be overcome, as even on contra Maybe-Stop trials the rats usually made the correct choice. To examine how this occurs we compared neural trajectories for correct, contra Maybe-Stop and No-Stop trials (Figure 5A; only correct trials without Stop cues are included). Just before the Go cue on Maybe-Stop trials, rats showed no difference on the Initiation Axis but were significantly shifted on the Selection axis, in the ipsiversive direction (Figure 5A,B). After the Go Cue, movement on the Initiation axis was delayed compared to No-Stop trials, but movement on the Selection Axis occurred earlier (Figure 5C; Video 3). Thus, on correctly-performed Maybe-Stop trials the GP network engaged a dynamical sequence that was not observed on No-Stop trials: they first overcame a proactive bias towards the alternative action, before proceeding to initiate the action that had been cued.

Figure 5. Multiple dynamics underlying slower reaction times.

(A) Comparison of GP population state between correct Maybe-Stop (contra) and No-Stop (contra) trials (−100 to +250 ms around Go cue; same state-space and format as Figures 3 and 4). Time points of significant Euclidean separation between conditions are marked by filled circles. (B) Permutation tests (same format as Figures 3 and 4) comparing Maybe-Stop (contra) and No-Stop (contra) trials at the time of contra Go cue presentation. GP activity is significantly biased in the ipsi direction, when the contra-instructing cue might be followed by a Stop cue. (C) Examination of distance travelled after Go cue confirms that in the Maybe-Stop condition the trajectory first moves primarily along the Selection Axis (left), before making substantial progress along the Initiation Axis (right). (D–F) Same as A-C, but comparing correct contra No-Stop trials with faster or slower RTs (median split of RTs). Unlike Maybe-Stop trials, spontaneously slow RT trials do not show a starting bias (on either Initiation or Selection axes) and do not move on the Selection Axis before moving on the Initiation Axis.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Comparison of RT-matched Maybe-Stop and No-Stop trajectories.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A-C) Same as Figure 5A–C but using RT-matched subsets of trials. For RT matching, each RT from the Maybe-Stop condition was paired with the closest RT from the No-Stop condition; if no pair could be found within 250 ms, the trial was not used. After RT matching the mean Maybe-Stop RT was 371 ms (median 370 ms) and the median No-Stop RT was 369 ms (median 360 ms). (D–F) Same as A-C but aligned on movement onset (Center out).
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Comparison of Proactive and spontaneously Slow RT trajectories at movement onset.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

All panels are as Figure 5, but aligned on movement onset (Center out).

Video 3. Comparing neural trajectories for correct, contra Maybe-Stop versus No-Stop trials.

Download video file (64.1KB, mp4)

Together our results indicate that, when faced with the challenging Maybe-Stop condition, rats adopt multiple, distinct, covert strategies. They can position neural activity farther from movement onset (on the Initiation Axis), but this produces limited hold violations – essentially making this a bet that the Stop cue will in fact occur. Alternatively, they can bias neural activity in the ipsi direction (on the Selection Axis). This delays contra choices, but also increases the rate of incorrect ipsi choices.

Slower RTs can arise through multiple dynamic mechanisms

We considered the possibility that this apparent ‘strategy’ for proactive inhibition simply reflects the slower RT. In other words, is the distinct trajectory seen for correct Maybe-Stop trials also seen for slower No-Stop trials? Our data indicate that this is not the case. Comparing Maybe-Stop trials with No-Stop trials with the same RT (RT-matching) again showed different positions on the Selection Axis at Go cue (Figure 5—figure supplement 1). This difference was not seen when comparing slower and faster RTs within the No-Stop condition (Figure 5D,E). Rather, spontaneously-slower RTs appeared to arise through slower evolution along both Initiation and Selection Axes simultaneously (Figure 5F). Furthermore, on Maybe-Stop trials movement along the Selection axis overshot the level reached on No-Stop trials, as if overcompensating for the initial bias on this axis (Figure 5A, Figure 5—figure supplement 2). This overshoot was not seen for spontaneously-slower No-Stop trials (Figure 5—figure supplement 2). We conclude that variation in RT reflects multiple dynamic processes within basal ganglia circuits, with slowing due to proactive inhibition involving distinct internal control mechanisms to spontaneous RT variation.

Although reducing the dimensionality of data can be very useful for visualizing trajectories through state-space, we wished to ensure that our conclusions are not distorted by this procedure. We therefore repeated key analyses within the full 376-dimensional state space. Defining Initiation and Selection Axes in the same way as before, but without the PCA step, produced essentially identical trajectory differences between conditions (Figure 6).

Figure 6. Defining Initiation, Selection Axes with or without prior dimension reduction.

Figure 6.

(A) Replotting major results from Figures 35 in two dimensions. The Initiation and Selection Axes are defined as in the main figures, that is using points in the 10-D PC space. (B) Same as A, but defining axes in the full 376-D state space (skipping the PCA step).

Discussion

Stop-signal tasks are widely-used to test cognitive control (Lipszyc and Schachar, 2010), with proactive inhibition considered especially reliant on top-down, effortful, resource-demanding processes (Jahanshahi et al., 2015). Yet there have been extended debates about which psychological and neural mechanisms support proactive control (Verbruggen and Logan, 2009; Chatham et al., 2012; Aron et al., 2014; Leunissen et al., 2016). We have demonstrated here that a key behavioral signature of proactive control – selective slowing of RTs when a Stop signal is expected - can arise through multiple covert strategies. These are visible as changes to the dynamic state of GPe by the time of Go cue presentation, and include a bias towards an alternative action, and/or starting further from the ‘point-of-no-return’ in action initiation.

Which internal strategies are employed for proactive inhibition is likely influenced by the specific experimental conditions (Mayse et al., 2014; Yoshida et al., 2018). For example, we used a brief limited hold period (800 ms) to encourage subjects to respond rapidly to the Go cue rather than waiting to see if the Stop cue is presented. This time pressure may have led rats to sometimes make guesses as to which cues will be presented, and position their neural state accordingly. We also used a task design with asymmetric (ipsi/contra) stop probabilities, to probe the selectivity of proactive inhibition (Aron and Verbruggen, 2008). Motivational aspects are known to be important in proactive inhibition (Meyer and Bucci, 2016): the ipsi bias we observed on the Selection axis on Maybe-Stop (contra) trials may partly reflect asymmetric reward expectancy (Kawagoe et al., 1998), simply because ipsi movements are more consistently rewarded from that state. Unlike human subjects, we cannot verbally instruct rats to perform the task in a certain way (although human cognitive strategies do not always follow experimenter intentions either). It might seem simpler, and less error-prone, for the rats to just select from the slower portion of their regular RT distribution. We suggest that they are unable to consistently do so, given the high spontaneous variability in RTs. The degree to which specific neural strategies are employed may also vary between rats; we found some preliminary evidence for this (Figure 4—figure supplement 1), though investigating this further would require more animals and more recorded cells in each animal.

The term ‘proactive’ or ‘cognitive’ control has been used to refer both to stop-signal tasks like this one, in which subjects are cued about the upcoming stop probability (e.g. Cai et al., 2011; Jahfari et al., 2012; Zandbelt et al., 2013), and also to uncued behavioral adjustments that subjects make after each trial (e.g. longer RTs following trials in which Stop cues occurred; Chen et al., 2010; Pouget et al., 2011; Mayse et al., 2014). Although not the focus of this study, our rats did slow down slightly on average after Stop trials or errors (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A). This slowing was associated with a modest shift on the Initiation Axis in the same, movement-opposed direction as in our main results (Figure 4—figure supplement 2B), but this effect did not reach significance. Thus both behavioral and neural data suggest that the cued component of proactive inhibition was substantially greater than post-trial adjustments under our particular task conditions.

Our ability to reveal distinct strategies for proactive inhibition relies on a dynamical systems approach with single-cell resolution. This method may be especially important for deciphering structures like GP, where projection neurons show continuous, diverse activity patterns. As intermingled GP neurons increased and decreased firing at each moment, the resulting network state changes would likely be undetected using aggregate measures such as photometry or fMRI. Speculatively, we suggest that an enhanced ability to make subtle adjustments to dynamical state may be part of the reason why GP projection neurons show high spontaneous activity, in contrast to (for example) the near-silence of most striatal projection neurons, most of the time.

Prior examinations of motor/premotor cortical dynamics during reaching movements in non-human primates have demonstrated distinct neural dimensions for movement preparation and execution (‘What’ to do) and movement triggering (‘When’ to do it) (Elsayed et al., 2016; Kaufman et al., 2016). Our Selection and Initiation axes are analogous, although our task lacks an explicit preparation epoch and has only two action choices (left vs. right). One notable difference in the non-human primate studies is that movement preparation occurred in distinct, orthogonal dimensions to movement execution, whereas we saw preparatory ‘bias’ along the same Selection axis that differentiated ipsi and contra trajectories during movement itself. Nonetheless, our observation that on correct Maybe-Stop trials, GP state evolved first along the Selection axis is consistent with evidence that movement preparation and movement initiation can be independent processes (Haith et al., 2016; Thura and Cisek, 2017), and that these can be differentially modulated by the basal ganglia and dopamine (Leventhal et al., 2014; Manohar et al., 2015). It also appears consistent with recent observations that, following an unexpected late change in target location, preparation dimensions are rapidly re-engaged (Ames et al., 2019).

The distinction between What and When dimensions is not readily compatible with sequential-sampling mathematical models of decision-making (Smith and Ratcliff, 2004; Brown and Heathcote, 2008; Noorani and Carpenter, 2016), which typically assume that RTs (When) directly reflect sufficient accumulation of evidence for a particular choice (What). Furthermore, when sensory cues are unambiguous the selection process appears to be much faster than standard RTs (Stanford et al., 2010; Haith et al., 2016). Why RTs are typically so much slower and more variable than required for sensory processing or action selection is not fully clear, but this extra time provides opportunity for impulsive or inappropriate responses to be overruled, to increase behavioral flexibility.

The GPe is well positioned to contribute to such behavioral control. GPe has bidirectional connections with the subthalamic nucleus, a key component of the ‘hyperdirect’ pathway from frontal cortex that slows decision-making under conditions of conflict (Cavanagh et al., 2011). GPe itself is the target of the ‘indirect’ (striatopallidal) pathway, believed to discourage action initiation (‘NoGo’; Yoshida and Tanaka, 2016; Kravitz et al., 2010), possibly due to pessimistic predictions of reward (Collins and Frank, 2014; Kim et al., 2017). In standard, firing rate-based models of basal ganglia function, GPe activity restrains actions by preventing pauses in the firing of basal ganglia output, that are in turn required to disinhibit movement-related activity in the brainstem and elsewhere (Chevalier and Deniau, 1990; Roseberry et al., 2016).

However, it is well-recognized that this model is too simple (Gurney et al., 2001; Klaus et al., 2019), and it does not account for the complex activity patterns within GPe that we and others have observed. For example, a straightforward application of the rate model might predict a systematic decrease in GPe firing rate with proactive inhibition, but we did not observe this (Figure 2), with the possible exception of trials with RT limit errors (Figure 2—figure supplement 1). Based on the current results, examining dimension-reduced population dynamics is a promising alternative approach for deciphering how subtle modulations in the firing of many basal ganglia neurons are coordinated to achieve behavioral functions.

At the same time, our study has several noteworthy limitations. Our reduction of complex dynamics to movement along Initiation and Selection axes is obviously a simplification. We did not record large populations of neurons simultaneously, which precludes effective analysis of neural dynamics on individual trials (Afshar et al., 2011). We did not classify GPe neurons by projection target (Mallet et al., 2016; Abecassis et al., 2020) largely because we did not consistently record sleep data to enable that classification (Mallet et al., 2016). We do not yet know the extent to which these population dynamics are shared with upstream (e.g. striatum) and downstream (e.g. substantia nigra pars reticulata) structures, which will be essential for elucidating how these dynamic changes actually influence behavior. Finally, we have not yet determined how the population dynamics reported here relate (if at all) to oscillatory dynamics reported in cortical-basal ganglia circuits during movement suppression (Swann et al., 2009; Cavanagh et al., 2011; Leventhal et al., 2012) and in pathological states such as Parkinson’s Disease (Hammond et al., 2007). These are all worthy subjects for future investigation.

Key resources

Rat (adult, male, Long-Evans, bred in-house).

Materials and methods

All animal experiments were approved by the University of California, San Francisco Committee for the Use and Care of Animals. Adult male Long-Evans rats were housed on a 12 hr/12 hr reverse light-dark cycle, with training and testing performed during the dark phase.

Behavior

Operant chambers (Med Associates, Fairfax VT) had five nose-poke holes on one wall, a food dispenser on the opposite wall, and a speaker located above the food port. The basic rat stop signal task has been previously described (Leventhal et al., 2012.; Mallet et al., 2016; Schmidt et al., 2013). At the start of each trial, one of the three more-central ports was illuminated (‘Light On’) indicating that the rat should poke in that port (‘Center In’) and wait. After a variable delay (500–1250 ms), a higher (4 kHz) or lower (1 kHz) pitch tone was presented for 50 ms (‘Go Cue’), instructing a move to the adjacent port on the left or right side respectively. In Go trials (those without a Stop cue) if the rat left the initial center port (‘Center Out’) within 800 ms of Go cue onset, and then moved to the correct side port (‘Side In’) within 500 ms, a sugar pellet reward was delivered to the food dispenser with an audible click. As the rat left the center port, the center port light was turned off and both side port lights turned on. On Stop trials, the Go cue was followed by a Stop cue (white noise, 125 ms) with a short delay (the stop-signal delay, SSD). The SSD was randomly selected on each trial within a range (uniform distribution) of 100–200 ms (four rats) or 100–250 ms (two rats). Stop trials were rewarded if the rat maintained its nose continuously within the start hole for a total of 800 ms after Go cue onset. Stop trials in which the rat initiated movement before the Stop cue began were converted into Go trials (i.e. no Stop cue was presented). Failed-Stop trials with RT >500 ms were excluded from electrophysiological analyses, since these were presumed to reflect trials for which rats successfully responded to the Stop cue, but then failed to maintain holding until reward delivery (see Leventhal et al., 2012; Schmidt et al., 2013; Mayse et al., 2014). Inter-trial intervals were randomly selected between 5 and 7 s. For included sessions, the median number of Go trials was 266 (range, 167–361) and the median number of Stop trials was 57 (range, 27–95).

To vary proactive inhibition, we changed the Stop cue probabilities between starting holes (as shown in Figure 1). The spatial mapping of probabilities was constant for each rat across sessions, but varied between rats. Within each session, the same start hole (and thus proactive condition) was repeated for 10–15 trials at a time. After ~3 months of training, rats showing consistent reaction time differences between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions were eligible for electrode implantation.

Electrophysiology

We report GP data from six rats (all animals in which we successfully recorded GP neurons during contraversive proactive inhibition). Each rat was implanted with 15 tetrodes (configured as independently-driveable bundles of 2–3 tetrodes, each within a polyimide tube with outer radius 140 µm), bilaterally targeting GP and substantia nigra reticulata (SNr). During task performance, wide-band (0.1–9000 Hz) electrophysiological data were recorded with a sampling rate of 30000/s using an Intan RHD2000 recording system (Intan Technologies). All signals were initially referenced to a skull screw (tip-flattened) on the midline 1 mm posterior to lambda. For spike detection we re-referenced to an electrode common average, and wavelet-filtered (Wiltschko et al., 2008) before thresholding. For spike sorting we performed automatic clustering units using MountainSort (Chung et al., 2017) followed by manual curation of clusters. Tetrodes were usually moved by 159 µm every 2–3 sessions. To avoid duplicate neurons we did not include data from the same tetrode across multiple sessions unless the tetrode had been moved by >100 µm between those sessions. Based on waveform and firing properties we further excluded an additional 25 units that appeared to be duplicates even though the tetrode had been moved. After recording was complete, we anesthetized rats and made small marker lesions by applying 10 µA current for 20 s for one or two wires of each tetrode. After perfusing the rats and slicing (at 40 µm) tissue sections were stained with cresyl violet and compared to the nearest atlas section (Paxinos, 2006).

Data analysis

Smoothed firing rates were obtained by convolving each spike time with a Gaussian kernel (30 ms SD). Firing rates were normalized (Z-scored) using the neuron’s session-wide mean and SD. Normalized average time series for contra and ipsi actions (500 ms each, around Center Out) were concatenated and used to construct a population activity matrix R = TC by N, with T = 251 (timepoints, at 2 ms intervals), C = 2 (ipsi/contra conditions), and N = 376 (the number of neurons). We subtracted the mean of each of the N columns to make data zero-centered, then performed principal components analysis (PCA) over matrix R using the MATLAB ‘svd’ function. Using the right singular vectors (W), we can calculate the PC scores (S) as S = RW. For example, the first column of S contains the first principal component (PC1) over time, and the first column of W contains the weights for each of the N units for PC1. We used the first 10 PCs for analysis, and the Euclidean distance between conditions was compared in this 10-D space. The projections onto the Initiation or Selection Axes were calculated as the dot product of the state space position vector and the axis vector. State-space positions around the Go cue (or Stop cue) were calculated using the set of weights W to project the Go cue–aligned (or Stop cue-aligned) firing rates into the 10-D PC space. In other words, each neuron has a weight for each PC, and we calculate a net population position along each PC by multiplying each neuron’s instantaneous firing rate by its weight, and summing across all neurons.

To test if whether state-space positions for two conditions (e.g. Successful- and Failed-Stops) are significantly separated, we ran permutation tests by randomly shuffling the trial conditions for each neuron (10000 shuffles for each test). Then, the distance in the population state space at each time point was reconstructed using the firing rate differences between the shuffled trial averages for each condition. For example, if the mean FR of a unit (n) in surrogate Failed Stop trials (c1) and surrogate Successful Stop trials (c2) at Stop cue time (t) is r(t,c1,n) and r(t,c2,n), respectively, the difference between two conditions in k-dimension, x(t,k) is:

x(t,k)=n=1N(r(t,c1,n)-rt,c2,n)×w(n,k)

Repeated shuffling produces a surrogate data distribution for differences at each time point, and the original difference between conditions is compared to this distribution to determine statistical significance.

Acknowledgements

We thank Michael Farries and Ali Mohebi for technical advice, Wei Wei and Vikaas Sohal for comments on the manuscript, and Alejandro Jimenez Rodriguez for discussions. This work was supported by NIH grants R01MH101697, R01NS078435 and R01DA045783, and the University of California, San Francisco.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Joshua D Berke, Email: joshua.berke@ucsf.edu.

Daeyeol Lee, Johns Hopkins University, United States.

Richard B Ivry, University of California, Berkeley, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Mental Health R01 MH101697 to Joshua D Berke.

  • National Institute on Drug Abuse R01 DA045783 to Joshua D Berke.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke R01NS078435 to Joshua D Berke.

  • University of California, San Francisco to Joshua D Berke.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Visualization, Writing - review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Ethics

Animal experimentation: All animal experiments were approved by the University of California, San Francisco Committee for the Use and Care of Animals (approval number: AN181071).

Additional files

Transparent reporting form

Data availability

The neurophysiology data and analysis code used in this study are available at figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12367541.

The following dataset was generated:

Gu BM, Schmidt R, Berke JD. 2020. Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition. figshare.

References

  1. Abecassis ZA, Berceau BL, Win PH, García D, Xenias HS, Cui Q, Pamukcu A, Cherian S, Hernández VM, Chon U, Lim BK, Kim Y, Justice NJ, Awatramani R, Hooks BM, Gerfen CR, Boca SM, Chan CS. Npas1+-Nkx2.1+ neurons are an integral part of the Cortico-pallido-cortical loop. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40:743–768. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1199-19.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Afshar A, Santhanam G, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Sahani M, Shenoy KV. Single-trial neural correlates of arm movement preparation. Neuron. 2011;71:555–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ames KC, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Simultaneous motor preparation and execution in a last-moment reach correction task. Nature Communications. 2019;10:2718. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-10772-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Arkadir D, Morris G, Vaadia E, Bergman H. Independent coding of movement direction and reward prediction by single pallidal neurons. Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:10047–10056. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2583-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aron AR. From reactive to proactive and selective control: developing a richer model for stopping inappropriate responses. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69:e55–e68. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.07.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex: one decade on. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:2424–2433. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4682-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Aron AR, Verbruggen F. Stop the presses: dissociating a selective from a global mechanism for stopping. Psychological Science. 2008;19:1146–1153. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02216.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brotchie P, Iansek R, Horne MK. Motor function of the monkey globus pallidus. 2. cognitive aspects of movement and phasic neuronal activity. Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 1991;114:1685–1702. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.4.1685. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Brown SD, Heathcote A. The simplest complete model of choice response time: linear ballistic accumulation. Cognitive Psychology. 2008;57:153–178. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2007.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cai W, Oldenkamp CL, Aron AR. A proactive mechanism for selective suppression of response tendencies. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:5965–5969. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6292-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carli M, Evenden JL, Robbins TW. Depletion of unilateral striatal dopamine impairs initiation of contralateral actions and not sensory attention. Nature. 1985;313:679–682. doi: 10.1038/313679a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cavanagh JF, Wiecki TV, Cohen MX, Figueroa CM, Samanta J, Sherman SJ, Frank MJ. Subthalamic nucleus stimulation reverses mediofrontal influence over decision threshold. Nature Neuroscience. 2011;14:1462–1467. doi: 10.1038/nn.2925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Chatham CH, Claus ED, Kim A, Curran T, Banich MT, Munakata Y. Cognitive control reflects context monitoring, not motoric stopping, in response inhibition. PLOS ONE. 2012;7:e31546. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Chen X, Scangos KW, Stuphorn V. Supplementary motor area exerts proactive and reactive control of arm movements. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30:14657–14675. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2669-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Chevalier G, Deniau JM. Disinhibition as a basic process in the expression of striatal functions. Trends in Neurosciences. 1990;13:277–280. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90109-N. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Asari T, Yamashita K, Morimoto H, Hirose S, Miyashita Y, Konishi S. Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. Cerebral Cortex. 2009;19:146–152. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn065. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Chung JE, Magland JF, Barnett AH, Tolosa VM, Tooker AC, Lee KY, Shah KG, Felix SH, Frank LM, Greengard LF. A fully automated approach to spike sorting. Neuron. 2017;95:1381–1394. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Collins AGE, Frank MJ. Opponent actor learning (OpAL): Modeling interactive effects of striatal dopamine on reinforcement learning and choice incentive. Psychological Review. 2014;121:337–366. doi: 10.1037/a0037015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Cunningham JP, Yu BM. Dimensionality reduction for large-scale neural recordings. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1500–1509. doi: 10.1038/nn.3776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. DeLong MR. Activity of pallidal neurons during movement. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1971;34:414–427. doi: 10.1152/jn.1971.34.3.414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Dunovan K, Lynch B, Molesworth T, Verstynen T. Competing basal ganglia pathways determine the difference between stopping and deciding not to go. eLife. 2015;4:e08723. doi: 10.7554/eLife.08723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Eagle DM, Baunez C, Hutcheson DM, Lehmann O, Shah AP, Robbins TW. Stop-signal reaction-time task performance: role of prefrontal cortex and subthalamic nucleus. Cerebral Cortex. 2008;18:178–188. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Elsayed GF, Lara AH, Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Cunningham JP. Reorganization between preparatory and movement population responses in motor cortex. Nature Communications. 2016;7:13239. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ersche KD, Jones PS, Williams GB, Turton AJ, Robbins TW, Bullmore ET. Abnormal brain structure implicated in stimulant drug addiction. Science. 2012;335:601–604. doi: 10.1126/science.1214463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gage GJ, Stoetzner CR, Wiltschko AB, Berke JD. Selective activation of striatal Fast-Spiking interneurons during choice execution. Neuron. 2010;67:466–479. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.06.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gardiner TW, Kitai ST. Single-unit activity in the globus pallidus and neostriatum of the rat during performance of a trained head movement. Experimental Brain Research. 1992;88:517–530. doi: 10.1007/BF00228181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Gurney K, Prescott TJ, Redgrave P. A computational model of action selection in the basal ganglia. I. A new functional anatomy. Biological Cybernetics. 2001;84:401–410. doi: 10.1007/PL00007984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Haith AM, Pakpoor J, Krakauer JW. Independence of movement preparation and movement initiation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:3007–3015. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3245-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hammond C, Bergman H, Brown P. Pathological synchronization in Parkinson's disease: networks, models and treatments. Trends in Neurosciences. 2007;30:357–364. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hanes DP, Schall JD. Neural control of voluntary movement initiation. Science. 1996;274:427–430. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5286.427. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hardung S, Epple R, Jäckel Z, Eriksson D, Uran C, Senn V, Gibor L, Yizhar O, Diester I. A functional gradient in the rodent prefrontal cortex supports behavioral inhibition. Current Biology. 2017;27:549–555. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.12.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Isoda M, Hikosaka O. Role for subthalamic nucleus neurons in switching from automatic to controlled eye movement. Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28:7209–7218. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0487-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Jahanshahi M, Obeso I, Rothwell JC, Obeso JA. A fronto-striato-subthalamic-pallidal network for goal-directed and habitual inhibition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2015;16:719–732. doi: 10.1038/nrn4038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jahfari S, Verbruggen F, Frank MJ, Waldorp LJ, Colzato L, Ridderinkhof KR, Forstmann BU. How preparation changes the need for top-down control of the basal ganglia when inhibiting premature actions. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:10870–10878. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0902-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV. Cortical activity in the null space: permitting preparation without movement. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:440–448. doi: 10.1038/nn.3643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaufman MT, Seely JS, Sussillo D, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV, Churchland MM. The largest response component in the motor cortex reflects movement timing but not movement type. Eneuro. 2016;3:ENEURO.0085-16.2016. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0085-16.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kawagoe R, Takikawa Y, Hikosaka O. Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia. Nature Neuroscience. 1998;1:411–416. doi: 10.1038/1625. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kim HF, Amita H, Hikosaka O. Indirect pathway of caudal basal ganglia for rejection of valueless visual objects. Neuron. 2017;94:920–930. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Klaus A, Alves da Silva J, Costa RM. What, if, and when to move: basal ganglia circuits and Self-Paced action initiation. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2019;42:459–483. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-072116-031033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Kravitz AV, Freeze BS, Parker PR, Kay K, Thwin MT, Deisseroth K, Kreitzer AC. Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature. 2010;466:622–626. doi: 10.1038/nature09159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Leunissen I, Coxon JP, Swinnen SP. A proactive task set influences how response inhibition is implemented in the basal ganglia. Human Brain Mapping. 2016;37:4706–4717. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Leventhal DK, Gage GJ, Schmidt R, Pettibone JR, Case AC, Berke JD. Basal ganglia beta oscillations accompany cue utilization. Neuron. 2012;73:523–536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Leventhal DK, Stoetzner C, Abraham R, Pettibone J, DeMarco K, Berke JD. Dissociable effects of dopamine on learning and performance within sensorimotor striatum. Basal Ganglia. 2014;4:43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.baga.2013.11.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lipszyc J, Schachar R. Inhibitory control and psychopathology: a meta-analysis of studies using the stop signal task. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2010;16:1064–1076. doi: 10.1017/S1355617710000895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1984;10:276–291. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Majid DS, Cai W, Corey-Bloom J, Aron AR. Proactive selective response suppression is implemented via the basal ganglia. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:13259–13269. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5651-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mallet N, Schmidt R, Leventhal D, Chen F, Amer N, Boraud T, Berke JD. Arkypallidal cells send a stop signal to striatum. Neuron. 2016;89:308–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Manohar SG, Chong TT, Apps MA, Batla A, Stamelou M, Jarman PR, Bhatia KP, Husain M. Reward pays the cost of noise reduction in motor and cognitive control. Current Biology. 2015;25:1707–1716. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Mayse JD, Nelson GM, Park P, Gallagher M, Lin SC. Proactive and reactive inhibitory control in rats. Frontiers in Neuroscience. 2014;8:104. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Meyer HC, Bucci DJ. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of proactive and reactive inhibition. Learning & Memory. 2016;23:504–514. doi: 10.1101/lm.040501.115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Noorani I, Carpenter RH. The LATER model of reaction time and decision. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;64:229–251. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Paxinos CW. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates.  Academic Press; 2006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  54. Pouget P, Logan GD, Palmeri TJ, Boucher L, Paré M, Schall JD. Neural basis of adaptive response time adjustment during saccade countermanding. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:12604–12612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Roseberry TK, Lee AM, Lalive AL, Wilbrecht L, Bonci A, Kreitzer AC. Cell-Type-Specific control of brainstem locomotor circuits by basal ganglia. Cell. 2016;164:526–537. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.12.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Schmidt R, Leventhal DK, Mallet N, Chen F, Berke JD. Canceling actions involves a race between basal ganglia pathways. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1118–1124. doi: 10.1038/nn.3456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Schmidt R, Berke JD. A Pause-then-Cancel model of stopping: evidence from basal ganglia neurophysiology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2017;372:20160202. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Shin S, Sommer MA. Activity of neurons in monkey globus pallidus during oculomotor behavior compared with that in substantia nigra pars reticulata. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2010;103:1874–1887. doi: 10.1152/jn.00101.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Smith PL, Ratcliff R. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions. Trends in Neurosciences. 2004;27:161–168. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2004.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Stanford TR, Shankar S, Massoglia DP, Costello MG, Salinas E. Perceptual decision making in less than 30 milliseconds. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:379–385. doi: 10.1038/nn.2485. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Swann N, Tandon N, Canolty R, Ellmore TM, McEvoy LK, Dreyer S, DiSano M, Aron AR. Intracranial EEG reveals a time- and frequency-specific role for the right inferior frontal gyrus and primary motor cortex in stopping initiated responses. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:12675–12685. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3359-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Thura D, Cisek P. The basal ganglia do not select reach targets but control the urgency of commitment. Neuron. 2017;95:1160–1170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Turner RS, Anderson ME. Pallidal discharge related to the kinematics of reaching movements in two dimensions. Journal of Neurophysiology. 1997;77:1051–1074. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.3.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Verbruggen F, Aron AR, Band GPH, Beste C, Bissett PG, Brockett AT, Brown JW, Chamberlain SR, Chambers CD, Colonius H, Colzato LS, Corneil BD, Coxon JP, Dupuis A, Eagle DM, Garavan H, Greenhouse I, Heathcote A, Huster RJ, Jahfari S, Kenemans JL, Leunissen I, Li C-SR, Logan GD, Matzke D, Morein-Zamir S, Murthy A, Paré M, Poldrack RA, Ridderinkhof KR, Robbins TW, Roesch M, Rubia K, Schachar RJ, Schall JD, Stock A-K, Swann NC, Thakkar KN, van der Molen MW, Vermeylen L, Vink M, Wessel JR, Whelan R, Zandbelt BB, Boehler CN. A consensus guide to capturing the ability to inhibit actions and impulsive behaviors in the stop-signal task. eLife. 2019;8:e46323. doi: 10.7554/eLife.46323. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Verbruggen F, Logan GD. Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 2009;35:835–854. doi: 10.1037/a0012726. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wessel JR, Aron AR. On the globality of motor suppression: unexpected events and their influence on behavior and cognition. Neuron. 2017;93:259–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wiltschko AB, Gage GJ, Berke JD. Wavelet filtering before spike detection preserves waveform shape and enhances single-unit discrimination. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 2008;173:34–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.05.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yoshida J, Saiki A, Soma S, Yamanaka K, Nonomura S, Ríos A, Kawabata M, Kimura M, Sakai Y, Isomura Y. Area-specific modulation of functional cortical activity during Block-based and Trial-based proactive inhibition. Neuroscience. 2018;388:297–316. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Yoshida A, Tanaka M. Two types of neurons in the primate globus pallidus external segment play distinct roles in antisaccade generation. Cerebral Cortex. 2016;26:1187–1199. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Zandbelt BB, Bloemendaal M, Neggers SF, Kahn RS, Vink M. Expectations and violations: delineating the neural network of proactive inhibitory control. Human Brain Mapping. 2013;34:2015–2024. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision letter

Editor: Daeyeol Lee1
Reviewed by: Daeyeol Lee2

In the interests of transparency, eLife publishes the most substantive revision requests and the accompanying author responses.

Acceptance summary:

To study the role of the basal ganglia in response inhibition, the activity of neurons in the rat globus pallidus (GPe) was recorded while the animal performed a stop-signal task. The key analyses focused on the dynamics of population level activity, with the insight that different states could be identified that were associated with movement initiation and movements associated with a particular action. Most important, the state was context-dependent, shifting away from that associated with initiation and movement representation as the probability of response inhibition increased. In sum, the study applies sophisticated tools to reveal the dynamics of a key region of the brain involved in response inhibition.

Decision letter after peer review:

Thank you for submitting your article "Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition" for consideration by eLife. Your article has been reviewed by three peer reviewers, including Daeyeol Lee as the Reviewing Editor and Reviewer #1, and the evaluation has been overseen by Richard Ivry as the Senior Editor.

The reviewers have discussed the reviews with one another and the Reviewing Editor has drafted this decision to help you prepare a revised submission.

As the editors have judged that your manuscript is of interest, but as described below that additional experiments are required before it is published, we would like to draw your attention to changes in our revision policy that we have made in response to COVID-19 (https://elifesciences.org/articles/57162). First, because many researchers have temporarily lost access to the labs, we will give authors as much time as they need to submit revised manuscripts. We are also offering, if you choose, to post the manuscript to bioRxiv (if it is not already there) along with this decision letter and a formal designation that the manuscript is “in revision at eLife”. Please let us know if you would like to pursue this option. (If your work is more suitable for medRxiv, you will need to post the preprint yourself, as the mechanisms for us to do so are still in development.)

Summary:

The authors have recorded the activity of neurons in the rat globus pallidus (GPe) while the animal performed a stop-signal task. The goal of this study was to examine the possible contribution of GPe in proactive inhibitory control whereby the animal could adjust the preparation of upcoming movement in order to reduce the initiation of undesired movements, namely, the movements that need to be canceled upon the arrival of stop signals. By examining the population activity in the state-space, the authors nicely demonstrated that increasing the probability of stop signal trials cause two different changes in GPe activity. First, the GPe population moved away from the region where movements can be triggered along the so-called initiation axis (along which the activity moves around the time of movement initiation). Second, the GPe activity moved away from the action that might be canceled along the different, so-called selection axis. The analyses and results are described clearly. Overall, the manuscript provides an important insight into the role of the basal ganglia in behavioral inhibition. Nevertheless, there are some interpretational limitations that the authors might need to discuss more.

Essential revisions:

1) One unfortunate consequence of the task design adopted in this study is that the position of the center port changes across 3 different conditions, and therefore could potentially influenced the activity of GPe neurons at the time of go cues. Can the authors exclude the possibility the changes in the GPe trajectory might result from the changes in the animal's body posture rather than from the preparatory bias for movement cancelation? Similarly, is it possible that activity during stop trial reflects the direction of the movement they need to make to get reward (i.e., the location of the food dispenser)? This would vary across trial-types (i.e., maybe- and no-stop trials) depending on the location of the central port.

2) The authors have used the phrase "cognitive control" (only) in the Abstract. While the results in this manuscript clearly provide important insights into the role of the basal ganglia in action selection and cancellation, however, it is unclear whether the type of control demonstrated in this study is analogous to cognitive control studied in human literature. In particular, there is no evidence that the adjustment is applied in response to the outcomes of recent trials, as in human and non-human primate studies. Another difference is that in this study, proactive control is applied directionally, which might not be the case in the standard stop signal task. How clearly can proactive "inhibitory" control be distinguished and separated from bias in action selection? These issues might need more careful discussion.

3) How will proactive control/state space approaches to these signals apply in instances of proactive control looking at trial history? For instance, as validation of the findings in this manuscript, can the authors show similar changes in neural firing/behavioral data on trials where rats experience multiple stops or goes in a row (e.g., conflict adaptation; post error slowing)? Presumably having just experienced a stop or an error, rats might slow their responding on the next trial and engage the strategies that the authors discuss. A similar story could be told for when there are multiple goes in a row.

4) Overall, some results can be explained well and more details can be included. Often results and analysis that explain fairly complicated figures are described in 1-2 sentences. Here are some examples.

a) It is not clear what Figure 2C,D is showing from the 1 sentence description in the text. It is said the contra and ipsi are equally represented but no statistic is given. It is not clear what "proactive control was associated with altered activity in different sets of GP neurons at different times" means and why the data is not shown.

b) It appears that the CDF in Figure 1D shows only the RT in non-stop (go) trials, whereas Figure 1B compares the RT in non-cancelled (error) stop trials, but this was not clearly explained. Also, it would be helpful to explain what RT and MT limit errors refer to at this point.

5) It is possible that GPe network state is not positioning anything strategically but is a reflection of upstream processing and/or the state that the body is in. Could GP activity states reflect that the animal has or is currently positioning its body in a certain way? Just because the rat is in the central port doesn't necessarily mean it is holding still or is not physically leaning in one direction or the other. My general feeling about these results is that the authors would get the exact same results in any task-related brain area using this analysis (e.g., dorsal striatum, SNr).

6) Figure 2 doesn't explain properly why these effects could not be observed at the level of single and population levels. It might be helpful to show firing rates for movements made in both directions for each of these figures. Also, how do these neurons and populations fire on errors (not moving quickly enough and going in the wrong direction). For the population histogram, the authors are averaging over all neurons, which may be problematic. From the heat plot it seems like half the neurons increase whereas the other half decrease firing during the trial. Averaging them all together would wash out any effects.

7) Some steps in the behavioral analysis might rest on assumptions that might not be valid. For example, it might be dangerous to group the stop trials where movements occur after stop cue together with go trials, since this strictly assumes that the go and stop processes are completely independent. The results from the primate literature (Schall) have shown that this assumption is not valid. In addition, it seems strange that stop trials with RT>500 ms were excluded. Were go trials with such long RT also removed from the analysis?

8) One interesting claim was that the trajectories of GP population activity were consistent with a spatial, not a temporal, offset along the initiation axis, but this was not entirely convincing. It appears that a temporal offset would require all (or most) of the neurons to have exactly the same delay. If only a subset of neurons showed this delay, or if the neurons showed different delays, wouldn't it also appear as a spatial offset? It might be useful to perform some simulations to document the sensitivity and specificity of using the PC-based analysis to uncover any relationship between neural activity and model predictions.

eLife. 2020 Jun 10;9:e57215. doi: 10.7554/eLife.57215.sa2

Author response


Essential revisions:

1) One unfortunate consequence of the task design adopted in this study is that the position of the center port changes across 3 different conditions, and therefore could potentially influenced the activity of GPe neurons at the time of go cues. Can the authors exclude the possibility the changes in the GPe trajectory might result from the changes in the animal's body posture rather than from the preparatory bias for movement cancelation? Similarly, is it possible that activity during stop trial reflects the direction of the movement they need to make to get reward (i.e., the location of the food dispenser)? This would vary across trial-types (i.e., maybe- and no-stop trials) depending on the location of the central port.

We are confident that the variable spatial position of the start port is not driving our key results:

– We have previously demonstrated (Gage et al., 2010) that GPe neurons are very sensitive to movement direction, but are insensitive to start port spatial location. Those experiments used the same behavioral apparatus and similar task sequence (once again starting from any of the three center-most holes, as in this manuscript, but using Go trials only).

– We counterbalanced the spatial positions of the No-Stop, Maybe-Stop-Contra, and Maybe-Stop-Ipsi start ports between rats. The shifts we observed with contraversive proactive inhibition occurred in the same direction (i.e. a bias away from movement along the Initiation axis, and a bias away from contraversive action on the Selection axis), regardless of whether the Maybe-Stop-Contra port was physically located on the apparatus side congruent with the implant side, or on the opposite side.

2) The authors have used the phrase "cognitive control" (only) in the Abstract. While the results in this manuscript clearly provide important insights into the role of the basal ganglia in action selection and cancellation, however, it is unclear whether the type of control demonstrated in this study is analogous to cognitive control studied in human literature. In particular, there is no evidence that the adjustment is applied in response to the outcomes of recent trials, as in human and non-human primate studies. Another difference is that in this study, proactive control is applied directionally, which might not be the case in the standard stop signal task. How clearly can proactive "inhibitory" control be distinguished and separated from bias in action selection? These issues might need more careful discussion.

The terms “cognitive control” and “proactive inhibition” have been broadly applied in the human literature across a range of task designs. Our specific Maybe-Stop variant was directly inspired by, and based upon, human Maybe-Stop studies by Adam Aron and colleagues (e.g. Cai et al., 2011, Majid et al., 2013). Both those studies, and ours, were designed to study the movement-selectivity of proactive inhibition, using explicit cues in advance to prompt adjustments in stopping performance. Aron hypothesized (2011) that the GP was especially involved in movement-selective proactive inhibition, hence our choice to study GP here using a similar task design. We now make clear in the Abstract that we are studying selective proactive inhibition.

Our distinction between proactive effects on Initiation and Selection Axes is directly intended to help distinguish between inhibiting actions in general, and being biased in the preparation and selection of actions. We observed both effects. An interesting question is whether any situation that produces a directional bias is reflected in a similar GP population shift along the Selection Axis as defined here. We are currently investigating this using a probabilistically-rewarded task where contra and ipsi actions are associated with different reward expectation (see related part of Discussion).

It is true that “proactive inhibition” has also been used to refer to uncued behavioral adjustments after trial outcome (i.e. trial history-dependent effects). We observe this form of proactive inhibition as well, and it may involve overlapping neural mechanisms (see new Figure 4—figure supplement 2). We have now noted this in the text, and clarified the differences between these distinct manifestations of cognitive control in the Discussion.

3) How will proactive control/state space approaches to these signals apply in instances of proactive control looking at trial history? For instance, as validation of the findings in this manuscript, can the authors show similar changes in neural firing/behavioral data on trials where rats experience multiple stops or goes in a row (e.g., conflict adaptation; post error slowing)? Presumably having just experienced a stop or an error, rats might slow their responding on the next trial and engage the strategies that the authors discuss. A similar story could be told for when there are multiple goes in a row.

Our rats do indeed slow their responding slightly after stops or errors, and this is associated with a slight shift along the Initiation Axis in the same direction as in our main results (i.e. starting further from movement onset; Figure 4—figure supplement 2). However, the effect is smaller than the main proactive effects we report, and does not reach significance with our current data.

4) Overall, some results can be explained well and more details can be included. Often results and analysis that explain fairly complicated figures are described in 1-2 sentences. Here are some examples.

a) It is not clear what Figure 2C,D is showing from the 1 sentence description in the text. It is said the contra and ipsi are equally represented but no statistic is given. It is not clear what "proactive control was associated with altered activity in different sets of GP neurons at different times" means and why the data is not shown.

It is true that we go relatively quickly over the individual neuron results in Figure 2; this is because the primary focus of the manuscript is the population dynamics and how they relate to behavior. Nonetheless, we have revised and extended the Results text to be hopefully more clear. We do give a statistical result for equal representation of contra and ipsi in Figure 2E (p=0.87) and have updated the legend to indicate that this is the same Wilcoxon signed rank test as in the previous panel.

The data that was previously not shown is now included in Figure 2—figure supplement 1C. This indicates that although some individual GP neurons significantly distinguish the Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions, we did not observe a neuronal subpopulation that maintains this distinction across extended periods (the timing of the Go cue is unpredictable). Our failure to observe such a subpopulation lead us to examine population dynamics.

b) It appears that the CDF in Figure 1D shows only the RT in non-stop (go) trials, whereas Figure 1B compares the RT in non-cancelled (error) stop trials, but this was not clearly explained. Also, it would be helpful to explain what RT and MT limit errors refer to at this point.

We have clarified Figure 1B to indicate that the RT distribution purple line refers specifically to Failed Stop trials (on Correct Stops, there is no RT to plot). We have noted in the legend that Figure 1D refers specifically to trials in which no Stop cue is presented, as the reviewer surmised. We have also added the definition of RT and MT limit errors to the Figure 1 legend.

5) It is possible that GPe network state is not positioning anything strategically but is a reflection of upstream processing and/or the state that the body is in. Could GP activity states reflect that the animal has or is currently positioning its body in a certain way? Just because the rat is in the central port doesn't necessarily mean it is holding still or is not physically leaning in one direction or the other. My general feeling about these results is that the authors would get the exact same results in any task-related brain area using this analysis (e.g., dorsal striatum, SNr).

Our task design exceeds prevailing standards in the rodent stop-signal field, as we constrain the rat to maintain head entry in the period preceding both go and stop cues. In any stop-signal study, regardless of species, one may be concerned that some unknown, uncontrolled aspect of behavior is contributing to the results. However, the specific character of our neural results (shifting away from contralateral movement initiation when contralateral movements may have to be cancelled) is very consistent with the proactive control interpretation that we favor.

Our GP results might indeed be mirrored in upstream and/or downstream structures, and determining whether this is the case is an important question we are addressing in ongoing research. Based on very preliminary data from SNr, it is not the case that the exact same results are present in any task-related brain area, but data collection for that study is not yet complete.

6) Figure 2 doesn't explain properly why these effects could not be observed at the level of single and population levels. It might be helpful to show firing rates for movements made in both directions for each of these figures. Also, how do these neurons and populations fire on errors (not moving quickly enough and going in the wrong direction). For the population histogram, the authors are averaging over all neurons, which may be problematic. From the heat plot it seems like half the neurons increase whereas the other half decrease firing during the trial. Averaging them all together would wash out any effects.

We address each of these points in turn in new Figure 2—figure supplement 1 panels:

– Panel B shows the firing rates for ipsi movements, in the same way we showed the contra movement in Figure 2C.

– Panel E shows the population average firing rates for Go-related errors: not moving quickly enough (RT limit error) and going in the wrong direction.

– Panel C shows the average activity separately for units with increases vs decreases in firing. We still do not see any significant difference between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop conditions as rats wait for the Go cue.

7) Some steps in the behavioral analysis might rest on assumptions that might not be valid. For example, it might be dangerous to group the stop trials where movements occur after stop cue together with go trials, since this strictly assumes that the go and stop processes are completely independent. The results from the primate literature (Schall) have shown that this assumption is not valid. In addition, it seems strange that stop trials with RT>500 ms were excluded. Were go trials with such long RT also removed from the analysis?

The behavioral analysis does not make this assumption. We now clarify (in Results and Figure 1D legend) that the RT comparison between Maybe-Stop and No-Stop trials includes only trials in which no Stop cue occurs.

The exclusion of Stop trials with RT>500ms serves to distinguish between “failures-to-stop” and “failures-to-wait”, in which the rat successfully cancels the action but then fails to sustain the hold for the required period (noted in Figure 1 legend). A prior, in-depth analysis (Mayse et al., 2014) demonstrated that it is important to distinguish between these, for example to correctly estimate stop-signal reaction times. In addition, we previously showed that this distinction between two types of errors is apparent in neural signatures of the stop process. For example, a pulse of beta oscillations is rapidly evoked by the Stop cue on Correct Stop trials, and Failed Stops with RT>500ms, but not on Failed Stops with RT<500ms (Leventhal et al., 2012). Finally, even we include RT>500ms stop failed trials, the key result on reactive stopping is unchanged (at the time of the Stop cue, GP position along the Initiation Axis is significantly different for Correct and Failed Stop trials; p<0.0001).

8) One interesting claim was that the trajectories of GP population activity were consistent with a spatial, not a temporal, offset along the initiation axis, but this was not entirely convincing. It appears that a temporal offset would require all (or most) of the neurons to have exactly the same delay. If only a subset of neurons showed this delay, or if the neurons showed different delays, wouldn't it also appear as a spatial offset? It might be useful to perform some simulations to document the sensitivity and specificity of using the PC-based analysis to uncover any relationship between neural activity and model predictions.

We now more clearly explain in the revised text that our key conceptual distinction is: does proactive inhibition involve an altered neural state in advance of the Go cue, or a change in neural information processing subsequent to Go cue onset? We provide compelling evidence for an altered neural state in advance of the Go cue, manifested as a spatial offset in state-space. Although interesting changes in information processing subsequent to Go cue onset can also occur (e.g. Figure 5—figure supplements 1 and 2) they cannot account for changes before the Go cue.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Gu BM, Schmidt R, Berke JD. 2020. Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition. figshare. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Transparent reporting form

    Data Availability Statement

    The neurophysiology data and analysis code used in this study are available at figshare: https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.12367541.

    The following dataset was generated:

    Gu BM, Schmidt R, Berke JD. 2020. Globus pallidus dynamics reveal covert strategies for behavioral inhibition. figshare.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES