Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2020 Aug 12;40(33):6264–6266. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0814-20.2020

Cannabis Extract Composition Determines Reinforcement in a Vapor Self-Administration Paradigm

Anand K Muthusamy 1,
PMCID: PMC7424876  PMID: 32801126

Introduction

The legalization of cannabis and shifting cultural attitudes have driven an increase in cannabis use and the proliferation of vapor delivery devices. The DSM-V recognizes “cannabis use disorder” under the umbrella of substance use disorders, but its neural mechanisms require greater clarity (Peters et al., 2020). Debate in the scientific community and the public sphere alike primarily asks, “is cannabis addictive?” and “are there negative effects from chronic use?” The first issue magnifies the second: if users compulsively seek cannabis or become dependent, then safe regimens become difficult to maintain.

Drug abuse studies in human populations generally are confounded by use of other drugs, medical history, and varying genetic background. Self-administration in animal models sidesteps these issues and has good construct validity given the volitional consumption (Koob et al., 2012). Unlike cocaine and opioid self-administration studies, however, self-administration of cannabis or Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) alone has been notoriously difficult to establish across several species because of THC's weak rewarding effect and aversive effects at high doses (Justinova et al., 2005; Fuchs et al., 2019). Although THC is the primary psychoactive compound in cannabis, extracts may have over 120 other phytocannabinoids, including cannabidiol (CBD), which has its own effects as an inverse agonist at cannabinoid receptors (Ibsen et al., 2017). Further complicating self-administration is the varying pharmacokinetics of different delivery methods. In particular, intravenous delivery, which is used for other addictive drugs, leads to fast infusion rates that trigger aversive effects for THC (Carbuto et al., 2012).

Freels et al. (2020) addressed these long-standing issues by designing a vapor-delivery method to successfully establish cannabis self-administration in rodents. Their paradigm bears the greatest similarity to human use to date by allowing volitional and titratable vapor delivery of cannabis extracts (MacCallum and Russo, 2018). The National Institute on Drug Abuse drug supply program provided whole cannabis extract enriched with either THC (CANTHC) or CBD (CANCBD). In the apparatus, rats could nose-poke to activate one of two ports indicated by a 60 s light cue. When one port was activated, one of three vapors was delivered: organic solvent vehicle, CANTHC, or CANCBD; when the other port was activated, nothing was delivered.

CANTHC uniquely reinforces self-administration by acting on cannabinoid receptor type 1 (CB1)

Freels et al. (2020) demonstrated that Sprague Dawley rats will stably self-administer CANTHC and perform more work for a single delivery of CANTHC vapor than for CANCBD or vehicle. Across fixed ratios of nose-pokes to vapor deliveries, rats maintained a consistent number of CANTHC vapor deliveries in each session, and the number of deliveries was significantly higher than for CANCBD or vehicle (∼5 to ∼12 deliveries/day, a baseline for nose-poking). In all experiments, Freels et al. (2020) observed some level of nose-poking for the vehicle delivery, suggesting some interest in the cue light and/or solvent vapor. Even under the 1:1 ratio, however, rats did not respond significantly more for CANCBD than for vehicle. Furthermore, when the rats faced a sequentially increasing number of nose-pokes required to earn vapor delivery, they worked significantly more for CANTHC, but not for CANCBD, than for vehicle. Notably, rats nose-poked for CANTHC most often in the first 15 min of each 1 h session. These results suggest that rats learn to nose-poke at a certain rate to achieve a desired THC level. Maintaining CANTHC consumption under a mounting workload points to the drug's reinforcing efficacy and is comparable with the human motivation to devote time and effort to seek an appealing stimulus (Fuchs et al., 2019).

Freels et al. (2020) found that systemic injection of AM251, a CB1-selective antagonist, reduced the CANTHC vapor seeking rate to vehicle control levels, whereas the CANCBD group was unaffected. While CB1 is widely expressed across the mammalian brain, a well-characterized midbrain reward mechanism implicated in the self-administration of other addictive substances likely underlies CANTHC's effect (Gardner, 2005). THC acts as partial agonist of CB1, which is abundantly expressed in VTA GABAergic terminals (Sperlágh et al., 2009). In the VTA, the activation of CB1 diminishes the GABAergic inhibition of dopaminergic neurons that project to the NAc (Gardner, 2005; Peters et al., 2020). The resulting increase in dopaminergic tone in the NAc is rewarding and can establish drug addiction (Peters et al., 2020).

THC:CBD ratio in extracts determines selectivity in self-administration and presentation of the tetrad response

Surprisingly, although rats were not willing to work as hard to earn CANCBD as they were to earn CANTHC, they self-administered CANCBD more selectively. In the behavior apparatus, the rats could learn which port provided any vapor as opposed to no outcome. Only the rats earning CANCBD achieved a fraction of nose-pokes at the active port that was significantly greater than the vehicle group. Quantification of cannabinoid concentrations in the rat brain likely explains this finding. Each of the two cannabis extracts had a small quantity of the nonenriched compound. CANTHC extract had a THC concentration nearly 30× that of CANCBD and a CBD concentration only 1/40 that of CANCBD. Nonetheless, after self-administration, brain THC concentrations were similar regardless of which extract was delivered, whereas the concentration of CBD was ∼3× greater in rats receiving CANCBD than in those receiving CANTHC. These results demonstrate that rats achieve pharmacologically relevant increases in brain THC with both extracts; the enrichment of THC in the CANCBD group might result from inhibition of THC metabolism by CBD (Jones and Pertwee, 1972). Furthermore, because THC disrupts spatial memory and acquisition of operant tasks in rats, it may increase the error in discrimination (Varvel et al., 2001; Delatte et al., 2002). Finally, CBD counteracts the psychotropic and aversive effects of THC particularly through action in the ventral hippocampus (Hudson et al., 2019). A balance between THC-driven motivation and CBD-protected learning may therefore underlie the discrimination disparity across CANTHC and CANCBD groups.

In a separate experiment, the rats' locomotion and metabolic parameters were measured over 10 d of fixed-ratio self-administration. Only rats that self-administered CANTHC exhibited some features of the classical physiological “tetrad” response: lowered spontaneous activity, antinociception, hypothermia, and catalepsy (Metna‐Laurent et al., 2017). Rats self-administering CANTHC spent more time inactive than those receiving CANCBD but also displayed significantly greater food consumption and energy expenditure. In contrast, locomotor and metabolic signatures were indistinguishable in the CANCBD and vehicle groups. This demonstrates that the extracts have different effects on some internal states (e.g., arousal and appetite). Still, the mechanism that relates drug action to internal state and physiological adaptations that lead to self-administration selectivity or chronic drug seeking is not yet clear.

Self-administration of CANCBD is more resistant to extinguishing while CANTHC elicits stronger reinstatement, raising questions about the underlying circuit adaptations

Freels et al. (2020) trained another cohort of rats to self-administer vapor over 19 d and then continued sessions with both ports set as inactive over 7 d. The CANCBD group, but not the CANTHC group, required significantly more trials than the vehicle group to extinguish nose-poking (defined as a 50% decrease in nose-pokes at the previously active port since the last session with vapor delivery). Transitioning the rats off of the vapor thus raised an apparent inconsistency with the prior results: rats were more resistant to extinguishing the seeking of CANCBD despite their greater motivation to consume CANTHC. This observation is especially curious given that CBD disrupts the association between rewarding effect and the spatial location where rats consume cocaine or opioids (de Carvalho and Takahashi, 2017; Mahmud et al., 2017). The result might be explained by a difference in learning rates due to reward prediction error for dopamine reinforcement (Glimcher, 2011). The light cue or vapor smell may be more salient to the CANTHC group because they experience the reinforcing effect of higher THC concentrations immediately. The CANTHC group may have then experienced a greater unexpected result under extinguishing conditions, eliciting a faster rate of learning to stop nose-poking.

Finally, to test the reinstatement of vapor seeking, Freels et al. (2020) provided an additional session after extinction, in which nose-poking the previously active port triggered the light cue, but no vapor delivery. Only rats previously receiving CANTHC increased their nose-poking relative to the vehicle group. This result indicates a sustained stronger motivation to seek CANTHC and is consistent with the previous results indicating that CANTHC has a greater reinforcing efficacy compared with that of CANCBD. This drive to seek CANTHC could be motivated by reward-seeking, withdrawal avoidance, or a combination of both factors (Fuchs et al., 2019).

The study of cannabis use disorder is now challenged with distinguishing the actions of each cannabis constituent in the development and reinforcement of maladaptive behaviors. Future work should characterize the pharmacokinetics for the method developed by Freels et al. (2020), given that human use of electronic cannabis vaporization demands considerable optimization (Hazekamp et al., 2006). Then, one could determine whether synthetic agonists, purified THC and CBD, or cannabis extracts are sufficient to establish self-administration. The work by Freels et al. (2020) also raises questions about reward encoding and prediction mechanisms. The critical question for addiction studies remains: what, if any, factor could transition an animal from controlled to compulsive cannabis seeking that forgoes well-being (Everitt et al., 2008)? Freels et al. (2020) have provided the behavioral neuroscience field with a method to address these questions with excellent fidelity to the human experience of cannabis use.

Footnotes

Editor's Note: These short reviews of recent J Neurosci articles, written exclusively by students or postdoctoral fellows, summarize the important findings of the paper and provide additional insight and commentary. If the authors of the highlighted article have written a response to the Journal Club, the response can be found by viewing the Journal Club at www.jneurosci.org. For more information on the format, review process, and purpose of Journal Club articles, please see http://jneurosci.org/content/jneurosci-journal-club.

I thank my adviser Dr. Henry A. Lester for guidance in the neuroscience of addiction; and Vinicius S. Ferreira for helpful comments on the manuscript. This work was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse grant DA049140.

The author declares no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Carbuto M, Sewell RA, Williams A, Forselius-Bielen K, Braley G, Elander J, Pittman B, Schnakenberg A, Bhakta S, Perry E, Ranganathan M, D'Souza DC, Yale THC Study Group (2012) The safety of studies with intravenous Δ 9-tetrahydrocannabinol in humans, with case histories. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 219:885–896. 10.1007/s00213-011-2417-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. de Carvalho CR, Takahashi RN (2017) Cannabidiol disrupts the reconsolidation of contextual drug‐associated memories in Wistar rats. Addict Biol 22:742–751. 10.1111/adb.12366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Delatte MS, Winsauer PJ, Moerschbaecher JM (2002) Tolerance to the disruptive effects of Δ9-THC on learning in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 74:129–140. 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00966-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Everitt BJ, Belin D, Economidou D, Pelloux Y, Dalley JW, Robbins TW (2008) Neural mechanisms underlying the vulnerability to develop compulsive drug-seeking habits and addiction. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:3125–3135. 10.1098/rstb.2008.0089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Freels TG, Baxter-Potter LN, Lugo JM, Glodosky NC, Wright HR, Baglot SL, Petrie GN, Yu Z, Clowers BH, Cuttler C, Fuchs RA, Hill MN, McLaughlin RJ (2020) Vaporized cannabis extracts have reinforcing properties and support conditioned drug-seeking behavior in rats. J Neurosci 40:1897–1908. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2416-19.2020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Fuchs RA, Higginbotham JA, Hansen EJ (2019) Animal models of addiction. In: Neural mechanisms of addiction (Torregrossa M, ed), pp 3–22. San Diego: Academic. [Google Scholar]
  7. Gardner EL. (2005) Endocannabinoid signaling system and brain reward: emphasis on dopamine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 81:263–284. 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.01.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Glimcher PW. (2011) Understanding dopamine and reinforcement learning: the dopamine reward prediction error hypothesis. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:15647–15654. 10.1073/pnas.1014269108 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Hazekamp A, Ruhaak R, Zuurman L, van Gerven J, Verpoorte R (2006) Evaluation of a vaporizing device (Volcano®) for the pulmonary administration of tetrahydrocannabinol. J Pharm Sci 95:1308–1317. 10.1002/jps.20574 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Hudson R, Renard J, Norris C, Rushlow WJ, Laviolette SR (2019) Cannabidiol counteracts the psychotropic side-effects of Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in the ventral hippocampus through bidirectional control of ERK1-2 phosphorylation. J Neurosci 39:8762–8777. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0708-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Ibsen MS, Connor M, Glass M (2017) Cannabinoid CB1 and CB2 receptor signaling and bias. Cannabis Cannabinoid Res 2:48–60. 10.1089/can.2016.0037 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Jones G, Pertwee RG (1972) A metabolic interaction in vivo between cannabidiol and Δ1‐tetrahydrocannabinol. Br J Pharmacol 45:375–377. 10.1111/j.1476-5381.1972.tb08092.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Justinova Z, Goldberg SR, Heishman SJ, Tanda G (2005) Self-administration of cannabinoids by experimental animals and human marijuana smokers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 81:285–299. 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.01.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Koob GF, Bloom FE, Kupfer DJ (2012) Animal models of drug addiction. In: Food and addiction: a comprehensive handbook (Brownell KD, Gold MS, eds). Oxford: Oxford UP. [Google Scholar]
  15. MacCallum CA, Russo EB (2018) Practical considerations in medical cannabis administration and dosing. Eur J Intern Med 49:12–19. 10.1016/j.ejim.2018.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Mahmud A, Gallant S, Sedki F, D'Cunha T, Shalev U (2017) Effects of an acute cannabidiol treatment on cocaine self-administration and cue-induced cocaine seeking in male rats. J Psychopharmacol (Oxf) 31:96–104. 10.1177/0269881116667706 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Metna‐Laurent M, Mondésir M, Grel A, Vallée M, Piazza PV (2017) Cannabinoid‐induced tetrad in mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci 80:9–59. 10.1002/cpns.31 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Peters KZ, Oleson EB, Cheer JF (2020) A brain on cannabinoids: the role of dopamine release in reward seeking and addiction. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. Advance online publication. Retrieved January 21, 2020. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a039305 10.1101/cshperspect.a039305 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Sperlágh B, Windisch K, Andó RD, Vizi ES (2009) Neurochemical evidence that stimulation of CB1 cannabinoid receptors on GABAergic nerve terminals activates the dopaminergic reward system by increasing dopamine release in the rat nucleus accumbens. Neurochem Int 54:452–457. 10.1016/j.neuint.2009.01.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Varvel S, Hamm R, Martin B, Lichtman A (2001) Differential effects of Δ9-THC on spatial reference and working memory in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 157:142–150. 10.1007/s002130100780 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES