Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2020 Sep 16;18(9):e3000828. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000828

Diet choice: The two-factor host acceptance system of silkworm larvae

Kana Tsuneto 1, Haruka Endo 1,2,*, Fumika Shii 1, Ken Sasaki 3, Shinji Nagata 2, Ryoichi Sato 1,*
Editor: Anurag A Agrawal4
PMCID: PMC7494105  PMID: 32936797

Abstract

Many herbivorous insects are mono- or oligophagous, having evolved to select a limited range of host plants. They specifically identify host-plant leaves using their keen sense of taste. Plant secondary metabolites and sugars are thought to be key chemical cues that enable insects to identify host plants and evaluate their quality as food. However, the neuronal and behavioral mechanisms of host-plant recognition are poorly understood. Here, we report a two-factor host acceptance system in larvae of the silkworm Bombyx mori, a specialist on several mulberry species. The first step is controlled by a chemosensory organ, the maxillary palp (MP). During palpation at the leaf edge, the MP detects trace amounts of leaf-surface compounds, which enables host-plant recognition without biting. Chemosensory neurons in the MP are tuned with ultrahigh sensitivity (thresholds of attomolar to femtomolar) to chlorogenic acid (CGA), quercetin glycosides, and β-sitosterol (βsito). Only if these 3 compounds are detected does the larva make a test bite, which is evaluated in the second step. Low-sensitivity neurons in another chemosensory organ, the maxillary galea (MG), mainly detect sucrose in the leaf sap exuded by test biting, allowing larvae to accept the leaf and proceed to persistent biting (feeding). The two-factor host acceptance system reported here may commonly underlie stereotyped feeding behavior in many phytophagous insects and determine their feeding habits.


Silkworm larvae are well known for their preference for mulberry leaves; this study shows that they choose their food by using a two-factor host acceptance system, deploying two different organs and sets of chemosensory neurons with differing sensitivities operating sequentially in time.

Introduction

Diet choice is essential for survival in all organisms. Many herbivorous insects have adapted to a limited range of host plants by overcoming plant defenses against insect herbivory, resulting in mono- and oligophagous insects (so-called specialist insects) [1]. In parallel, specialists are able to precisely distinguish their host plants from other nonhost plants in their ecosystem using chemical senses, including gustation and olfaction [2]. In general, gustation is important in determining the acceptance or rejection of a potential food source, whereas olfaction is required when searching for host plants from a distance [3].

Plant-feeding insect larvae exhibit a typical stepwise feeding behavior, comprising foraging, palpation, biting, and swallowing [4,5]. Sensory exploration by palpation of the leaf surface is important for host-plant selection in various plant-feeding insects, such as caterpillars, beetles, and grasshoppers [69]. For example, the meadow grasshopper Chorthippus parallelus sometimes rejects a diet following palpation using peripheral chemosensory organs without biting [6]. Conversely, leaf-surface extracts of the host plant Poa annua induced biting behavior in the migratory locust Locusta migratoria, whereas those of nonhost plants were rejected [10]. Thus, insects use leaf-surface compounds as chemical cues for host-plant selection, but the neuronal basis for this behavior and to what extent the leaf-surface behavior contributes to whole host-plant selection or leaf choice are poorly understood.

Feeding in insect herbivores is controlled by their physiological responses to phytochemicals that determine the acceptance or rejection of a leaf based on the balance of feeding stimulants and deterrents contained in plant leaves [3,11,12]. Chemical ecological studies have identified plant secondary metabolites as feeding stimulants and deterrents for many insect herbivores [2]. For example, with regard to the silkworm Bombyx mori, a specialist on mulberry leaves, Hamamura and his colleagues identified nonvolatile compounds from mulberry leaves as key signals: isoquercitrin (ISQ) and β-sitosterol (βsito) as biting factors and chlorogenic acid (CGA) as a feeding stimulant that increases feeding amounts in a long-term assay [11,13,14,15]. Thus, feeding stimulants and deterrents in the diet choice have been evaluated by their effects on the number of bites and feeding amounts in most cases. However, these observations do not always reflect the instantaneous behavior of insects that accept or reject certain plants [16], and thus, careful investigation of behavior at the leaf surface is required for further understanding of role of plant secondary metabolites in host-plant selection.

In the present study, we assigned chemical host cues to taste organs and feeding behaviors to precisely understand the mechanisms underpinning host acceptance in the silkworm. We found that ultrasensitive chemosensory neurons in the maxillary palp (MP) of silkworm larvae detect a set of key mulberry compounds, including CGA, quercetin glycosides, and βsito, inducing a test bite. Sucrose and myo-inositol in leaf saps stimulate lateral sensilla in the maxillary galea (MG) to induce continuous biting and acceptance of the leaf. We propose a two-factor host acceptance system, controlled by 2 peripheral chemosensory organs and mainly driven by 6 phytochemicals, that underlies oligophagy in silkworms.

Results

Two peripheral chemosensory organs control two-step biting behaviors in the silkworm

To clarify the mechanism of host acceptance by silkworm larvae, we observed larval feeding of a host leaf from white mulberry Morus alba. When a silkworm encounters a leaf, it first palpates the leaf edge using a peripheral chemosensory organ known as the maxilla, intermittently bites the edge several times, and finally engages in continuous biting (2–3 times per second) with its head shaking in the dorsoventral direction along the leaf edge (Fig 1A; S1 Movie). The intermittent biting with palpation and the continuous biting with head-moving are termed test biting and persistent biting [4,5]. We hypothesized that sensing of chemical cues from an M. alba leaf via the maxilla induces test biting because test biting always occurs after palpation with the maxilla. The maxilla consists of the MP and MG (Fig 1B). To assess the roles of the MP and MG in the induction of test biting, we used MP- or MG-ablated larvae. MP-ablated larvae showed palpation, but no test or persistent biting (Fig 1C and 1D; S2 Movie). MG-ablated larvae showed palpation and test biting, stopped biting within 1 minute, and did not progress to persistent biting (Fig 1C and 1D; S3 Movie). When we ablated an olfactory organ antenna (AN), the larvae showed palpation, test biting, and persistent biting similar to intact larvae (Fig 1C and 1D; S4 Movie). Note that AN ablation increased the number of bites during first 30 seconds, although the reasons for this are unclear. These results indicate that the MP and MG are essential for the induction of test and persistent biting.

Fig 1. MP and MG are responsible for test and persistent biting.

Fig 1

(A) Feeding on mulberry leaves by silkworm larvae. (1) Palpation: a silkworm larva first palpates the leaf surface with its maxilla (MP and MG) for 5–30 seconds. The white arrow and arrowhead indicate MP and MG. (2) Test biting: the larva bites the leaf edge several times intermittently during palpation. (3) Persistent biting: the larva nibbles the leaf edge repeatedly (2–3 times per second) with its head moving in the dorsoventral direction along the leaf edge. Magenta arrows indicate the direction of head movement. (B) Mouthparts of a silkworm larva. Upper larval mouthparts including 1 pair of maxilla. Lower higher-magnification view of the maxilla. The maxilla consists of the MG and MP. The MG has 2 gustatory sensilla, the LS and MS. (C) Frequency of biting by intact, MP-ablated, MG-ablated, and AN-ablated larvae of mulberry leaves over a 10-second period. Data are means ± SD (n = 5). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. (D) Representative raster plots of the timing and duration of the biting behavior of larvae in (C). Red and green lines indicate test and persistent bites, respectively. AN, antenna; LS, lateral styloconic sensillum; MG, maxillary galea; MP, maxillary palp; MS, medial styloconic sensillum.

MP recognizes edible leaves by sensing leaf-surface compounds and induces test biting

To assess how such MP- and MG-controlled biting behaviors contribute to host acceptance of the silkworm, we observed feeding behavior towards leaves of various plant species. The silkworm is a specialist for some Morus species, including M. alba. In addition, the leaves of several Cichorioideae plants of the Asteraceae are consumed in relatively small amounts by silkworm larvae [17,18]. Of the larvae, 88.9% ± 5.3% showed test biting of M. alba within 1 minute after reaching the leaf edge, compared with 70.0% ± 10.0% and 63.3% ± 13.3% for 2 Cichorioideae plants, Sonchus oleraceus and Taraxacum officinale (Fig 2A). Of the larvae, 80% ± 11.5%, 53.3% ± 14.5%, and 30% ± 10% proceeded to persistent biting of M. alba, S. oleraceus, and T. officinale, respectively. In contrast, the larvae had a lower probability of test biting (3%–33%) of 12 inedible leaves, which were finally rejected by most of them without persistent biting (Fig 2A and 2B). Thus, the probability of test biting was higher for edible than for inedible leaves, and persistent biting was induced only by edible leaves. These results suggest that silkworm larvae select their diet sequentially at 2 points, before test and persistent biting, and that host-plant recognition is largely completed when they make a first bite.

Fig 2. The MP detects leaf-surface compounds and triggers test biting of edible leaves.

Fig 2

(A and B) Proportion of larvae showing test biting (A) and persistent biting (B) of edible and inedible leaves. (C and D) Proportion of larvae showing test biting of intact mulberry leaves or mulberry leaves wiped with sheets of Kimwipe (Nippon Paper Crecia Co., Tokyo, Japan) paper moistened with water or methanol (C) or filter paper treated with methanol or mulberry leaf-surface extract (D). MP+ and MP− indicate intact and MP-ablated larvae. (A–D) Biting of 10 larvae was observed for 1 minute. Experiments were repeated as independent biological replicates (n = 3–5). Data are means ± SE. The same letters indicate no significant difference (P > 0.05) by one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey post hoc test. (E) Typical spikes from chemosensory sensilla of the MP towards 5 mM NaCl (negative control) and leaf-surface extract (compounds from a leaf/10 mL). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. ANOVA, analysis of variance; MP, maxillary palp.

Insects are likely to sense leaf-surface compounds during palpation [16]. To elucidate whether leaf-surface compounds induce test biting, we wiped M. alba leaves with water or methanol, which markedly decreased the proportion of larvae showing test biting (Fig 2C). Conversely, 76.7% ± 8.8%, 37.5% ± 10.3%, and 36.0% ± 10.8% of the larvae showed test biting of filter paper treated with methanol and leaf-surface extracts of M. alba, S. oleraceus, and T. officinale, respectively (Fig 2D; S1A–S1C Fig). MP ablation diminished test biting towards filter paper treated with an extract of M. alba leaf surface (Fig 2D). Furthermore, tip recording of the sensilla in the MP [17] revealed that MP neurons responded to the leaf-surface extract (Fig 2E). These findings indicate that compounds in edible leaf-surface extracts stimulate the MP and trigger test biting.

Test biting requires a set of host-plant compounds detected by ultrasensitive MP sensory neurons

To identify inducers of test biting, we searched for secondary metabolites in edible leaves of M. alba, T. officinale, and Lactuca indica [18,19] in the plant-metabolite database KNApSAcK [20] because secondary metabolites are thought to be key chemical cues for host-plant recognition [2]. The search yielded CGA and quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside (Q3R). CGA reportedly increases the amount of food intake by the silkworm [15]; Q3R is an analog of ISQ, which reportedly induces biting by the silkworm [14]. In addition, we focused on βsito because it also reportedly induces biting by the silkworm [13]. We first recorded the responses of the MP and MG towards these compounds. Surprisingly, MP responded to the 4 compounds at the attomolar and femtomolar levels (Fig 3A). In contrast, the MG did not respond to these compounds even at higher concentrations (S2B and S2C Fig). Next, we assessed whether the 4 compounds induced test biting. Filter paper, which was treated with each single compound, mixtures of 2 compounds, and a mixture of ISQ, Q3R, and βsito induced test biting by 20%–40% of larvae. In contrast, mixtures of 3 compounds (CGA + ISQ + βsito and CGA + Q3R + βsito) and the mixture of all 4 compounds resulted in a high probability of test biting comparable to the M. alba leaf-surface extract (Fig 2D and Fig 3B) but did not induce persistent biting (S1D Fig). In agreement with the ultrasensitivity of the MP, filter papers treated with extremely dilute mixtures of CGA, ISQ, and βsito still induced test biting to some extent (Fig 3C). Meanwhile, a mixture of D-fructose, sucrose, D-glucose, and myo-inositol did not induce biting (Fig 3B). These results suggest that a trace amount of the set of CGA + ISQ/Q3R + βsito contribute to host recognition and induction of test biting.

Fig 3. A mixture of CGA, ISQ/Q3R, and βsito induces test biting by stimulating the MP.

Fig 3

(A) Typical electrophysiological recordings from the MP in response to CGA, ISQ, Q3R, and βsito. (B and C) Fraction of larvae showing test biting of filter paper treated with CGA, ISQ, Q3R, βsito, and sugars (sucrose, D-fructose, myo-inositol, and D-glucose) (B) and with an extremely dilute mixture of CGA, ISQ, and βsito (C). Filter paper was treated with 100 μL of 1 mM stimulant solution (100 nmol of each compound/5 cm2) (B) or a dilution series of the CGA, ISQ, and βsito mixture (C). Biting by 10 larvae was observed for 1 minute. Experiments were repeated as independent biological replicates (n = 3–5). Data are means ± SE. Statistical analysis was performed using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey post hoc test. The same letters indicate no significant difference (P > 0.05). An asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05; ***P < 0.001; ****P < 0.0001). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. ANOVA, analysis of variance; CGA, chlorogenic acid; ISQ, isoquercitrin; MP, maxillary palp; Q3R, quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside; βsito, β-sitosterol.

Sucrose and myo-inositol induce persistent biting via MG and modulate the amount of food intake

Next, we investigated the role of the MG in inducing persistent biting. The lateral styloconic sensillum (LS) in the MG of the silkworm larvae is involved in recognition of feeding stimulants and has 3 neurons specifically tuned to D-glucose, sucrose, and myo-inositol, respectively, at around the millimolar level [21]. Therefore, we hypothesized that sugars in the leaf sap exuded by test biting stimulate the MG and induce persistent biting. Because feeding initiation is strictly regulated by test biting, we conducted an agar-based food intake assay using starved larvae [22] to simply evaluate persistent biting. In this assay, starved larvae no longer exert specific preference for mulberry leaves and sometimes randomly bite; this biting substitutes for test biting, and consequently, persistent biting occurred in the absence of the inducers of test biting. Alternatively, unlike when feeding on leaves, the MG directly detected high concentrations of compounds at the agar surface during palpation, resulting in induction of persistent biting. Larvae fed an agar diet containing sucrose at >10 mM showed persistent biting (Fig 4A). The amount of food intake seemed to correlate with the duration of persistent biting. Indeed, a sucrose dose-dependent increase in the body weight was observed in intact and MP-ablated larvae (S3B and S3C Fig). The magnitude of the sucrose-induced increase in larval weight was significantly smaller in MG-ablated larvae than in intact larvae (Fig 4A), suggesting an important role of persistent biting via MG in modulating the amount of food intake. Meanwhile, myo-inositol and D-glucose themselves did not induce larval weight increase (Fig 4B; S3D Fig), whereas myo-inositol showed a supplemental effect in the presence of sucrose (Fig 4B) in agreement with a previous study [22]. These results suggest that sucrose and myo-inositol contribute to induction of persistent biting by stimulating MG.

Fig 4. Suc induces persistent biting by stimulating the MG in cooperation with Ino.

Fig 4

(A–C) Effects of Suc (A), Ino (B), and inducers of test biting CGA, ISQ, and βsito (C) on the increase in larval body mass by agar-based food intake assay. Fifth-instar larvae were starved for 3 days beginning at the end of molting. The body mass of the 3 larvae increased after feeding of the agar-based diet (9% cellulose and 1% agar) for 3 h. Magenta bars denote median. Statistical analysis was performed using Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn test (A and B) and Mann–Whitney U-test (C). “ns” indicates no significant difference; an asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ****P < 0.0001). MG+ and MG− indicate intact and MG-ablated larvae. (D) Proposed model of host recognition and acceptance by the silkworm. For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. CGA, chlorogenic acid; Glc, D-glucose; Ino, myo-inositol; ISQ, isoquercitrin; MG, maxillary galea; MP, maxillary palp; Q3r, quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside; Suc, sucrose; βsito, β-sitosterol.

Finally, we assessed whether the inducers of test biting (CGA, ISQ, and βsito) and sugars (sucrose, myo-inositol, and D-glucose) resulted in an increase in larval weight similar to that induced by M. alba leaf extract and intact leaf. A mixture of 10 mM sucrose, 5 mM myo-inositol, and 5 mM D-glucose, similar to the concentrations in M. alba leaves [23], resulted in an increase in larval weight in the presence, but not the absence, of a mixture of sugars, similar to an M. alba leaf and a leaf-extract–containing agar-based diet (Fig 4C). The test biting induced by CGA + ISQ + βsito accelerated the first bite (S4 Fig), which might cause persistent biting and consequently increase the total food intake. Therefore, we concluded that these 6 compounds are major phytochemical drivers of silkworm feeding (Fig 4D).

Discussion

In the present study, we integrated chemical cues, chemosensory organs, and biting behaviors to provide new understanding of the mechanisms by which silkworm larvae accept mulberry leaves. Since the 1970s, entomologists have noticed that insects may identify their host plants at the leaf surface, but the mechanisms underlying this phenomenon remained largely unknown. We report here that ultrasensitive chemosensory neurons in the MP enable silkworm larvae to sense leaf-surface compounds and thus recognize their host plants. CGA, quercetin glycosides, and βsito were isolated as feeding stimulants for silkworm larvae, but it had been generally thought that these compounds could not explain the preference for mulberry leaves by silkworms because they are not specific to mulberry. Now, our findings suggest that the combination of the 3 compounds at the leaf surface explains the preference. Silkworm larvae do not feed on some Moraceae leaves, such as those of fig trees, but will feed on an agar-based diet containing both nonhost Moraceae leaves and mulberry leaves [18], suggesting that the nonhost Moraceae leaves do not contain feeding deterrents, but rather lack feeding stimulants. The composition of the inducers of test biting at the leaf surface can vary, even among the Moraceae. Alternatively, there may be limited plant species that produce the required combination within the habitat of B. madarina, an ancestor of the domesticated silkworm.

Host-plant recognition in larval feeding and adult oviposition seem to employ quite similar systems, driven by multiple host-plant compounds. Some butterflies are reported to require a specific combination of 6–10 host-plant compounds to induce egg laying [2426]. This requirement is very similar to the host-plant acceptance mechanisms that we identified in silkworm larvae feeding. Furthermore, in the swallowtail butterfly Papilio xuthus, a specialist feeder on Rutaceae plants, a specific combination of the 3 neurons detecting multiple oviposition stimulants is responsible for oviposition [27]. The requirement of the combination of 3 compounds to trigger test biting in silkworm larvae suggests that different chemosensory neurons are responsible for CGA, quercetin glycosides, and βsito and that the firing of multiple neurons is required to trigger test biting. Thus, insect herbivores may utilize their own combinations of phytochemicals for host-plant recognition in feeding and oviposition.

Our study clarifies an essential role of the MP in host-plant selection and feeding initiation in the silkworm. It was known that the MP has both gustatory and olfactory chemosensory neurons in lepidopteran insects [28], but its role in gustation had previously been poorly studied. We found that the silkworm MP is tuned to several mulberry compounds, but the specificities of more than 20 gustatory neurons in the MP [17] for chemical tastants such as sugars, amino acids, secondary metabolites, salt, and water remain largely uncharacterized. Future work will identify the neurons and receptor molecules that are essential for host-plant recognition. In addition, the possible role of olfaction by the MP in host-plant selection should be addressed. To our knowledge, the sensitivity of the attomolar and femtomolar ranges in gustation is the highest determined to date in insects and other animals. The leaf-surface cuticles are covered by a waxy layer, and therefore, the amounts of phytochemicals at the leaf surface are expected to be low. It is plausible that this ultrahigh sensitivity of the MP is necessary to detect trace amounts of inducers of test biting at the leaf surface.

The MG has been shown to play a main role in host-plant selection in lepidopteran insects [3,29]. The medial styloconic sensillum (MS) of the silkworm MG is known to respond to feeding deterrents, including coumarin, caffeine, and nicotine, at micromolar to millimolar levels [30,31]. Considering the two-factor host acceptance system, a probable role of the MS in the silkworm is to probe for toxic compounds in leaf sap generated by test biting. Most larvae exhibiting test biting on inedible leaves did not proceed to persistent biting, supporting the putative role of the MS in a final check to exclude inedible leaves by detecting feeding deterrents. Numerous information by previous studies on caterpillar neurophysiological responses of the MG to feeding deterrents [2,3,29] may suggest a similar role of MG in other lepidopteran insects. Note that the MG in some lepidopteran species respond to host-plant secondary metabolites [3,29], whereas silkworm MG did not respond to mulberry secondary metabolites we tested (CGA and quercetin glycosides) (S2B and S2C Fig). In these species, host-plant secondary compounds may stimulate the MG and induce persistent biting in combination with sucrose and myo-inositol. Meanwhile, insect herbivores often reject a leaf without biting. Glendinning and colleagues reported that Manduca sexta larvae rejected a diet treated with an extract from a nonhost plant by sensing it via the MP [32], suggesting the possibility that the MP senses feeding deterrents at the leaf surface with ultrahigh sensitivity. Thus, the two-factor host acceptance system likely comprises sensory responses to deterrents and stimulants by both the MP and MG.

By expanding existing models for caterpillars that suggest that feeding is governed by the balance of feeding stimulants and deterrents that stimulate the MG [3,33,34], the two-factor host acceptance system we proposed here may offer a general explanation of host-plant selection in lepidopteran insects. In monophagous/oligophagous caterpillars (i.e., specialists), host-plant recognition by MP is probably conserved, and the tuning of neurons in the MP to unique combinations of phytochemicals must reflect a history of insect adaptation to their host plants. In polyphagous caterpillars (i.e., generalists), feeding initiation may be less restricted by the MP; thus, rejection of foods may fall under control of responses by the MG to deterrent compounds as shown in previous studies [2,3]. The stepwise feeding behavior, comprising feeding initiation by recognizing specific foods and followed by sustained feeding, is reported in not only phytophagous insects like caterpillars [4,5] and grasshoppers [35] but a wider range of insects. In the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, the yeast feeding program starts with proboscis extension regulated by the labellum, and subsequent sustained feeding is regulated by taste pegs [36]. This suggests that the two-factor system for diet choice, utilizing different chemosensory organs and operating sequentially in time, is largely conserved beyond phytophagous insects. Further comparative studies using other insects will reveal a more concrete picture of the two-factor host acceptance system underpinning their feeding habits.

Materials and methods

Insects

The silkworm (B. mori Kinshu × Showa hybrid) eggs were purchased from Ueda Sanshu Ltd (Nagano, Japan). The silkworms were reared on an artificial diet, Silkmate 2M (Nihon-Nosan Co. Ltd., Kanagawa, Japan) with 16L-8D at 25°C. Larvae were provided with fresh diets every day to synchronize growth.

Biting assay towards leaves and filter paper

Videos of silkworm feeding on leaves and filter paper and filter paper were taken by a D5100 camera (Nikon, Tokyo, Japan) equipped with a telephoto lens AF-S DX Micro Nikkor 85 mm (Nikin, Tokyo, Japan). Larvae were used 24–48 h after molting to the fifth instar. To prevent nonspecific biting due to starvation, all larvae fed on artificial diets for 2 minutes before use. For elucidation of the role of chemosensory organs, the MP, MG, or AN were quickly removed from a larva with fine forceps after the 2-minute feeding. Videos were taken for around 30 to 60 seconds after starting palpation of leaves and filter papers. Fresh host and nonhost leaves were collected within 2 weeks before use around the campuses of Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology (Fuchu and Koganei, Tokyo, Japan) and stored at 4°C. For preparation of wiped leaves and leaf-surface extract, fresh leaves were gently wiped with Kimwipes (Nippon Paper Crecia Co., Tokyo, Japan) wetted with water or methanol. Kimwipes were dried in a drying machine, wetted with water or methanol again, and put into a centrifugal filter column (Merck Millipore, Darmstadt, Germany), and centrifuged to collect leaf-surface extracts. Filter paper (size: 1 cm long, 5 cm wide) was treated with surface extracts from a leaf. A larva moved freely after being put on a leaf, and all larvae showed palpation of all tested plant leaves. For the biting assay using commercial compounds, filter paper was impregnated with 100 μl of the stimulant solution. CGA (Nacalai tesque, Kyoto, Japan), ISQ (Extrasynthese, Lyon, France), Q3R (Extrasynthese), and/or βsito (Abcam, Cambridge, UK) were dissolved in ethanol, followed by vaporization of ethanol. Filter paper was inserted into a slit on foamed polystyrene (S1A Fig). A larva was put on the edge of the slit and videotaped. All larvae palpated on filter paper (S1B Fig). The fraction of larvae showing test or persistent biting was calculated for groups of 10 individuals.

An agar-based food intake assay

To determine whether phytochemicals induce persistent biting, we used an agar-based food intake assay [22] with some modification. Agar-based diets basically contain 9% cellulose and 1% agarose in Elix water (Merck Millipore, Tokyo, Japan). Stimulants except for βsito and cellulose were mixed in advance and added to an agarose solution boiled using the microwave for dissolution. This mixture solution was vortexed well and poured into a 90-mm petri dish (Kanto chemical Co., Inc., Tokyo, Japan). After the agar-based diet solidified, βsito dissolved in EtOH was applied to the diet, followed by EtOH evaporation. For preparation of agar-based diets containing mulberry leaf extract, mulberry leaves were cut into small pieces and dried in a drying machine at 60°C. Dried leaves were crushed into powder. The composition of the mulberry leaf extract diet was 75% mulberry leaf powder (w/v), 1% agarose (w/v), and 24% Elix water (v/v) based on the ratio of dry and wet mass of intact mulberry leaves. Three to five larvae were put on each petri dish, and larval mass before and after 3 h feeding was measured. Increases of larval mass were regarded as amounts of food intake because no feces were observed after 3 h feeding at 25°C. We adopted increases of body mass (mg) as the index of food intake because no significant correlation between amount of food intake and the original body mass was observed (r = 0.11, P > 0.5) by Pearson’s correlation analysis (S3A Fig).

Tip recording

Tip recordings from all sensilla of the MP and the lateral and medial sensilla of the MG were conducted to determine whether neurons respond to CGA, ISQ, Q3R, and βsito. All 8 sensilla in the MP (S2A Fig) were stimulated together. Because LS and MS in MG have a myo-inositol neuron and a deterrent neuron that detects nicotine, respectively [17,30], they were used as positive control for stimulants of LS and MS (S2B and S2C Fig). Fifth-instar larvae starved until use were used for recording. CGA, ISQ, and Q3R were dissolved and serially diluted in water, whereas βsito was dissolved and diluted in ethanol. In dilution, solutions were stirred well for more than 30 minutes. We used 5 mM NaCl as an electrolytic solution. Methyl-β-cyclodextrin (βCD) was used as a complexing agent as described in Brown and colleagues [37] with some modifications. A solution of 2 mg/mL βsito in ethanol was added into 2.5% βCD in 5% NaCl solution so that the final concentration of βsito was 50 μM. The solution was put on a heating block at 80°C until it becomes clear. Tip recording methods were conducted based on Sasaki and colleagues [22]. Stimulants were filled up in glass microelectrodes and a silver wire inside of the electrode was connected to a TastePROBE amplifier (Syntech, Kirchzarten, Germany). The sensilla of MP and the sensilla of MG were capped with the recording electrode, using a micromanipulator to stimulate the chemosensory neurons and to record the response simultaneously. As a reference electrode, a fine silver wire insulated to its tip was inserted into the base of the maxilla. Electrical signals were recorded on a computer via a PowerLab 4/25 and analyzed using CHART 5 (ADInstrument, Bella Vista, Australia).

Statistics

All statistical analyses were performed using Prism ver.8 (GraphPad, La Jolla, CA, USA). We used parametric tests (Fig 2A–2D, Fig 3B and 3C, and S1C Fig) and nonparametric tests (Fig 4A, 4B and 4C and S3C and S3D Fig). See S1 Data for more details (for example, exact sample sizes and P-value).

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Biting assay using filter paper.

(A) Biting assay using filter paper. Filter paper was inserted into a slit on foamed polystyrene. (B) All larvae palpated at the edge of the filter paper irrespective of treatment (left), and larvae showed test biting of treated filter paper. (C) Proportion of larvae (n = 10) showing test biting of filter paper treated with leaf-surface extracts of 2 edible leaves of S. oleraceus and T. officinale over 1 minute. Experiments were repeated as independent biological replicates (n = 3–5). Data are means ± SE. Statistical analysis was performed using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey post hoc test. An asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. (D) Representative raster plots of the timing and duration of biting behavior by larvae using filter paper treated with M. alba leaf-surface extract or a mixture of CGA, ISQ, and βsito. ANOVA, analysis of variance; CGA, chlorogenic acid; ISQ, isoquercitrin; βsito, β-sitosterol.

(TIF)

S2 Fig. Tip recording of sensilla in the MG.

(A) Schematic of sensilla in the MP, which has 8 sensilla (5 putative gustatory and 3 olfactory sensilla). (B and C) Typical electrophysiological recordings from LS (B) and MS (C) in the MG in response to CGA, ISQ, Q3R, and βsito. Ino and Nico were used as positive controls for LS and MS, respectively. CGA, chlorogenic acid; Ino, myo-inositol; ISQ, isoquercitrin; LS, lateral styloconic sensillum; MG, maxillary galea; MP, maxillary palp; MS, medial styloconic sensillum; Nico, nicotine; Q3R, quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside; βsito, β-sitosterol.

(TIF)

S3 Fig. Supplemental agar-based food-intake assay.

(A) Correlation between the original larval body mass and the increase in body mass after feeding a 100 mM Suc-containing agar-based diet for 3 h. Correlation coefficient (r) by Pearson correlation analysis (n = 51). (B) Representative raster plot of the timing and duration of biting when feeding agar containing 100 mM Suc. (C) Suc-dependent increase in larval weight in MP-ablated larvae after 3 h. (D) An effect of Glu on the increase in larval mass weight by agar-based food-intake assay. Magenta bars denote median. Statistical analysis was performed using Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn test. “ns” indicates no significant difference; an asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. Glc, D-glucose; MP, maxillary palp; Suc, sucrose.

(TIF)

S4 Fig. Effect of a mixture of inducers of test biting on increase in proportion of feeding larvae.

Sugars (10 mM sucrose, 5 mM myo-inositol, and 5 mM D-glucose) were added to the basic agar food (9% cellulose and 1% agar). The following inducers of test biting were added: 100 μM CGA, 1 μM ISQ, and 3 μg/cm2 βsito. Data are from biological triplicate experiments (n = 5); error bars indicate SE. For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. CGA, chlorogenic acid; ISQ, isoquercitrin; βsito, β-sitosterol.

(TIF)

S1 Movie. Biting behavior of an intact larva towards a mulberry leaf.

(MP4)

S2 Movie. Biting behavior of an MP-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

MP, maxillary palp.

(MP4)

S3 Movie. Biting behavior of an MG-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

MG, maxillary galea.

(MP4)

S4 Movie. Biting behavior of an AN-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

AN, antenna.

(MP4)

S1 Data. Statistic information used in this study.

(XLSX)

S2 Data. Raw data used in this study.

(XLSX)

Acknowledgments

We thank Toru Shimada and Hiroki Takai (The University of Tokyo) for technical assistance in electrophysiological experiments at the early stage of this study, Naoaki Watanabe (Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology) for assistance in sampling various plant leaves, Yutaka Banno (Kyushu University) for providing fresh M. alba leaves, and David G. Heckel (Max Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology) and Marian R. Goldsmith (University of Rhode Island) for helpful discussion and critical reading of the manuscript.

Abbreviations

AN

antenna

ANOVA

analysis of variance

CGA

chlorogenic acid

ISQ

isoquercitrin

LS

lateral styloconic sensillum

MG

maxillary galea

MP

maxillary palp

MS

medial styloconic sensillum

Q3R

quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside

βCD

methyl-β-cyclodextrin

βsito

β-sitosterol

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

1. Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) KAKENHI Grant Number 17K19261 to RS (https://kaken.nii.ac.jp/ja/grant/KAKENHI-PROJECT-17K19261/) Grant Number 18J00733 to HE (https://kaken.nii.ac.jp/ja/grant/KAKENHI-PROJECT-18J00733/) 2. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Gatehouse JA. Plant resistance towards insect herbivores: a dynamic interaction. New Phytologist. 2002; 156: 145–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Pentzold S, Burse A, Boland W. Contact chemosensation of phytochemicals by insect herbivores. Nat Prod Rep. 2017; 34: 478–483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Chapman RF. Contact chemoreception in feeding by phytophagous insects. Annu Rev Entomol. 2003; 48: 455–484. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Ma WC. Dynamics of feeding responses in Pieris brassicae Linn. as a function of chemosensory input: a behavioral, ultrastructural and electrophysiological study, Meded Landbouwhogesch Wageningen. 1972; 72: 1–162. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Devitt BD, Smith JJB. Action of mouthparts during feeding in the dark-sided cutworm, Euxoa messoria (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). The Canadian Entomologist. 1985; 117: 343–349. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bernays EA, Chapman RF. Food selection by Chorthippus parallelus (Zetterstedt) (Orthoptera: Acrididae) in the field. J Anim Ecol. 1970; 39: 383–394. [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chapman RF and de Boer G. Regulatory Mechanisms in Insect Feeding. London, UK: Chapman & Hall; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Blaney WM, Simmonds MSJ, Ley SV, Katz RB. An electrophysiological and behavioural study of insect antifeedant properties of natural and synthetic drimane-related compounds. Physiol Entomol. 1987; 12: 281–291. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Harrison GD. Host-plant discrimination and evolution of feeding preference in the Colorado potato beetle Leptinotarsa decemlineata. Physiol Entomol. 1987; 12: 407–415. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bernays EA, Blaney WM, Chapman RF, Cook AG. The ability of Locusta migratoria (L.) to perceive plant surface waxes. In: Jermy T, editor. The Host Plant in Relation to Insect Behaviour and Reproduction. New York and London: Plenum Press; 1976. pp. 35–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hamamura Y. Food selection by silkworm larvae. Nature. 1959; 183: 1746–1747. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hirao T, Arai N. On the role of gustatory recognition in host-plant selection by the silkworm, Bombyx mori. L. Jpn J Appl Entomol Zool. 1991; 35: 197–206. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Hamamura Y, Hayashiya K, Naito K. β-sitosterol as one of the Biting Factors. Nature. 1961; 190: 880–881. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hamamura Y, Hayashiya K, Naito K, Matsuura K, Nishida J. Food selection by silkworm larvae. Nature. 1962; 194: 754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Naito K, Hayashiya K. Studies on the Micro Constituent in Mulberry Leaves. Part VI. Isolation of Chlorogenic Acid. J Agric Biol Chem. 1965; 39: 237–238. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Chapman RF, Bernays EA. Insect behavior at the leaf surface and learning as aspects of host plant selection. Experientia. 1989; 45: 215–222. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Asaoka K. Ultrastructure of Maxillary Sensilla in the Silkworm, Bombyx mori: Differences among Strains? J. Insect Biotechnol Sericol. 2003; 72: 117–125. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Uekita K, Ariga Y, Ishida K, Nimura K. Studies on the determinants in the leaves fed exclusively by the silkworm, Bombyx mori L [in Japanese]. J Food Sci., Kyoto Women's University. 1971; 26: 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Ohnuma A, Tazima Y. Feeding experiments of polyphagous silkworm larvae on various plant leaves [in Japanese]. J Seric Sci Jpn. 1997; 66: 431–437. [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Afendi FM, Okada T, Yamazaki M, Hirai-Morita A, Nakamura Y, Nakamura K, et al. KNApSAcK family databases: integrated metabolite-plant species databases for multifaceted plant research. Plant Cell Physiol. 2011; 53: e1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ishikawa S. Responses of maxillary chemoreceptors in the larva of the silkworm, Bombyx mori, to stimulation by carbohydrates. J Cell Physiol. 1963; 61: 99–107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sasaki K, Ooki Y, Endo Y, Asaoka K. Effects of dietary inositol with sucrose stimulation on chewing and swallowing motor patterns in larvae of the silkworm Bombyx mori. Physiol Entomol. 2013; 38: 326–336. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Kikuta S, Endo H, Tomita N, Takada T, Morita C, Asaoka K, et al. Characterization of a ligand-gated cation channel based on an inositol receptor in the silkworm, Bombyx mori. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2016; 74: 12–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Honda K. Identification of host-plant chemicals stimulating oviposition by swallowtail butterfly, Papilio protenor. J Chem Ecol. 1990; 16: 325–337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Ohsugi T, Nishida R, Fukami H. Multi-component system of oviposition stimulants for a Rutaceae-feeding swallowtail butterfly, Papilio xuthus (Lepidoptera: Papilionidae). Appl Entmol Zool. 1991; 26: 29–40. [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Murakami T. Studies on the relationships between host-plant acceptability and plant constituents in host selection by a swallowtail butterfly, Papilio polytes. J Grad Sch Biosp Sci. 2003; 42: 58–59. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Ryuda M, Calas-List D, Yamada A, Marion-Poll F, Yoshikawa H, Tanimura T, K. et al. Gustatory sensing mechanism coding for multiple oviposition stimulants in the swallowtail butterfly, Papilio xuthus. J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 914–924. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schoonhoven LM, Dethier VG. Sensory aspects of host plant discrimination by lepidopterous larvae. Archives Neerlandaises de Zoologie. 2002; 16: 497–530. [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Schoonhoven LM, van Loon JJA. An inventory of taste in caterpillars: each species its own key. Acta Zoologica Academiae Scientiarum Hungaricae 48 (Suppl. 1). 2002; 215–263. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ishikawa S. Electrical response and function of a bitter substance receptor associated with the maxillary sensilla of the silkworm, Bombyx mori L. J Cell Physiol. 1966; 67: 1–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Asaoka K. Deficiency of gustatory sensitivity to some deterrent compounds in “polyphagous” mutant strains of the silkworm, Bombyx mori. J Comp Physiol A. 2000; 186: 1011–1018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Glendinning JI, Valcic S, Timmermann BN. Maxillary palps can mediate taste rejection of plant allelochemicals by caterpillars. J Comp Physiol A. 1998; 183: 35–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dethier VG. Electrophysiological studies of gustation in lepidopterous larvae II. Taste spectra in relation to food-plant discrimination. Z Vergl Physiol. 1973; 82: 103–134. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Simmonds MSJ, Blaney WM. Gustatory codes in lepidopterous larvae. Symp Biol Hung. 1990; 39: 17–27. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Blaney WM, Chapman RF. The functions of the maxillary palps of Acrididae (Orthoptera). Entomol Exp Appl. 1970; 13: 363–376. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Steck K, Walker SJ, Itskov PM, Baltazar C, Moreira JM, Ribeiro C. Internal amino acid state modulates yeast taste neurons to support protein homeostasis in Drosophila. eLife. 2018; 7: e31625. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brown AJ, Sun L, Feramisco JD, Brawn MS, Goldstein JL. Cholesterol addition to ER membranes alters conformation of SCAP, the SREBP escort protein that regulates cholesterol metabolism. Mol Cell. 2002; 10: 237–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lauren A Richardson

23 Jan 2020

Dear Dr Endo,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "Diet choice: The two-factor authentication system of silkworm larvae" for consideration as a Research Article by PLOS Biology.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff as well as by an academic editor with relevant expertise and I am writing to let you know that we would like to send your submission out for external peer review. Please note, however, that the outcome of our discussion of your manuscript is that we have some reservations as to the overall strength of novel biological insight offered by your data. We will need to be persuaded by the reviewers that the paper has the potential after revision to offer the significant advance that we require for publication.

Before we can send your manuscript to reviewers, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire.

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Jan 25 2020 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks in preparation for peer review. Once your manuscript has passed all checks it will be sent out for review.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Lauren A Richardson, Ph.D

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Decision Letter 1

Ines Alvarez-Garcia

8 Mar 2020

Dear Dr Endo,

Thank you very much for submitting your manuscript "Diet choice: The two-factor authentication system of silkworm larvae" for consideration as a Research Article at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by three independent reviewers.

As you will see, the reviewers find the conclusions interesting and valuable for the field, but they also raise several issues that need to be addressed to strengthen the results and conclusions. After discussing the reviews with the Academic Editor, we feel that the inclusion of the additional experiments suggested by the reviewers could improve the manuscript to a sufficient level for us to consider it for publication.

In light of the reviews (attached below), we will not be able to accept the current version of the manuscript, but we would welcome re-submission of a much-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments. We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent for further evaluation by the reviewers.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 2 months.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point by point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Ines

--

Ines Alvarez-Garcia, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Carlyle House, Carlyle Road

Cambridge, CB4 3DN

+44 1223–442810

---------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewers’ comments

Rev. 1:

This manuscript reports an interesting study of neurophysiological and behavioral mechanisms of host-plant acceptance by larval silkworms. As far as I know, the identification of the relative roles of the maxillary palps and galeae in the process of host acceptance represents a new finding. Previous work had mostly focused on particular subsets of the taste system, such as the medial or lateral sensilla of the galeae, or behavioral mechanisms by themselves. And limited work had been done on taste receptors of the maxillary palps. In the present manuscript, the coupling of the electrophysiological work on the taste system encompassing the palps and galeae with behavioral assays was especially refreshing and insightful. The authors conclude that the silkworm accepts a host plant via a "two-factor authentication system" in that stimulation of the taste cells of the maxillary palps first cause biting of the plant, and, second, stimulation of the taste cells of the galeae are responsible for sustained biting (i.e. feeding). This finding has the potential to explain host acceptance behavior by the larvae of other species of Lepidoptera and perhaps other insect orders.

Despite my enthusiasm, I have several general criticisms that I introduce here and pursue in greater detail in my specific comments. First, the interpretation of the neurophysiological mechanism of the first step of host acceptance is missing the potential role of olfaction during palpation. The design of the experiments could not determine this possibility in some cases because olfactory responses were not measured. In one other experiment in which the antennae were ablated, there is evidence that this treatment increases biting (Fig. 1), which is consistent with the hypothesis that olfactory stimuli play a role in the first step of host acceptance by deterring biting. Second, there are some significant gaps in scholarship, particularly from the late 1990's through the early 2000's. Many papers were published elucidating gustatory neurophysiological mechanisms of host-plant acceptance by caterpillars of several species during this time. These uncited papers are important in placing the findings of this manuscript in context. For example, Chapman 2003 (see references below) proposed a generalized gustatory neurophysiological mechanism of host-plant acceptance by phytophagous insects (especially caterpillars). The present manuscript would greatly benefit from placing its content in that context. The authors should discuss whether their findings agree with and extend the Chapman proposal in new ways or whether they argue against the Chapman proposal? I will admit that this field is not my main field, so I don't know much of the literature since the Chapman 2003 review. But I see that the review has been cited 192 times as of today, so there may be much more recent work to cite here.

Specific comments

Abstract, line 15. Replace "nutritional value" with "quality as food" because the food quality of a plant represents both nutritional value and effects of plant secondary metabolites. On the same line, replace "by which insects select host plants through sensing a variety of gustatory inputs" with "of host-plant selection" to avoid lengthy redundancy.

Abstract, line 17. I strongly suggest replacing "authentication" with "host recognition" system. The latter is a conventional phrase that many researchers would understand. Moreover, the authors use "host recognition" elsewhere in the manuscript. There is no need to introduce a new term here. This change should be made to the title and all other parts of the manuscript.

Abstract, lines 22-24. This description of the electrophysiological role of the maxillary galea is lacking much understanding about this part of the taste system, as described in various other studies and summarized in Chapman 2003. Sucrose is a critical stimulant, but secondary metabolites are key as well, with either stimulating or deterrent effects on biting and feeding.

Introduction, line 30. Add "host" before "plants" for clarity, and remove "and carving their own niche" because it does not add any real information.

Introduction, lines 35-37. An important reference that is missing here is a book on this topic by Chapman and de Boer from 1995 (see references).

Introduction, lines 41 and 42. Add a hyphen, so it reads "host-plant selection." Elsewhere, this version of the phrase is used.

Introduction, lines 41-42. I would like to see a bit more of a summary of what's known and what isn't here. It's not entirely fair to say "the neuronal basis" of host-plant selection "and its roles at the leaf surface in whole host-plant selection or leaf choice are poorly understood." While there is certainly more understanding needed, this characterization of the state of knowledge is rather dismissive of what is known (some of which is described in the following paragraph).

Introduction, line 45. Here is where the Chapman 2003 review should be cited. This paragraph should be expanded with some of the information in that paper and related ones.

Introduction, line 49. There is a grammatical issue with "number of feces." I'm not sure what the authors are trying to say here, so I can't really offer a correction.

Introduction, lines 50-51. The end of this paragraph places this study in a very narrow context, as if everyone in the field was mostly interested in understanding host-plant selection by Bombx mori. In fact, B. mori is one of several model species used in this field, and the highly relevant work on these other species can help place the B. mori work in a broader context.

Introduction, lines 57-58. Replace "authentication" with "host recognition" and replace "that underlies oligophagy" with "responsible for the initiation of feeding" for clarity.

Results, line 62. Replace "behavior towards" with "of" for simplicity and clarity.

Results, line 78. Replace "whether" with "how" and replace "contributes" with "contribute" for grammatical improvement.

Results, line 81. The introduction of Lactuca indica here is confusing because this plant is not tested in the study reported here. I wondered if this name was a mistake because it seems like the authors might be referring to Sonchus oleraceus instead. Also in line 81, replace "consumed a relatively small amount" with "consumed in relatively small amounts."

Results, lines 101-102. Again, Lactuca indica is mentioned here, but not elsewhere.

Results, line 111. Replace "were" with "was."

Results, lines 118-119. Here is where I think the possible role of olfactory inputs should be mentioned.

Results, lines 123-124. Here is where I think the broader range of stimulants should be considered for taste cells of the lateral sensilla. It might be the case that LS taste cells only respond to these sugars, but that is not the case for other caterpillar species.

Results, line 135. The "important role of the MG in modulating the amount of food intake" is already known from work on other species. If this information were added to the Introduction section, the statement here could say that this finding confirms "the important role…" rather than suggesting that this is a new finding.

Results, line 137. Replace "in consistent" with "in agreement."

Results, line 140. Remove "that" after "whether."

Discussion, lines 150-151. I suggest changing "comprehensively upgrade" to something like "provide new."

Discussion, line 153. Here, as in the Introduction, the state of knowledge of this field is mischaracterized. Adding the new scholarship should help the authors re-characterize the previous state of knowledge, so their contribution can be more accurately described.

Discussion, lines 157-161. Here is another place where I think it is important to consider the possible role of olfactory inputs in modulating the biting response. Note that the agar-based diet is not likely to have the olfactory stimuli that leaves would have.

Discussion, lines 164-167. I don't find this argument very convincing. In natural forests, canopy branches and leaves of multiple tree species often intersect, and individual caterpillars sometimes fall or walk off their host plant. Even dietary specialist caterpillars do not necessarily stay on the same plant for their entire larval lives. Therefore, it is likely to be important for dietary specialists of trees to have the ability to recognize their specific hosts. I agree that such specific signals do not necessarily have to be single compounds that are unique to the host, even though there are cases in which this is true (Bernays et al. 2003, see references). As argued at the end of the following paragraph, unique combinations of compounds can give specific signals, as seen in pheromone-based species recognition systems of insects.

Discussion, lines 169-178. This paragraph is missing much recent scholarship (see references below) on caterpillar neurophysiological mechanisms of feeding.

Discussion, lines 183-184. Replace "taste modalities such as sweet, bitter…" with "chemical tastants wuch as sugars, amino acids, secondary metabolites…" The former are subjective sensations, whereas my suggestion gives objective information.

Discussion, line 191. Replace "The MG has been believed to play a main role in…" with "The MG has been shown to play a main role in…" This would be a good place to cite Chapman 2003.

Discussion, line 193. Note that the MG in at least one species of caterpillar is known to respond to a stimulating secondary metabolite (pyrrolizidine alkaloids) at 10-12 M. Also replace "authentication" with "host recognition."

Discussion, line 198-199. It's also possible that olfaction plays a role in deterrent inputs provided by the MP, which contains gustatory and olfactory sensilla.

Discussion, lines 199-200. Replace "authentication" with "host recognition" and replace "negative selection" and "positive selection" with terms that are not conventionally understood to have different meanings in biology (evolutionary biology in this case). In this context, I suggest stimulating and deterrent effects on feeding.

Discussion, lines 202-206. This final paragraph reads as if the authors are struggling to place their work in a larger context. I think they will find it easier to do this when they re-frame the Introduction with additional scholarship (see references). The revised Introduction will help set up their study and the final points in the Discussion section can more clearly specify the authors' contribution in addition to specific gaps in the knowledge that future research can address.

Replace "authentication" in the first sentence. Later, the statement "oligophagy and polyphagy are likely consequences of the strict and loose restriction, respectively, of feeding initiation by the MP" confuses proximate and ultimate mechanisms in biology. It is almost certainly the case that the characteristics of the MP and their proximate effects on feeding behavior are a consequence of natural selection for narrow or broad diets in herbivorous insects. I suggest either rewriting this phrase or omitting it. Finally, the concluding line in the paragraph is not truly informative or helpful. At worst, it misleads the reader to believe that "mechanisms underpinning host-plant selection" are not really understood at all. In fact, there are entire books written about this topic (e.g., Bernays and Chapman, 1994, Host-plant selection by phytophagous insects).

Methods, line 215. Change to "Videos of silkworm feeding on leaves and filter paper…" The videos are great, by the way.

Methods, line 218. Add "on" after "fed."

Methods, line 219. Add "the" before "maxillary" and make palp, galea, and antenna plural in this sentence. Also, replace "was" with "were."

Methods, line 221. Replace "towards" with "of."

Methods, line 223. Replace "in" with "at."

Methods, line 225. Replace "wet" with "wetted."

Methods, line 228. The phrase "all larvae started finally palpation towards all kinds of plant leaves" is not grammatically correct or clear. I don't know exactly what the authors are trying to say, so I don't have any correction to offer. On this line at the end, add "the" after "For."

Methods, line 229. Add "of the" before "stimulant."

Methods, line 234. Replace "investigated" with "calculated."

Methods, line 236. Add "an" before "agar-based."

Methods, line 241. Add "the" before "agar-based."

Methods, line 243. Provide the temperature at which the leaves were dried in the oven.

Methods, line 245. Add "the" before "ratio" and replace "weight" with "mass" here and throughout this paragraph.

Methods, line 252. Add "the" before "lateral" and change "sensillum" to "sensilla."

Methods, line 256. Starving the larvae here and in the food intake assay can affect the results of these experiments. In addition to generalized effects of food deprivation, there can be specific effects on taste and feeding responses that reflect the insect's physiological state of nutrient deprivation. For example, dietary deficiencies in carbohydrates tend to increase the taste and feeding responses of caterpillars to carbohydrates (e.g., Bernays et al. 2004a in references). I doubt this issue has large effects on the results reported here, but I want the authors to be aware of this phenomenon in case they are not already.

Methods, line 257. The part of the line after "whereas" is incomplete.

Methods, line 261. Replace "being" with "it became."

Figure 1 legend, line 379. Change "persistent bites." to "persistent bites, respectively."

Figure 4. Replace "weight" in the y-axis labels of panels A, B, C with "mass." Same for the axis labels in Fig. S3.

Figure 4 legend, line 409. Replace "weight" with "mass." Same for the legend of Fig. S3.

References

Bernays and Chapman. 1994. Host-plant selection by phytophagous insects. Chapman & Hall.

Chapman and de Boer, editors. 1995. Regulatory mechanisms in insect feeding. Chapman & Hall.

Bernays et al. 1998. Plant acids modulate chemosensory responses in Manduca sexta larvae. Physiological Entomology 23:193-201.

Bernays and Chapman. 2001. Taste cell responses in the polyphagous arctiid Grammia geneura: towards a general pattern for caterpillars. Journal of Insect Physiology 47:1029-1043.

Bernays et al. 2002. A highly sensitive taste cell for pyrrolizidine alkaloids in the lateral galeal sensillum of a polyphagous caterpillar, Estigmene acrea. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 188:715-723.

Bernays et al. 2002. A taste receptor neurone dedicated to the perception of pyrrolizidine alkaloids in the medial galeal sensillum of two polyphagous caterpillars. Physiological Entomology 27:312-321.

Bernays et al. 2003. Taste receptors for pyrrolizidine alkaloids in a monophagous caterpillar. Journal of Chemical Ecology 29:1709-1722.

Chapman. 2003. Contact chemoreception in feeding by phytophagous insects. Annual Review of Entomology 48:455-484.

Bernays et al. 2004a. Changes in taste receptor cell sensitivity in a polyphagous caterpillar reflect carbohydrate but not protein imbalance. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 190:39-48.

Bernays et al. 2004b. Gustatory responsiveness to pyrrolizidine alkaloids in the Senecio specialist, Tyria jacobaeae (Lepidoptera, Arctiidae). Physiological Entomology 29:67-72.

Rev. 2:

In this manuscript, the authors investigated the diet choice behavior of the silkworm, a famous specialist which feeds on mulberry and several close species, with different bioassay methods. They explained how mulberry preference of the silkworm was related to the appropriate combination of the three compounds in mulberry leaves. They further proposed that a two-factor authentication system, controlled by two peripheral gustatory organs of maxillary palp (MP) and maxillary galea (MG), and driven by six phytochemicals, that underlies oligophagy in silkworms. This work is interesting and provides valuable information in diet choice of phytophagous insect species. However, the conclusion is too rough to be convincing.

Assuming that combination of three compounds are responsible for mulberry preference, how to explain that silkworm larvae also feed on artificial diet (should be different compound combination even this diet contained mulberry leaves)? It was reported that polyphagous silkworm mutant strains feed on non-mulberry diets (Iizuka et al., 2012). A recent study also showed that a gustatory receptor gene determines the silkworm feeding preference (Zhang et al., 2019). In addition, possibly there are other phytochemicals in mulberry leave will also induce feeding behavior including palpation and biting. The reviewer strongly suggests that using a polyphagous mutant strain, such as Sawa-J, to make clear comparison.

The tip recording results showed in Figure 3A and Figure S2B and C were very confusing. The concentration of chlorogenic acid (CGA), isoquercitrin (ISQ), quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside (Q3R), and β-sitosterol used in MP and MG testing were quite different. In addition, stimulus intensity is usually a positive function between stimulus concentration and the number of spikes elicited in a peripheral neuron. Thus, the authors should add the experiment of tip recording (both MP and MG) with gradient concentration. Otherwise, the present results could not support the conclusion that MP responded to the four compounds and MG did not respond to these compounds.

Rev. 3: Heiko Vogel - note that this reviewer has waived anonymity

This is a very nice manuscript by Tsuneto and colleagues, who have studied the perception system by which silkworm larvae decide to continuously feed (or reject) specific host plant species/plant diets.

The focus of the manuscript is on a proposed "two-factor" authentication system which larvae utilize to first perform leaf palpation, using the maxillary palp, and detecting low levels of leaf surface compounds. This first step is followed by test bites of the larvae, and includes subsequent detection of leaf sap compounds by chemosensory neurons localized on the maxillary galea and finally the induction of persistent feeding - in case the larvae encounter the correct host plant.

The authors start by illustrating what is known from the literature on decision making related to herbivorous insect feeding. Prior work from the 1960s and 1970s and more recent papers from several authors have indicated leaf surface compounds as important cues for herbivorous insects to accept host plants. In addition, numerous compounds (also mostly present on leaf surfaces) have been identified which induce oviposition of female insects. However, the major factors, insect physiology and molecular players in host plant acceptance and more generally the "turning points" in decision making of herbivorous larvae when feeding on plants were not well understood.

I found it especially interesting to see that the larval "test biting" clearly shows a frequency gradient on leaves of different host plants. In contrast to this, the persistent biting falls into two clearly separated groups: one group with the acceptable host plants show a gradient of larvae performing persistent biting and the other group (inedible leaves) display essentially zero persistent biting of larvae.

To summarize, this work does a nice job of combining multiple types of experimental approaches/assays. It is a nice finding that shows how a specialized (oligophagous) herbivorous insect can - by going through a series of "decision points" - ultimately discriminate host and non-host plants and thus accept or reject a specific plant diet. The study is well conceived and connects well with previous results, and the methodology employed in the different aspects is state of the art. The results are properly documented, and their interpretation in the discussion is quite reasonable.

I have just minor suggestions for improvement, but see no major flaws that can't be easily addressed with some additions/modifications.

Minor comments:

In all of the figures it would be helpful to provide explanations for all of the abbreviations used. Otherwise the interpretation of at least parts of the figures is severely hampered.

In the discussion the authors compare their findings on compound perception, neuronal perception and decision making in larvae to adult oviposition on plant leaves. Although superficially touched upon, here or in other parts of the discussion the authors could add a few sentences on what their findings might mean in general for monophagous/oligophagous herbivores (i.e. specialist) versus highly polyphagous (i.e. generalist) ones. Do polyphagous species also require a "two-factor authentication system" - and if so what would be the main discriminatory chemical compounds. Or are polyphagous species instead rather relying on deterrents for decision making and host plant acceptance. Even though this would be rather speculative, it would provide a better approach at the more general question of what defines a oligophagous versus a highly polyphagous herbivore.

Decision Letter 2

Ines Alvarez-Garcia

26 Jun 2020

Dear Dr Endo,

Thank you for submitting your revised Research Article entitled "Diet choice: The two-factor host acceptance system of silkworm larvae" for publication in PLOS Biology. I have now obtained advice from two of the original reviewers and have discussed their comments with the Academic Editor.

Based on the reviews, we will probably accept this manuscript for publication, assuming that you will modify the manuscript to clarify some points in the text raised by Reviewer 1 (see file attached to this email). Please also make sure to address the data and other policy-related requests noted at the end of this email.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks. Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. In addition to the remaining revisions and before we will be able to formally accept your manuscript and consider it "in press", we also need to ensure that your article conforms to our guidelines. A member of our team will be in touch shortly with a set of requests. As we can't proceed until these requirements are met, your swift response will help prevent delays to publication.

*Copyediting*

Upon acceptance of your article, your final files will be copyedited and typeset into the final PDF. While you will have an opportunity to review these files as proofs, PLOS will only permit corrections to spelling or significant scientific errors. Therefore, please take this final revision time to assess and make any remaining major changes to your manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Early Version*

Please note that an uncorrected proof of your manuscript will be published online ahead of the final version, unless you opted out when submitting your manuscript. If, for any reason, you do not want an earlier version of your manuscript published online, uncheck the box. Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

*Submitting Your Revision*

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include a cover letter, a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable), and a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Ines

--

Ines Alvarez-Garcia, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

ialvarez-garcia@plos.org

Carlyle House, Carlyle Road

Cambridge, CB4 3DN

+44 1223–442810

------------------------------------------------------------------------

DATA POLICY: PLEASE READ

Many thanks for sending us the raw data underlying all the graphs shown in the figures. I have a couple of requests that remain to be addressed:

- Please delete the tab labelled Fig. 3D from the Data S2 Raw Data file – there is no D section in this figure and the data seems to be the same than Fig. 3C.

- Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on WHERE THE UNDERLYING DATA CAN BE FOUND.

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewers' comments

Rev. 1:

This manuscript is much improved, thanks to the careful attention of the authors to the comments by the reviewers. My minor comments were addressed to my satisfaction in this revision. My major comments were mostly addressed to my satisfaction, even if I disagree with one or two responses from the authors.

For example, I am not convinced by the argument that "silkworm larvae have no reasons to deter biting of mulberry leaves" as rationale for discounting the possibility that olfactory inputs might deter biting. From an evolutionary ecology perspective, I can think of at least one reason that a caterpillar might use olfactory cues from its host plant to decide whether to feed or move elsewhere to feed. I preface my speculation with the important point that the decision to feed by a dietary specialist herbivore is not merely an automatic response to the correct host plant species (as the authors suggest in this manuscript). Insect herbivores use chemical cues to perceive phenotypic variation among individual host plants and parts of host plants, and they can respond adaptively to such variation (e.g., Knolhoff and Heckel 2014, Annual Review of Entomology 59:263). Even dietary specialist herbivores do not respond to every plant of the correct host species in the same way. For example, they might discriminate among plants with high or low resistance traits, some of which are inducible. Regarding olfactory cues, plant volatiles induced by herbivores contain information that predators and parasitoids use to locate their herbivorous prey and hosts (e.g., Heil 2014, New Phytologist 204:297), and herbivores can also use these volatile compounds emitted by plants to accept or reject host plants (e.g., Kessler and Baldwin 2001, Science 291:2141; De Moraes et al. 2001, Nature 410:577). Granted, demonstrations of this phenomenon are limited to adult moths and oviposition behavior. However, caterpillars have been shown to respond behaviorally to the induced chemistry of their host plants over very short time and spatial scales (e.g., Perkins et al. 2013, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 280:20122646). Even though Bombyx mori is a domesticated species, it might retain some ability to use olfactory cues from its host plant in ways that would have been adaptive for its wild ancestors.

I understand that my hypothesis for this unexplained detail in Fig. 1C is highly speculative, thus I am not asking for the authors discuss it. However, I want them to consider it for future work on the functional role of olfaction in caterpillar feeding behavior.

The only revisions I would insist on at this point are some minor things to correct grammatical mistakes in sections that were re-written or added. My corrections and edits for these points are given as sticky notes on the pdf version of the manuscript attached with my review.

Rev. 2:

Authors made significant improvement in the revised manuscript and may be accepted now.

Attachment

Submitted filename: PBIOLOGY-D-20-00139_R2_reviewer1 notes.pdf

Decision Letter 3

Ines Alvarez-Garcia

17 Aug 2020

Dear Dr Endo,

On behalf of my colleagues and the Academic Editor, Anurag A Agrawal, I am pleased to inform you that we will be delighted to publish your Research Article in PLOS Biology.

The files will now enter our production system. You will receive a copyedited version of the manuscript, along with your figures for a final review. You will be given two business days to review and approve the copyedit. Then, within a week, you will receive a PDF proof of your typeset article. You will have two days to review the PDF and make any final corrections. If there is a chance that you'll be unavailable during the copy editing/proof review period, please provide us with contact details of one of the other authors whom you nominate to handle these stages on your behalf. This will ensure that any requested corrections reach the production department in time for publication.

Early Version

The version of your manuscript submitted at the copyedit stage will be posted online ahead of the final proof version, unless you have already opted out of the process. The date of the early version will be your article's publication date. The final article will be published to the same URL, and all versions of the paper will be accessible to readers.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have not yet opted out of the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may do so on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for submitting your manuscript to PLOS Biology and for your support of Open Access publishing. Please do not hesitate to contact me if I can provide any assistance during the production process.

Kind regards,

Alice Musson

Publishing Editor,

PLOS Biology

on behalf of

Ines Alvarez-Garcia,

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Fig. Biting assay using filter paper.

    (A) Biting assay using filter paper. Filter paper was inserted into a slit on foamed polystyrene. (B) All larvae palpated at the edge of the filter paper irrespective of treatment (left), and larvae showed test biting of treated filter paper. (C) Proportion of larvae (n = 10) showing test biting of filter paper treated with leaf-surface extracts of 2 edible leaves of S. oleraceus and T. officinale over 1 minute. Experiments were repeated as independent biological replicates (n = 3–5). Data are means ± SE. Statistical analysis was performed using one-way ANOVA followed by Tukey post hoc test. An asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. (D) Representative raster plots of the timing and duration of biting behavior by larvae using filter paper treated with M. alba leaf-surface extract or a mixture of CGA, ISQ, and βsito. ANOVA, analysis of variance; CGA, chlorogenic acid; ISQ, isoquercitrin; βsito, β-sitosterol.

    (TIF)

    S2 Fig. Tip recording of sensilla in the MG.

    (A) Schematic of sensilla in the MP, which has 8 sensilla (5 putative gustatory and 3 olfactory sensilla). (B and C) Typical electrophysiological recordings from LS (B) and MS (C) in the MG in response to CGA, ISQ, Q3R, and βsito. Ino and Nico were used as positive controls for LS and MS, respectively. CGA, chlorogenic acid; Ino, myo-inositol; ISQ, isoquercitrin; LS, lateral styloconic sensillum; MG, maxillary galea; MP, maxillary palp; MS, medial styloconic sensillum; Nico, nicotine; Q3R, quercetin-3-O-rhamnoside; βsito, β-sitosterol.

    (TIF)

    S3 Fig. Supplemental agar-based food-intake assay.

    (A) Correlation between the original larval body mass and the increase in body mass after feeding a 100 mM Suc-containing agar-based diet for 3 h. Correlation coefficient (r) by Pearson correlation analysis (n = 51). (B) Representative raster plot of the timing and duration of biting when feeding agar containing 100 mM Suc. (C) Suc-dependent increase in larval weight in MP-ablated larvae after 3 h. (D) An effect of Glu on the increase in larval mass weight by agar-based food-intake assay. Magenta bars denote median. Statistical analysis was performed using Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn test. “ns” indicates no significant difference; an asterisk indicates a significant difference (*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01). For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. Glc, D-glucose; MP, maxillary palp; Suc, sucrose.

    (TIF)

    S4 Fig. Effect of a mixture of inducers of test biting on increase in proportion of feeding larvae.

    Sugars (10 mM sucrose, 5 mM myo-inositol, and 5 mM D-glucose) were added to the basic agar food (9% cellulose and 1% agar). The following inducers of test biting were added: 100 μM CGA, 1 μM ISQ, and 3 μg/cm2 βsito. Data are from biological triplicate experiments (n = 5); error bars indicate SE. For numerical raw data, please see S2 Data. CGA, chlorogenic acid; ISQ, isoquercitrin; βsito, β-sitosterol.

    (TIF)

    S1 Movie. Biting behavior of an intact larva towards a mulberry leaf.

    (MP4)

    S2 Movie. Biting behavior of an MP-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

    MP, maxillary palp.

    (MP4)

    S3 Movie. Biting behavior of an MG-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

    MG, maxillary galea.

    (MP4)

    S4 Movie. Biting behavior of an AN-ablated larva towards a mulberry leaf.

    AN, antenna.

    (MP4)

    S1 Data. Statistic information used in this study.

    (XLSX)

    S2 Data. Raw data used in this study.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 200528 response to reviewers comment PBIO.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: PBIOLOGY-D-20-00139_R2_reviewer1 notes.pdf

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: 200629 response to reviewers comment.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLoS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES