Abstract
Craving, defined here as a strong desire to eat, is a common experience that drives behavior. Here we discuss the concept of craving from historical, physiological, and clinical perspectives, and review work investigating the effects of cue reactivity and cue-induced craving on eating and weight outcomes, as well as underlying neural mechanisms. We also highlight the significance of cue reactivity and craving in the context of our “toxic food environment” and the obesity epidemic. We then summarize our work developing the Regulation of Craving (ROC) task, used to test the causal effects of cognitive strategies on craving for food and drugs as well as the underlying neural mechanisms of such regulation. Next, we review our recent development of a novel ROC-based intervention that trains individuals to use cognitive strategies to regulate craving, with promising effects on subsequent food choice and caloric consumption. We end by discussing future directions for this important line of work.
Keywords: Craving, cue reactivity, regulation, choice, value, training
At the 2019 annual meeting of the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behaviors in Utrecht, the last author presented our lab’s work on the regulation of craving task, as well as the development and validation of a cognitive training procedure based on this task. In this review, we provide a synthesis of the background and motivation for those studies, summarize important relevant work, and discuss future directions for refining and disseminating this training.
We will begin with a demonstration. Please look at this freshly baked brownie, and imagine that it is on a plate, right in front of you (Figure 1). You can see there are chocolate chips and nuts on top, and warm fudge running down the sides. Now, please imagine picking up this warm brownie, bringing it close to your nose, and inhaling the sweet scent of chocolate. Imagine bringing it to your mouth and taking a bite. Can you feel the rich texture of the dough, and the indulgent taste? As you imagine this, you may feel your mouth watering, perhaps your heart rate quickens ever-so-slightly. And with this, you may notice that you really want to eat a brownie. If so, then what you are experiencing is known as craving.
Figure 1.
Brownie for craving demonstration. Image courtesy of istockphoto.com/portfolio/anthonysp.
Craving can be defined in a myriad of ways. In our conceptualization – as in most common conceptualizations – craving is defined as a strong desire, urge, or wanting [e.g., 1, 2–4]. For example, in the current 5th edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM-5; 3], craving is listed as a diagnostic criterion for substance use disorders and is defined as “a strong desire or urge to use drugs.”
As a human experience, craving has been troubling philosophers for centuries. In the Buddhist Four Noble Truths, craving is said to be the cause of all suffering [5]; the philosopher Baruch Spinoza said that it is “the very essence of man” [6]; and Bertrand Russell said that it prompts all human activity [7]. These thinkers join in the belief that craving is a very common experience (as confirmed by modern epidemiological studies showing that craving is experienced by −99% of adults [8]) that also drives behavior [9].
Craving as a Learned Response
Psychologically, we consider craving to be a complex, multidimensional construct that can be understood using a learning-based model of behavior. To conceptualize this, one could think back to the classical experiments of Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov. In the late 19th century, Pavlov began researching salivation in dogs in response to feeding. He started with the fundamental observation that his dogs naturally salivated in response to eating food, and often as soon as it was placed in front of them. Then, Pavlov started to ring a bell each time before placing food in front of the dogs. After a number of repeated bell-food pairings, Pavlov rang the bell without placing food in front of the dogs, and observed that they salivated in response to the sound of the bell, even when food did not follow. Pavlov formalized his observations into a paradigm now known as classical or Pavlovian conditioning [10]. In this framework, ingested food is considered to be an “unconditioned stimulus” and salivation is considered to be an “unconditioned response” (that is, these “unconditioned” factors do not depend on or require any learning; salivation is a natural response to food). When Pavlov introduced the bell before food, he was adding a “neutral stimulus” that previously did not elicit a salivation response from the dogs. After repeated pairing with the food (the unconditioned stimulus), the bell became a “conditioned stimulus” that predicts food presentation, and came to elicit a “conditioned response,” salivation. As such, bell-cued salivation is a learned stimulus-response connection.
In the years since, research has repeatedly shown similar effects in animals as well as in humans. Indeed, after repeated exposure, food-related cues (e.g. labels/packaging for brownies, the sight of brownies) that are present at the time of eating acquire the ability to predict the ingested food, becoming conditioned stimuli that evoke conditioned responses. Specifically, experimental work in animals and humans has shown that cues paired with food elicit strong conditioned responses, including increased salivation [10, 11], heart rate [12], gastric activity [13], hormone secretions [14, 15], dopamine neuron firing and release [16, 17], and subsequent neural activity in regions typically associated with reward [18].
Food cues that become conditioned (e.g., food pictures, advertisements, labeling) are often external. However, for humans in particular, conditioned food cues can also include internal factors such as the experience of stress [19], negative emotion [20], hormonal changes [21], and food-related thoughts [22]. Such conditioned cues reliably evoke conditioned bodily responses including increased salivation [11], heart rate [12, 23], gastric activity [12, 23] and neural activity in the ventral striatum [VS; a region typically associated with reward; 18, 24]. Further, these physiological conditioned responses are often accompanied by the conscious experience of craving, which in this context is termed “cue-induced craving” [25, 26]. Thus, we can think of craving as a type of cue reactivity, or as an emergent psychological property of physiological cue reactivity, or more broadly as a conditioned response to food cues. Accordingly, experimental work in humans has demonstrated that exposure to food cues strongly and consistently produces the conscious experience of craving, and that these associations are learned quickly [27]. To illustrate, even previously-neutral stimuli (e.g., serving trays) can come to elicit cue-induced craving for chocolate when presented alone, after just a single Pavlovian conditioning session in which they were paired with chocolate [e.g., 27].
Notably, craving can be experienced for specific foods (e.g., chocolate), for groups of foods (e.g., desserts), and for food in general. Across such contexts, craving is traditionally assessed by asking individuals to self-report their current desire to eat, or the current intensity of their craving. To this end, researchers have used both multi-item questionnaires and single-item scales. For example, some studies have used the Food Cravings Questionnaire-State to assess current levels of craving for specific foods, which includes questions such as “I have an intense desire to eat [one or more specific foods]” [e.g., 28, 29–32]. Other studies have used the Food Craving Inventory to assess frequency of cravings for four specific categories of foods (sweets, fats, carbohydrates, and fast food fats); scores can also be calculated for food in general [e.g., 31, 33, 34, 35]. Other scales have also been used to assess frequency and intensity for food cravings, such as those developed by Weingarten and Elston [36, 37], and Hill [37, 38]. Further, many studies have assessed craving (including cue-induced craving) for specific foods by collecting responses to single-item, Likert-type or Visual Analog Scale, with questions such as “how much do you want to eat this food” or “rate the intensity of your craving” [e.g., 2, 39, 40–45].
Food cues, Craving, Eating, and the “Toxic Food Environment”
In the literature, it has been consistently documented that food and drug cues increase craving. For example, an early meta-analysis by Carter & Tiffany (1999) showed that across drugs of abuse, participants demonstrated significant increases in self-reported craving when exposed to drug-related vs. neutral stimuli [46]. Similar data have been reported for food, such that exposure to food cues significantly increases craving for food [e.g., 25, 32, 40, 47–50]. Importantly, a large body of work has shown that food cue exposure not only increases physiological responses and craving, but also increases eating. Further, this body of work has also shown that the magnitude of cue reactivity and of cue-induced craving consistently and reliably predicts subsequent eating as well as weight gain in both adults and children [9, 51]. We recently assessed this systematically in a meta-analysis that quantified the effects of food cue exposure, cue reactivity, and the experience of craving on eating and weight-related outcomes in both the short- and long-term [9]. Overall, we found medium effect sizes of food cue exposure, cue reactivity, and craving on eating and weight outcomes, suggesting that cue exposure and craving lead to eating and weight gain. Taken together, these data are consistent with the idea that food cues increase craving, and that craving then leads to eating. Another meta-analysis focused on a subset of studies that used food advertisements as cues; this meta-analysis reported a small-to-moderate effect size of advertisement exposure on eating, such that participants ate more after exposure to food advertising [51]. Together, these findings show that cue exposure increases eating and that the magnitude of cue reactivity and craving reliably predicts eating and weight-related outcomes. This suggests that they are important targets for the prevention and treatment of obesity.
Indeed, the implications of such findings extend to the natural ecology, where food cues for energy-dense “unhealthy” foods – such as advertisements for fast food – are abundant. We now live in an environment with pervasive advertisements (e.g., TV commercials, billboards) for low-nutrient, high-calorie foods [52, 53]. According to the Rudd Center for Food Policy & Obesity, food companies spent $1.28 billion advertising snack foods in 2014, an increase of 4% from the $1.23 billion spent in 2010 [54]. During this time, exposure to such advertising increased by 10% and 29% for children and teens, respectively [54]. In addition, the number of advertisements viewed by adults has steadily increased since 2007, growing from −5,500 in 2007 to >7,000 in 2014 [55]. On a population level, this increase in food cues leads to increases in eating: not surprisingly, both adults and children who live in a food environment with advertised, high-calorie foods (e.g. fast food) eat those foods more frequently and have higher Body Mass Index [BMI; 56, 57–59]. Some have deemed this a “toxic food environment,” in which widespread food cues lead to increased food consumption and weight gain [60–62]. These outcomes have important public health implications. Today, over two-thirds of the United States population is overweight or obese, and obesity is the second leading cause of preventable morbidity and mortality in the United States [63, 64].
Neural Mechanisms Underlying Cue Reactivity and Craving
Given the link between food cues, craving, and eating – and the possible role this has in the obesity epidemic – much neuroimaging work has been devoted to understanding the neural mechanisms underlying these processes. This work has revealed that cue reactivity and cue-induced craving express in brain regions that could serve as targets for modulation.
Prior brain imaging work in drug users has shown that cue exposure consistently increases craving along with neural activity in regions including the ventral tegmental area [VTA; e.g., 43, 65], ventral striatum [VS; e.g., 43, 66], amygdala [65, 67], insula [67, 68], orbitofrontal cortex [OFC; e.g., 43, 65, 66, 67], and anterior cingulate cortex [ACC; e.g., 43, 65, 66], as well as its subgenual region [sgACC; e.g., 43, 66]. These regions have previously been associated with emotion [particularly, the amygdala, VS, OFC, ACC; 69, 70, 71] and the experience of drug effects, which have been shown to activate the dopamine reward pathway [e.g., 72, 73]. Importantly, these regions have also been implicated in food cue exposure and craving: similar studies have been conducted with food stimuli, and the regions involved are largely similar to those for drugs, in both healthy individuals and those with eating pathology [18, 24, 74].
Over the past decade, several meta-analyses have been conducted to elucidate the robustness and reliability of brain networks and regions involved in food cue reactivity. One meta-analysis identified the insula, a region commonly associated with gustatory/taste processing [75], to consistently respond to olfactory as well visual food cues [76]. Another meta-analysis focusing on response to visual food cues identified the OFC, insula, and fusiform gyrus, with hunger modulating the response in the amygdala and OFC, and energy content of the food modulating the response in the VS [77]. Other meta-analyses have focused on these processes in individuals with obesity, and found that these individuals exhibit greater activation of VS and ACC, along with lower activation in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in response to visual food cues, compared with healthy individuals [78, 79]. Studies have also suggested that self-reported craving correlates with neural activity in regions including VS and OFC [e.g., 43, 80]; results from one study further suggest that neural response to food cues in the VS predicts future weight gain [81]. Notably, this network of regions that are linked with cue reactivity and craving lies along the reward-related dopamine pathway [82], overlaps with the default mode network [83], is commonly activated in both positive and negative emotion [e.g., 70, 84], and can serve as a target for modulation. Indeed, these are the regions in which we would expect to see reduced activity if craving is effectively regulated.
Strategies and Interventions to Reduce Craving and Eating
To address obesity on a population level, some public-health interventions have attempted to change the structure of the food environment by altering food prices (e.g., soda tax) or reducing availability of foods failing to meet specific nutrition standards [e.g., junk food bans; 85]. Other approaches have attempted to alter the choice architecture surrounding eating behaviors by “nudging” people towards healthy behaviors [i.e., by presenting food in smaller portions or having healthy optimal defaults in menus; 85, 86–88]. Unfortunately, these interventions have had relatively little success in real-world settings because they are limited in implementation and generalizability [89–91]. Further, even if such changes were successfully implemented, ultimately, most people would still be exposed to tempting food cues in situations in which these nudge strategies have yet to be implemented (e.g., in our own kitchens). Therefore, it is critical to consider the mechanisms underlying this epidemic and identify targets for interventions on an individual level.
For example, how can we reduce craving in an environment rife with food cues? Under tempting circumstances, we could exert self-control over our thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, which can be facilitated through the application of cognitive strategies [92]. Consistently, cognitive strategies for the regulation of craving are an important component of cognitive-behavioral treatments (CBTs) for obesity and eating disorders [93], which are known to be effective [94, 95]. We developed the Regulation of Craving (ROC) task to experimentally measure the specific causal effect of regulation strategies on craving, as well as the neural mechanisms underlying craving and its regulation [e.g., 42, 43].
In the original study of the ROC task, cigarette smokers and healthy adults were presented with photographs of cigarettes and foods that had been previously shown to induce craving [42]. During each trial of the ROC task (see Figure 2 for schematic representation), participants were exposed to one of these cues, and were instructed to follow one of two instructions: NOW – focus on the immediate feelings associated with consuming the item (e.g., it will taste good), or LATER – focus on the long-term negative consequences associated with repeated consumption (e.g., this increases my risk for heart disease). Then, participants were asked to rate their craving for the specific food or cigarette they just saw, using a 1 (not at all) to 5 (very much) Likert-type scale (Figure 2). Importantly, the LATER instruction is a strategy drawn from CBTs for addiction, eating disorders, and obesity [e.g., 96, 97, 98]. In addition, its prospective thinking component is related more broadly to Episodic Future Thinking (EFT), which involves engaging episodic memory to vividly simulate future events [99. 100]. and has been shown to reduce monetary delay discounting and caloric consumption [101–103]. Behaviorally, participants reported significantly reduced cravings for both food and cigarettes after applying the LATER strategy vs. the NOW instruction. In a follow up study, we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to probe the neural systems underlying these effects [43]. In this study, we replicated the original results, finding significant reductions in craving in the LATER condition. Further, we found that this cognitive down-regulation of craving was associated with decreased activation in regions implicated in craving (e.g., VS & sgACC, vmPFC), along with recruitment of regions associated with cognitive control (e.g., dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; dlPFC, vlPFC). In addition, these two neural effects were found to be related to each other: decreases in behavioral reports of craving correlated with decreases in VS activity and were inversely correlated with increases in dlPFC activity. Importantly, VS activity fully mediated the relationship between dlPFC activity and reported craving, suggesting that the dlPFC could exert an inhibitory effect on VS, which in turn affects the experience and reports of craving.
Figure 2.
Regulation of Food Craving (ROC) Task
Schematic representation of one trial from the original Regulation of Craving (ROC) task. Participants first saw a fixation cross, followed by an instruction. Specifically, NOW instructed participants to focus on the immediate feelings associated with consuming the item (e.g., it will taste good). LATER – a strategy drawn from CBT treatment protocols – indicated that they should focus on the long-term negative consequences associated with repeated consumption (e.g., this increases my risk for heart disease). Instructions were followed by an image, including unhealthy foods, after which participants rated their craving. Pizza image courtesy of istockphoto.com/portfolio/bhofack2.
The downregulatory effects of cognitive strategies on craving have been replicated many times, in both healthy populations and drug-using populations, for both food and drugs [44, 104– 113]. In addition, the role of the dlPFC has been well established in the context of emotion regulation, and regulation of craving for drugs and food [e.g., 43, 71, 109, 114–117], as well as in EFT [100]. In subsequent studies, we also tested slightly different instructions words (e.g., NEGATIVE instead of LATER) and replicated the original findings as well (see below).
These promising effects led us to develop training protocols targeting cue reactivity and cue-induced craving, with the hopes that such preemptive training in cognitive strategies, prior to temptation, would improve subsequent eating behavior [P50 DA09241; 44, 118]. This approach has several advantages. First, cognitive strategies can be generated by the individual and are adaptable based on the situation, unlike externally-applied, situation-specific nudges. Second, rather than modifying the environment, training in cognitive strategies aims to alter the internal architecture of food choices, including how individuals crave and value foods, allowing individuals to nudge themselves toward change. Such training may also alter the subjective valuation of healthy vs. unhealthy options during food choice. Indeed, it has been proposed that value computation of choice options is a key factor in successful self-control [119–122]. Consistent with this idea, computational work has suggested that food choices are driven (at least to some degree) by estimations of the benefit or value of possible outcomes. In turn, these estimations are based on (a) the value assigned to each item on various dimensions (e.g., the healthiness and tastiness of foods), and (b) the weight assigned to each dimension (e.g., importance of healthiness vs. tastiness) by any particular individual, at any particular timepoint [107, 122–126]. In turn, this further suggests that training in cognitive strategies – as described below – could alter either the value of options or the weight placed on dimensions of health and taste, thus leading to sustainable change.
To test this, we conducted several studies to develop an intervention to improve food choices and eating behavior [44]. First, we developed and tested two cognitive strategy instructions that frame foods as “bad for you” or “good for you.” The NEGATIVE strategy involves thinking about negative consequences of eating food (e.g. health consequences or disliking the taste), whereas the POSITIVE strategy involves thinking about the positive aspects of eating food (e.g. health benefits or liking the taste). Across two studies, we exposed participants to both healthy and unhealthy foods, and compared participants’ self-reported craving for these foods (assessed on a 1 to 5 Likert-type scale following each specific food) after following these instructions to a control LOOK instruction (i.e., just look at the food and respond naturally).
Consistent with prior work, we found that the NEGATIVE strategy decreased craving for both unhealthy as well as healthy foods. In contrast, the POSITIVE strategy increased craving for healthy as well as unhealthy foods. Importantly, we found that both strategies also influenced the subjective valuation of both healthy and unhealthy foods, operationalized as willingness-to-pay (WTP), such that the POSITIVE strategy increased WTP by 21-69 cents, and the NEGATIVE strategy decreased WTP by 56-80 cents, compared to the LOOK instruction. To illustrate this, participants in one of our studies were willing to pay $1.28 (US dollars) more for healthy foods like broccoli, after using the POSITIVE vs. the NEGATIVE strategy. They were also willing to pay $1.75 less for unhealthy foods like brownies after using the NEGATIVE strategy. Notably, this magnitude of value change is far greater than reported effects of other population-level interventions; it is at least 2.5-10x more than that of the proposed penny-perounce soda tax [127]. Finally, these cognitive strategies were effective across individual differences in BMI.
Based on these strategies (and ongoing work with cigarette smokers), we developed Regulation of Craving Training (ROC-T) as a novel and brief intervention [44]. ROC-T contains both a framing (informational essay) and a training component, and it trains participants to use cognitive strategies to either (a) increase craving for healthy foods (POSITIVE ROC-T condition), or (b) decrease craving for unhealthy foods (NEGATIVE ROC-T condition; conditions are italicized; strategies are not italicized). In the next set of studies, we tested whether ROC-T, compared to a CONTROL no-training “look-only” condition, could change choices of healthy vs. unhealthy foods as well as food consumption. In these studies, participants completed a binary food choice task (e.g., healthy food vs. unhealthy food) before and after ROC-T (See Figure 3 for schematic representation of one choice; see [44] for schematics of the full studies). In one of the studies, participants also completed a “food taste test” following the second food choice task, in which we measured caloric intake. Across studies, we found that after completing ROC-T, participants in both the POSITIVE and NEGATIVE ROC-T conditions chose significantly more healthy vs. unhealthy foods, and that these participants consumed 93-121 fewer calories compared to participants CONTROL. Finally, in another study, we compared ROC-T to a framing-only control condition, and found that the training component of ROC-T is an important “active ingredient”; that is, the training component was necessary for maximal change in food choice to take place.
Figure 3.
Food Choice Task
Schematic representation of one trial from the Food Choice Task. Before and after ROC-T, participants completed binary food choices, most of which were between healthy and unhealthy foods (other choices were between healthy vs. healthy or between unhealthy vs. unhealthy foods). Pizza image courtesy of istockphoto.com/portfolio/bhofack2. Salad image courtesy of istockphoto.com/portfolio/semenovp.
Future Directions
Taken together, this work shows that cognitive strategies can be used effectively to regulate craving. Based on that initial insight, we developed a targeted, mechanism-focused, cognitive strategy-based intervention to prevent and reduce unhealthy eating that also alters the valuation of healthy and unhealthy foods. We are currently pursuing several avenues of work to extend these findings. For example, we are testing the boundary conditions of ROC-T, such as optimal “dose” and durability of effects under time pressure, stress, and cognitive load. In addition, we are investigating the neural mechanisms underlying the promising effects of ROC-T. We are also beginning to test the longevity of these effects, including the cumulative effects of repeated sessions and their effect on long-term outcomes such as weight, and to consider the effects of ROC-T on other constructs such as food reinforcement.
Further, dismantling and comparing the efficacy of specific components of the POSITIVE and NEGATIVE strategies is important to explore. Overall, there is limited work examining the efficacy of different strategies and their components in the context of craving, and existing evidence does not clearly indicate that certain strategies are differentially effective. For example, prior research in cigarette smokers has tested the efficacy of gain-framed (i.e., benefits of quitting smoking) vs. loss-framed messages (i.e., costs of continuing to smoke) on smoking cessation. This work suggests that gain-framed messages may be more effective in the short-term [128–130], but not the long term [128], and that this difference is influenced by gender and risk perceptions associated with quitting smoking [130]. In our own work testing regulation of craving training (ROC-T), we found that the NEGATIVE ROC-T and POSITIVE ROC-T condition were equally effective in improving food choice across three of four studies [44]. Further, in another study testing the regulation of food craving participants were allowed to select cognitive strategies from a suggested list [113]. The authors reported that the most commonly applied strategies were (1) focusing on the negative consequences of eating the food (as in our ROC task), and (2) imagining that something was wrong with the food [113]. However, reduction in craving did not vary by strategy [113]. Thus, it is important to explore additional strategies that may be differentially effective (e.g., mindfulness), and to further ask whether there are individual differences in strategy choice and efficacy.
In addition, because the food environment often presents more complex situations than making binary decisions between “healthy” and “unhealthy” foods, one useful next step would be to investigate foods that are often thought of as healthy but are also energy-dense (e.g., nuts, avocados) and thus might be detrimental to goals related to weight loss. Thus, we hope to make ROC-T adaptable to different eating and weight goals. Ultimately, we hope that ROC-T can serve as a standalone computerized intervention as well as “homework” as part of longer interventions for healthy eating, weight loss, obesity, and potentially for eating disorders.
Conclusion
Craving – in the context of food – is defined as a strong desire to eat, and has troubled philosophers for centuries [5–7]. It is a common experience [8] that drives eating behavior and predicts weight gain over time [9]. Craving can be understood using a learning-based model of behavior, where it has been formalized within the framework of classical or Pavlovian conditioning [10]. Within this framework, food cues (such as sight or smell of food, food pictures, and advertisements) that are present at the time of eating become conditioned stimuli that acquire the ability to predict eating, and evoke conditioned responses such as increased salivation [11], heart rate [12, 23], gastric activity [12, 23], and neural activity in regions typically associated with reward [e.g., VS; 18, 24]. In humans, these responses are often in tandem with the conscious experience of craving, or “cue-induced craving” [25, 26]. As such, craving can be considered to be a type of cue reactivity, or an emergent property from it.
This is important clinically because we now live in an environment with pervasive food cues, such as TV advertisements [52, 53]. Over the past decade, industry spending on advertisements for low-nutrient, high-calorie foods and population exposure to such advertisements have steadily increased [54, 55]. Experts have deemed this a “toxic food environment,” in which abundant food cues lead to increased eating and weight gain [60–62], exacerbating the obesity epidemic [63, 64]. Indeed, we and others have used systematic and quantitative meta-analytic techniques to show that cue exposure increases eating, and that cue-induced craving reliably predicts eating and weight outcomes [9, 51]. Thus, they are promising targets for the prevention and treatment of obesity.
Thus, we developed and tested an individual-level, internally-applied intervention based on prior work showing that cognitive strategies for the regulation of craving successfully reduce self-reported craving and reward-related neural reactivity to unhealthy food cues [e.g., 43]. We first demonstrated that cognitive strategies decrease craving for unhealthy foods by emphasizing their negative consequences, increase craving for healthy foods by emphasizing their positive benefits, and importantly, change food valuation (willingness to pay) for both healthy and unhealthy foods [44]. In addition, we showed that Regulation of Craving Training (ROC-T) – a novel intervention based on these cognitive strategies – significantly increased healthy food choice and reduced caloric consumption [44]. Finally, we found that the training component of ROC-T was necessary for maximal change in food choice [44]. To extend these promising findings, we are now investigating the boundary conditions, neural mechanisms, and longevity of ROC-T. Taken together, these findings and future directions have important theoretical, public health, and clinical implications for the prevention and treatment of obesity and eating disorders.
Highlights.
Craving – defined as a strong desire to eat – is a common experience
Food craving predicts eating and weight gain
Cognitive strategies can be used to regulate craving
Training in cognitive strategies reduces eating and improves food choice
Acknowledgements
The authors thank many co-authors and fellow researchers for their contributions to the research in this review; Italia Hanik, Nilo Vafay, Sophy Xiong, and Sophie Shang for help copyediting; and Uri Berger, and David Saunders for their helpful comments on the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the NIH grant P50 DA09241 (project 3) and R01 DA043690 to HK.
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
Competing Interests Statement
The authors declare no competing financial interests. HK provided consulting services to Indivior, Inc, on topics unrelated to this manuscript or this work.
References
- [1].Havermans RC “You Say it’s Liking, I Say it’s Wanting…”. On the difficulty of disentangling food reward in man. Appetite. 2011,57:286–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [2].Cornell CE, Rodin J, Weingarten HP Stimulus-Induced Eating When Satiated. Physiology & Behavior. 1989,45:695–704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [3].APA. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders - 5th Edition Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- [4].Robinson TE, Berridge KC The neural basis of drug craving: An incentive-sensitization theory of addiction. Brain Research Reviews. 1993,18:247–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [5].Bodhi B In the Buddha’s words: An anthology of discourses from the Pali Canon. Boston, MA: Simon and Schuster; 2005. [Google Scholar]
- [6].Spinoza B The Ethics: Treatise on the Emendation of the Intellect. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett Publishing Company; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- [7].Russell B What desires are politically important?: PA Norstedt; 1950. [Google Scholar]
- [8].Osman JL, Sobal J Chocolate cravings in American and Spanish individuals: Biological and cultural influences. Appetite. 2006,47:290–301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [9].Boswell RG, Kober H Food cue reactivity and craving predict eating and weight gain: A meta analytic review. Obesity Reviews. 2016,17:159–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [10].Pavlov IP Conditioned Reflexes. New York: Dover Publications; 1927. [Google Scholar]
- [11].Nederkoorn C, Smulders FT, Jansen A Recording of swallowing events using electromyography as a non-invasive measurement of salivation. Appetite. 1999,33:361–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [12].Nederkoorn C, Smulders FT, Jansen A Cephalic phase responses, craving and food intake in normal subjects. Appetite. 2000,35:45–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [13].Stern RM, Crawford HE, Stewart WR, Vasey MW, Koch KL Sham feeding. Digestive diseases and sciences. 1989,34:521–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [14].Sugino T, Hasegawa Y, Kurose Y, Kojima M, Kangawa K, Terashima Y Effects of ghrelin on food intake and neuroendocrine function in sheep. Anim Reprod Sci. 2004,82–83:183–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [15].Teff KL, Engelman K Palatability and dietary restraint: effect on cephalic phase insulin release in women. Physiology & behavior. 1996,60:567–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [16].Schultz W A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward. Science. 1997,275:1593–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [17].Roitman MF, Stuber GD, Phillips PE, Wightman RM, Carelli RM Dopamine operates as a subsecond modulator of food seeking. J Neurosci. 2004,24:1265–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [18].Pelchat ML, Johnson A, Chan R, Valdez J, Ragland JD Images of desire: food-craving activation during fMRI. Neuroimage. 2004,23:1486–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [19].Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, Potenza MN Neural correlates of stress- and food-cue induced food craving in obesity. Diabetes Care. 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [20].Hepworth R, Mogg K, Brignell C, Bradley BP Negative mood increases selective attention to food cues and subjective appetite. Appetite. 2010,54:134–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [21].Malik S, McGlone F, Bedrossian D, Dagher A Ghrelin modulates brain activity in areas that control appetitive behavior. Cell metabolism. 2008,7:400–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [22].Richard A, Meule A, Reichenberger J, Blechert J Food cravings in everyday life: An EMA study on snack-related thoughts, cravings, and consumption. Appetite. 2017,113:215–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [23].Nederkoorn C, Jansen A Cue reactivity and regulation of food intake. Eating Behaviors. 2002,3:61–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [24].Tang D, Fellows L, Small D, Dagher A Food and drug cues activate similar brain regions: a meta-analysis of functional MRI studies. Physiology & behavior. 2012,106:317–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [25].Jansen A A learning model of binge eating: cue reactivity and cue exposure. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 1998,36:257–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [26].Jansen A, Havermans R, Nederkoorn C Cued Overeating In: Preedy VR, ed. Handbook of Behavior, Food, and Nutrition. New York: Springer; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- [27].Van Gucht D, Vansteenwegen D, Van den Bergh O, Beckers T Conditioned craving cues elicit an automatic approach tendency. Behav Res Ther. 2008,46:1160–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [28].Cepeda-Benito A, Gleaves DH, Williams TL, Erath SA The development and validation of the state and trait food-cravings questionnaires. Behavior therapy. 2000,31:151–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [29].Moreno S, Rodriguez S, Fernandez MC, Tamez J, Cepeda-Benito A Clinical validation of the trait and state versions of the Food Craving Questionnaire. Assessment. 2008,15:375–87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [30].Cepeda-Benito A, Fernandez MC, Moreno S Relationship of gender and eating disorder symptoms to reported cravings for food: construct validation of state and trait craving questionnaires in Spanish. Appetite. 2003,40:47–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [31].Batra P, Das SK, Salinardi T, Robinson L, Saltzman E, Scott T, et al. Relationship of cravings with weight loss and hunger. Results from a 6 month worksite weight loss intervention. Appetite. 2013,69:1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [32].Ng L, Davis C Cravings and food consumption in Binge Eating Disorder. Eat Behav. 2013,14:472–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [33].White MA, Whisenhant BL, Williamson DA, Greenway FL, Netemeyer RG Development and validation of the food-craving inventory. Obesity Research. 2002,10:107–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [34].Martin CK, O’Neil PM, Tollefson G, Greenway FL, White MA The association between food cravings and consumption of specific foods in a laboratory taste test. Appetite. 2008,51:324–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [35].Jakubowicz D, Froy O, Wainstein J, Boaz M Meal timing and composition influence ghrelin levels, appetite scores and weight loss maintenance in overweight and obese adults. Steroids. 2012,77:323–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [36].Weingarten HP, Elston D Food Cravings in a College Population. Appetite. 1991,17:167–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [37].Gilhooly CH, Das SK, Golden JK, McCrory MA, Dallal GE, Saltzman E, et al. Food cravings and energy regulation: the characteristics of craved foods and their relationship with eating behaviors and weight change during 6 months of dietary energy restriction. International journal of obesity. 2007,31:1849–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [38].Hill AJW, C. FL; Blundell JE, Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite. 1991,17:187–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [39].Buckland NJ, Finlayson G, Hetherington MM Slimming starters. Intake of a diet-congruent food reduces meal intake in active dieters. Appetite. 2013,71:430–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [40].Buckland NJ, Finlayson G, Hetherington MM Pre-exposure to diet-congruent food reduces energy intake in restrained dieting women. Eat Behav. 2013,14:249–54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [41].Lopez RB, Hofmann W, Wagner DD, Kelley WM, Heatherton TF Neural predictors of giving in to temptation in daily life. Psychol Sci. 2014,25:1337–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [42].Kober H, Kross EF, Mischel W, Hart CL, Ochsner KN Regulation of craving by cognitive strategies in cigarette smokers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 2010,106:52–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [43].Kober H, Mende-Siedlecki P, Kross EF, Weber J, Mischel W, Hart CL, et al. Prefrontal-striatal pathway underlies cognitive regulation of craving. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010,107:14811–6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [44].Boswell RG, Sun W, Suzuki S, Kober H Training in Cognitive Strategies Reduces Eating and Improves Food Choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (PNAS). 2018,115:E11238–E47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [45].Fedoroff I, Polivy J, Herman CP The specificity of restrained versus unrestrained eaters’ responses to food cues: general desire to eat, or craving for the cued food? Appetite. 2003,41:7–13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [46].Carter BL, Tiffany ST Meta-analysis of cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction. 1999,94:327–40. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [47].Fedoroff I, Polivy J, Herman CP The Effect of Pre-exposure to Food Cues on the Eating Behavior of Restrained and Unrestrained Eaters. Appetite. 1997,28:33–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [48].Jansen A, Vanreyten A, van Balveren T, Roefs A, Nederkoorn C, Havermans R Negative affect and cue-induced overeating in non-eating disordered obesity. Appetite. 2008,51:556–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [49].Lambert KG, Neal T, Noyes J, Parker C, Worrel P Food-related Stimuli Increase Desire to Eat in Hungry and Satiated Human Subjects. Current Psychology: Research & Reviews. 1991,10:297–303. [Google Scholar]
- [50].Ferriday D, Brunstrom JM How does food-cue exposure lead to larger meal sizes? Br J Nutr. 2008,100:1325–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [51].Boyland EJ, Nolan S, Kelly B, Tudur-Smith C, Jones A, Halford JC, et al. Advertising as a cue to consume: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of acute exposure to unhealthy food and nonalcoholic beverage advertising on intake in children and adults. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2016,103:519–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [52].Harris JL, Pomeranz JL, Lobstein T, Brownell KD A crisis in the marketplace: how food marketing contributes to childhood obesity and what can be done. Annu Rev Public Health. 2009,30:211–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [53].Gearhardt AN, Bragg MA, Pearl RL, Schvey NA, Roberto CA, Brownell KD Obesity and public policy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2012,8:405–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [54].Harris JL, Schwartz M, Shehan CV, Hyary M, Appel J, Haraghey K, et al. Snack FACTS 2015: Evaluating snack food nutrition and marketing to youth. Rudd Center for Food Policy & Obesity. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [55].Shehan CV, Harris JL Trends in television food advertising to young people: 2014 update. Rudd report. 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [56].Prentice AMJ, Fast foods SA, energy density and obesity: a possible mechanistic link. Obes Rev. 2003,4:187–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [57].Jeffery RW, Baxter J, McGuire M, Linde J Are fast food restaurants an environmental risk factor for obesity? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2006,3:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [58].Rosenheck R Fast food consumption and increased caloric intake: a systematic review of a trajectory towards weight gain and obesity risk. Obes Rev. 2008,9:535–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [59].McClure AC, Tanski SE, Gilbert-Diamond D, Adachi-Mejia AM, Li Z, Li Z, et al. Receptivity to television fast-food restaurant marketing and obesity among U.S. youth. Am J Prev Med. 2013,45:560–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [60].Brownell KD, Horgen KB Food Fight: The Inside Story of the Food Industry, America’s Obesity Crisis, and What We Can Do About It. New York: McGraw Hill, Contemporary Books; 2004. [Google Scholar]
- [61].Morris MJ, Beilharz JE, Maniam J, Reichelt AC, Westbrook RF Why is obesity such a problem in the 21st century? The intersection of palatable food, cues and reward pathways, stress, and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [62].Wadden TA, Brownell KD, Foster GD Obesity: Responding to the global epidemic. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2002,70:510–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [63].Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011-2012. Jama. 2014,311:806–14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [64].Flegal KM, Kruszon-Moran D, Carroll MD, Fryar CD, Ogden CL Trends in obesity among adults in the United States, 2005 to 2014. JAMA. 2016,315:2284–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [65].Due DL, Huettel SA, Hall WG, Rubin DC Activation in Mesolimbic and Visuospatial Neural Circuits Elicited by Smoking Cues: Evidence From Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002,159:954–60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [66].David SP, Munafo MR, Johansen-Berg H, Smith SM, Rogers RD, Matthews PM, et al. Ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens activation to smoking-related pictorial cues in smokers and nonsmokers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry. 2005,58:488–94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [67].Franklin TR, Wang Z, J. W, Sciortino N, Harper D, Li Y, et al. Limbic activation to cigarette smoking cues independent of nicotine withdrawal: a perfusion fMRI study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007,32:2301–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [68].Wilson SJ, Sayette MA, Delgado MR, Fiez JA Instructed smoking expectancy modulates cue-elicited neural activity: a preliminary study. Nicotine & Tobacco Research. 2005,7:637–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [69].Lindquist KA, Wager TD, Kober H, Bliss-Moreau E, Barrett LF The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2012,35:121–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [70].Kober H, Barrett LF, Joseph J, Bliss-Moreau E, Lindquist K, Wager TD Functional grouping and cortical-subcortical interactions in emotion: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage. 2008,42:998–1031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [71].Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez RB, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. 2014,24:2981–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [72].Di Chiara GD, Imperato A Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 1988,85:5274–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [73].Drevets WC, Gautier C, Price JC, Kupfer DJ, Kinahan PE, Grace AA, et al. Amphetamine-induced dopamine release in human ventral striatum correlates with euphoria. Biological Psychiatry. 2001,49:81–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [74].Schienle A, Schafer A, Hermann A, Vaitl D Binge-eating disorder: reward sensitivity and brain activation to images of food. Biol Psychiatry. 2009,65:654–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [75].de Araujo IE, Geha P, Small DM Orosensory and homeostatic functions of the insular taste cortex. Chemosensory perception. 2012,5:64–79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [76].Huerta CI, Sarkar PR, Duong TQ, Laird AR, Fox PT Neural bases of food perception: Coordinate-based meta-analyses of neuroimaging studies in multiple modalities. Obesity. 2014,22:1439–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [77].van der Laan LN, de Ridder DT, Viergever MA, Smeets PA The first taste is always with the eyes: a meta-analysis on the neural correlates of processing visual food cues. Neuroimage. 2011,55:296–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [78].Devoto F, Zapparoli L, Bonandrini R, Berlingeri M, Ferrulli A, Luzi L, et al. Hungry brains: A meta-analytical review of brain activation imaging studies on food perception and appetite in obese individuals. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [79].Brooks SJ, Cedernaes J, Schiöth HB Increased prefrontal and parahippocampal activation with reduced dorsolateral prefrontal and insular cortex activation to food images in obesity: a meta-analysis of fMRI studies. PloS one. 2013,8:e60393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [80].Ulrich M, Steigleder L, Grön G Neural signature of the food craving questionnaire (FCQ)-Trait. Appetite. 2016,107:303–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [81].Demos KE, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM Individual differences in nucleus accumbens activity to food and sexual images predict weight gain and sexual behavior. J Neurosci. 2012,32:5549–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [82].Haber SN, Knutson B The reward circuit: linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010,35:4–26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [83].Raichle ME The brain’s default mode network. Annual review of neuroscience. 2015,38:433–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [84].Wager TD, Barrett LF, Bliss-Moreau E, Lindquist K, Duncan S, Kober H, et al. The Neuroimaging of Emotion In: Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF, eds. Handbook of Emotions. 3rd ed New York: Guilford Press; 2008. p. 249–71. [Google Scholar]
- [85].Liu PJ, Wisdom J, Roberto CA, Liu LJ, Ubel PA Using behavioral economics to design more effective food policies to address obesity. Applied Economic Perspectives and Policy. 2014,36:6–24. [Google Scholar]
- [86].Johnson EJ, Shu SB, Dellaert BGC, Fox C, Goldstein DG, Haubl G, et al. Beyond nudges: Tools of a choice architecture. Marketing Letters. 2012,23:487–504. [Google Scholar]
- [87].Loewenstein GB,T; Volpp KG Asymmetric paternalism to improve health behaviors. JAMA. 2007,298:2415–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [88].Thaler RH, Sunstein CR Nudge: Improving Decisions About Health, Wealth, and Happiness. New York, NY: Penguin Books; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- [89].Crosbie E, Schillinger D, Schmidt LA State Preemption to Prevent Local Taxation of Sugar-Sweetened Beverages. JAMA Internal Medicine. 2019,179:291–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [90].Just D, Price J Default options, incentives and food choices: evidence from elementary-school children. Public health nutrition. 2013,16:2281–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [91].Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Hall KD, McPherson K, Finegood DT, Moodie ML, et al. The global obesity pandemic: shaped by global drivers and local environments. The Lancet. 2011,378:804–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [92].Gross JJ Emotion Regulation: Current Status and Future Prospects. Psychological Inquiry. 2015,26:1–26. [Google Scholar]
- [93].Wilson GT, Grilo CM, Vitousek KM Psychological treatment of eating disorders. American Psychologist. 2007,62:199–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [94].Corbalán MD, Morales EM, Canteras M, Espallardo A, Hernández T, Garaulet M Effectiveness of cognitive–behavioral therapy based on the Mediterranean diet for the treatment of obesity. Nutrition. 2009,25:861–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [95].Ashton K, Drerup M, Windover A, Heinberg L Brief, four-session group CBT reduces binge eating behaviors among bariatric surgery candidates. Surgery for Obesity and Related Diseases. 2009,5:257–62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [96].Carroll KM Therapy Manuals for Drug Addiction, Manual 1: A Cognitive-Behavioral Approach: Treating Cocaine Addiction Rockville, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse, US Deparment of Health and Human Services; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- [97].Grilo CM, Masheb RM, Wilson GT, Gueorguieva R, White MA Cognitive-behavioral therapy, behavioral weight loss, and sequential treatment for obese patients with binge-eating disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011,79:675–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [98].Fairburn CG, Cooper Z Eating Disorders: A Transdiagnostic Protocol In: Barlow DH, ed. Clinical handbook of psychological disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual. 5th ed New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2014. p. 670–702. [Google Scholar]
- [99].Atance CM, O’Neill DK Episodic future thinking. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2001,5:533–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [100].Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and applications. 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [101].Daniel TO, Stanton CM, Epstein LH The future is now: reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking. Psychological science. 2013,24:2339–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [102].Daniel TO, Said M, Stanton CM, Epstein LH Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and energy intake in children. Eating behaviors. 2015,18:20–4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [103].Dassen FC, Jansen A, Nederkoorn C, Houben K Focus on the future: episodic future thinking reduces discount rate and snacking. Appetite. 2016,96:327–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [104].Littel M, Franken IHA Intentional Modulation of the Late Positive Potential in Response to Smoking Cues by Cognitive Strategies in Smokers. PloS One. 2011,6:e27519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [105].Lopez RB, Vohs KD, Wagner DD, Heatherton TF Self-regulatory strength: neural mechanisms and implications for training In: Gendolla GHE, ed. Handbook of Biobehavioral Approaches to Self-Regulation. New York: Springer Science; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- [106].Naqvi NH, Ochsner KN, Kober H, Kuerbis A, Feng T, Wall M, et al. Cognitive regulation of craving in alcohol-dependent and social drinkers. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research. 2015,39:343–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [107].Hutcherson CA, Plassmann H, Gross JJ, Rangel A Cognitive regulation during decision making shifts behavioral control between ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal value systems. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012,32:13543–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [108].Yokum S, Stice E Cognitive regulation of food craving: effects of three cognitive reappraisal strategies on neural response to palatable foods. International Journal of Obesity. 2013,37:1565–70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [109].Suzuki S, Mell MM, O’Malley S, Krystal J, Anticevic A, Kober H Regulation of Craving and Negative Emotion in Alcohol Use Disorder. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [110].McDermott KED, Lillis J, McCaffery JM, Wing RR Effects of Cognitive Strategies on Neural Food Cue Reactivity in Adults with Overweight/Obesity. Obesity. 2019,27:1577–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [111].Ghahremani DG, Faulkner P, Cox C. M, London ED Behavioral and neural markers of cigarette-craving regulation in young-adult smokers during abstinence and after smoking. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2018,43:1616–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [112].Strickland JC, Reynolds AR, Stoops WW Regulation of cocaine craving by cognitive strategies in an online sample of cocaine users. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2016,30:607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [113].Giuliani NR, Calcott RD, Berkman ET Piece of cake. Cognitive reappraisal of food craving. Appetite. 2013,64:56–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [114].Volkow ND, Baler RD NOW vs LATER brain circuits: implications for obesity and addiction. Trends in Neurosciences. 2015,38:345–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [115].Silvers JA, Insel C, Powers A, Franz P, Weber J, Mischel W, et al. Curbing craving: behavioral and brain evidence that children regulate craving when instructed to do so but have higher baseline craving than adults. Psychological Science. 2014,25:1932–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [116].Volkow ND, Fowler JS, Wang GJ, Telang F, Logan J, Jayne M, et al. Cognitive control of drug craving inhibits brain reward regions in cocaine abusers. Neuroimage. 2010,49:2536–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [117].Wang GJ, Volkow ND, Telang F, Jayne M, Ma Y, Pradhan K, et al. Evidence of gender differences in the ability to inhibit brain activation elicited by food stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009,106:1249–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [118].Lopez RB, Ochsner KN, Kober H Brief training in regulation of craving reduces smoking: Effects of strategy. . in prep. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [119].Berkman ET, Hutcherson CA, Livingston JL, Kahn LE, Inzlicht M Self-control as value-based choice. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2017,26:422–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [120].Shenhav A The perils of losing control: Why self-control is not just another value-based decision. Psychological Inquiry. 2017,28:148–52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [121].Buckholtz JW Social norms, self-control, and the value of antisocial behavior. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015,3:122–9. [Google Scholar]
- [122].Rangel A Regulation of dietary choice by the decision-making circuitry. Nature Neuroscience. 2013,16:1717–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [123].Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A Self-control in decision-making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science. 2009,324:646–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [124].Hare TA, Malmaud J, Rangel A Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience. 2011,31:11077–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [125].Plassmann H, ODoherty JP, Rangel A Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision making. The Journal of neuroscience. 2010,30:10799–808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [126].Sokol-Hessner P, Hutcherson C, Hare T, Rangel A Decision value computation in DLPFC and VMPFC adjusts to the available decision time. Eur J Neurosci. 2012,35:1065–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [127].Brownell KD, Frieden TR Ounces of prevention - the public policy case for taxes on sugared beverages. New England Journal of Medicine. 2009,360:1805–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [128].Toll BA, Martino S, Latimer A, Salovey P, O’Malley S, Carlin-Menter S, et al. Randomized Trial: Quitline Specialist Training in Gain-Framed vs Standard-Care Messages for Smoking Cessation. Journal of the National Cancer Institute. 2010,102:96–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [129].Toll BA, O’Malley SS, Katulak NA, Wu R, Dubin JA, Latimer A, et al. Comparing Gain- and Loss-Framed Messages for Smoking Cessation With Sustained-Release Bupropion: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. 2007,21:534–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- [130].Toll BA, Salovey P, O’Malley SS, Mazure CM, Latimer A, McKee SA Message framing for smoking cessation: The interaction of risk perceptions and gender. Nicotine & tobacco research. 2008,10:195–200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]