Skip to main content
The Journal of Neuroscience logoLink to The Journal of Neuroscience
. 2020 Nov 18;40(47):9066–9077. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0782-20.2020

Regulation of Olfactory Associative Memory by the Circadian Clock Output Signal Pigment-Dispersing Factor (PDF)

Johanna G Flyer-Adams 1, Emmanuel J Rivera-Rodriguez 1, Junwei Yu 1, Jacob D Mardovin 1, Martha L Reed 1, Leslie C Griffith 1,
PMCID: PMC7673005  PMID: 33106351

Abstract

Dissociation between the output of the circadian clock and external environmental cues is a major cause of human cognitive dysfunction. While the effects of ablation of the molecular clock on memory have been studied in many systems, little has been done to test the role of specific clock circuit output signals. To address this gap, we examined the effects of mutations of Pigment-dispersing factor (Pdf) and its receptor, Pdfr, on associative memory in male and female Drosophila. Loss of PDF signaling significantly decreases the ability to form associative memory. Appetitive short-term memory (STM), which in wild-type (WT) is time-of-day (TOD) independent, is decreased across the day by mutation of Pdf or Pdfr, but more substantially in the morning than in the evening. This defect is because of PDFR expression in adult neurons outside the core clock circuit and the mushroom body (MB) Kenyon cells (KCs). The acquisition of a TOD difference in mutants implies the existence of multiple oscillators that act to normalize memory formation across the day for appetitive processes. Interestingly, aversive STM requires PDF but not PDFR, suggesting that there are valence-specific pathways downstream of PDF that regulate memory formation. These data argue that the circadian clock uses circuit-specific and molecularly diverse output pathways to enhance the ability of animals to optimize responses to changing conditions.

SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT From humans to invertebrates, cognitive processes are influenced by organisms' internal circadian clocks, the pace of which is linked to the solar cycle. Disruption of this link is increasingly common (e.g., jetlag, social jetlag disorders) and causes cognitive impairments that are costly and long lasting. A detailed understanding of how the internal clock regulates cognition is critical for the development of therapeutic methods. Here, we show for the first time that olfactory associative memory in Drosophila requires signaling by Pigment-dispersing factor (PDF), a neuromodulatory signaling peptide produced only by circadian clock circuit neurons. We also find a novel role for the clock circuit in stabilizing appetitive sucrose/odor memory across the day.

Keywords: clock, Drosophila, memory, mushroom body, neuropeptide

Introduction

Cognition is influenced by the circadian clock. Within individual cells, time is kept by a molecular clock; the coordination of time across tissues in the organism is performed by the 'core clock', a small population of individually cycling neurons bound together to form a coherent circuit. The outputs of this circuit generate rhythms of physiology and behavior that oscillate with a period aligned to the solar cycle. Circadian peaks and troughs in learning and memory have been documented in humans as well as rodent and invertebrate models (Gerstner and Yin, 2010; Wright et al., 2012) such that misalignments between an organism's cycling internal clock and external conditions (jetlag, social jetlag, and shift work disorders) impair cognition/memory across phyla (Wittmann et al., 2006; Kott et al., 2012; Weingarten and Collop, 2013; Wright et al., 2013). Irreversible neurodegenerative diseases that affect cognition such as Parkinson's and Alzheimer's exhibit comorbid circadian dysfunction (Videnovic et al., 2014). Even natural aging degrades the fidelity of the body's clock in a manner that has been linked to cognitive decline (Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019). Thus, a complete understanding of how the body's clock regulates cognition could generate new therapeutic approaches.

To date, the clock-memory link has largely been investigated by breaking the transcriptional feedback loop providing intracellular timekeeping in cells of the clock circuit. Eliminating cycling with altered light conditions affects novel object recognition in rodents (Ruby et al., 2013) and time-of-day (TOD)-dependent associative memory in Drosophila (Lyons and Roman, 2009; Le Glou et al., 2012; Chouhan et al., 2015). Clock gene mutations such as Per1, Per2, and Bmal1 generate impairments in contextual fear and spatial memory tasks in mice and are involved in all phases of memory processing (Wang et al., 2009; Rawashdeh et al., 2014; Wardlaw et al., 2014; Snider and Obrietan, 2018). In Drosophila, mutations of the clock genes period and clock impair TOD memory and can disrupt memory generally (Sakai et al., 2004; Lyons and Roman, 2009; Le Glou et al., 2012; Fropf et al., 2014, 2018; Chouhan et al., 2015). But studies using molecular clock mutants do not fully replicate conditions that exist in common human clock-related cognitive disorders; in most, a functional molecular clock is present but outputs are misaligned with environmental cues. To fully understand the role of clock circuit outputs on cognition, it is necessary to manipulate output pathways in the context of an intact molecular clock.

Environmentally-cued circadian rhythms of behavior and physiology are generated by the outputs of the core clock circuit which are organized by peptidergic signaling from a few pacemaker neurons (Aton et al., 2005). In Drosophila, this peptide is Pigment-dispersing factor (PDF), an 18-amino acid peptide produced in the adult brain solely by 16 ventrolateral clock neurons (LNvs; Renn et al., 1999; Park et al., 2000). PDF released from the LNvs signals within the core clock circuit through its only known receptor PDFR (Hyun et al., 2005; Lear et al., 2005; Mertens et al., 2005; Shafer et al., 2008) to coordinate the activity of the ∼150 core clock neurons (Peng et al., 2003; Lin et al., 2004; Stoleru et al., 2005; Yoshii et al., 2009; Liang et al., 2016). Core clock circuit output directs rhythmic aspects of physiology such as locomotor activity and sleep, and PDF also functions in these output pathways. In this way, the PDF/PDFR signaling pathway both maintains free-running circadian activity and promotes output behaviors.

Here, we investigate the involvement of PDF signaling in Drosophila memory, examining the role of clock output on associative olfactory memory for the first time in the context of a functional molecular clock. We find a novel adult-specific requirement for PDF/PDFR signaling in memory that is distinct from its role in synchronizing the clock circuit. We also provide evidence for the existence of a second PDF receptor that allows valence-specific regulation of associative olfactory memory by PDF.

Materials and Methods

Fly stocks and husbandry

All experimental flies were collected directly after eclosion and maintained in incubators at 25°C with a 12/12 h light/dark (12/12 LD) cycle. Flies were reared and housed on cornmeal dextrose food with yeast except those used for GeneSwitch experiments. For Figure 1, 24hr memory, and aversive memory experiments, mixed males and females were used; for cell-specific PDFR expression memory experiments and all activity experiments, males were used; for imaging experiments, females were used. Fly strains used include: Canton-S wild-type (WT), ;;pdf01 (BDSC#26654; backcrossed 6× into WT), han5304;; (BDSC#33068; backcrossed 6× into WT), ;;nsyb-GAL4/TM6B (Goodwin et al., 2018), han5304;;UAS-pdfr-myc13 (gift of Paul Taghert), ;;elav-GS-GAL4 (Osterwalder et al., 2001), VT030559-GAL4 (VDRC ID# v206077), pdfR-2A-LexA;; and ;;pdfattP (Deng et al., 2019), w-,UAS-mCD8-IVS-RFP,LexAop-mCD8-IVS-GFP;; (generated from BDSC#61681), ;pdf-GAL4; (Park et al., 2000), UAS-P2X2 (Lima and Miesenböck, 2005), MB-LexA (Pitman et al., 2011), clk4.1-GAL4 (Zhang et al., 2010), ;LexAop-EPAC; (BDSC#76 031), ;pdfR(10 kb)-GAL4; (Parisky et al., 2016), ;clk856-GAL4; (gift from M. Rosbash).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

The core clock supports appetitive STM throughout the circadian cycle via a PDF signaling pathway. Appetitive olfactory 2-min STM of WT and PDF pathway mutants. A, A cartoon protocol of appetitive olfactory 2-min STM training and testing shows 1-min pairings of two neutral CSs (odor) with and without US (sucrose) interspersed with 1 min rest and followed directly by a 2-min CS choice test. B, C, LI scores for STM tested every 4 h through the 24-h cycle and double-plotted, with white and gray background indicating lights on/off of entrainment phase, Zeitgeber time (ZT) as indicated. Each genotype was fit with one-term Fourier curve, R2 values shown in A. B, Mean LI (circles; n = 8 for each time point) and C normalized mean LI (triangles; mean subtracted, then divided by SD). D, E, STM of pdf01 and han5304 mutants compared with WT at ZT1 and ZT13. Mean LI scores are shown ± SEM, with individual datapoints (circles). Letter categorization indicates groups of statistical equivalence (p > 0.05) or difference found by two-way ANOVA with interactions, Bonferroni post hoc comparisons (all significant comparisons, D: p < 0.01, E: p < 0.005).

Temporal panneuronal expression of pdfr (elav-GS-GAL4, adult only)

GeneSwitch experimental flies (han5304;;elav-GS-GAL4/UAS-Pdfr) were housed after eclosion on normal yeast food containing 0.2 g/L RU486 (Sigma, catalog #8046) diluted into 100% ethanol; control flies were housed after eclosion on normal food containing equal volume 100% ethanol. Total exposure time was 6–12 d. Flies were starved for appetitive learning assays on nutrient-free agarose ± RU486 or ethanol control.

Learning assays

Appetitive and aversive associative olfactory memory assays were performed in an environmental room in red light at 25°C with 65% ambient humidity. Flies were between 4–14 d old [short-term memory (STM)] or 4–10 d old [long-term memory (LTM)] and given at least 10-min acclimation to the environmental room before training or testing. Data for each experiment was pooled from at least three independent experimental days.

Appetitive learning assays were performed as previously described (Liu et al., 2012) using a modification of the methods pioneered by the Quinn lab (Tempel et al., 1983; Tully and Quinn, 1985). Briefly, flies were starved to 10% mortality. Filter papers were prepared blank or with 2 M sucrose as null or unconditioned stimulus (US), and 10% MCH and OCT prepared as conditioned stimulus (CS) odors. As schematized in Figure 1A, 50–100 starved flies were loaded into a vial and exposed to a one trial training of sequential CSA-null and CSB-US pairings of 1-min (STM) or 2-min (LTM) duration. Flies were then either tested for CS preference (2-min STM) or placed into fresh food vials for 4 h and then starved for 20 h before CS preference testing (24-h LTM). Testing involved a 2-min sequential exposure to CS odors, after which flies choosing either odor were counted. A preference index (PI) was calculated for each trial as [(# of flies in CSA) – (# of flies in CSB)]/[(# of flies in CSA) + (# of flies in CSB)]. This PI was averaged with the PI of a temporally-paired CS reciprocal trial to generate the final learning index (LI), calculated as a percentage. In this way, each datapoint shown for these experiments represents 100–200 flies and controls for odor bias. To confirm mutant detection of unpaired stimuli, 2-min preference tests were performed (Table 1) during which flies chose between a stimulus vial (containing either 10% OCT, 10% MCH, 2 m sucrose, or 24 spaced 1-s 90-V shocks) and a neutral vial (PI calculated as above.) Mutant flies showed preferences equivalent to WT for OCT (two-way ANOVA: pgen = 0.086, pZT = 0.52, pgenxZT = 0.90), sucrose (two-way ANOVA: pgen = 0.69, pZT = 0.43, pgenxZT = 0.93), and shock (one-way ANOVA: pWT,pdf01 = 0.736, pWT,han5304 = 0.141, ppdf01,han5304 = 0.027), while han5304 flies showed altered MCH preference compared with WT and pdf01 mutants (two-way ANOVA: pgen = 0.0015, pZT = 0.47, pgenxZT = 0.93; Bonferroni post hoc for effect of genotype: pWT,pdf01 = 0.39, pWT,han5304 = 0.0012, ppdf01,han5304 = 0.046).

Table 1.

Mutant stimulus preference

OCT MCH Sucrose Shock
ZT1 WT −8.99 ± 17.69 −11.44 ± 16.67 70.19 ± 7.73 −52.1 ± 4.9
pdf01 −28.43 ± 6.88 3.56 ± 11.36 66.58 ± 10.99 −46.4 ± 7.3
han5304 2.61 ± 10.65 42.54 ± 11.41 61.13 ± 9.43 −67.0 ± 3.5
ZT13 WT −6.51 ± 12.51 −20.12 ± 14.98 72.13 ± 5.04 -
pdf01 −16.20 ± 6.02 1.13 ± 15.23 74.90 ± 7.67 -
han5304 6.81 ± 13.42 29.56 ± 11.19 67.79 ± 10.21 -

Starved mixed male and female flies were tested at ZT1 and ZT13 for innate (unpaired) odor and sucrose preference. Data reported as the mean ± SEM (odor and sucrose, n = 8 per group; shock, n = 16/group). Two-way ANOVA shows no significant differences of genotype, ZT, or interactions for OCT and sucrose preference (p > 0.05). Two-way ANOVA for MCH preference shows no significant differences of ZT or interactions (p > 0.05), shows effect of genotype (pgen< 0.005; Bonferroni post hoc comparison: pWT,han5304 and ppdf01,han5304 < 0.05, pWT,pdf01 = n.s.) shock preference one-way ANOVA shows effect of genotype (pgen < 0.05; Bonferroni post hoc comparison: ppdf01,han5304 < 0.05, pWT,pdf01 and pWT,han5304 = n.s.)

Aversive learning STM assays were performed similarly to appetitive STM assays with the following modifications. Flies were not starved before training, and the US was provided by supplying 12 1-s 90-mV shocks during the 1-min CS-US pairing. For appetitive STM, flies were immediately tested for CS preference and a final LI calculated from paired reciprocal tests.

Activity assays and data extraction

Male flies were collected immediately after eclosion and individually loaded into sleep tubes and detectors as described previously (Haynes et al., 2015). Flies were entrained for 3 d to 12/12 LD in 25°C incubators, after which 3 d of 12/12 LD baseline activity was collected followed by release into free-running DD conditions for 9 d. During this time locomotor activity data were collected using the Drosophila Activity Monitor (DAM; TriKinetics) as previously described (Agosto et al., 2008). Subsequently, beam break counts were extracted using DAMFileScan (TriKinetics) and analyzed for sleep and activity using MATLAB SCAMP (v.2019, Chris Vecsey, Skidmore) as done previously (Haynes et al., 2015). DD analysis was performed on days 2–8 DD data, with percent of population rhythmic values calculated using cutoff criteria where a rhythmicity index (RI) below 0.2 was considered arrhythmic. LD data were calculated from an average of days 1–3 in LD. Custom MATLAB scripts were written to extract evening anticipation onset and evening peak values from 30-m binned LD activity data of individual subjects and are available on GitHub (https://github.com/Griffith-Lab). Evening anticipation onset was calculated as the max value of the three-bin rolling average of the first derivative for data between Zeitgeber time (ZT)6-ZT11.5 (to prevent artifact from lights-off startle effects). Evening peak was calculated as the max value between ZT6 and ZT12.

Imaging

Functional imaging was performed ex vivo in whole brains with imaging hardware, solutions and acquisition software as described previously (Haynes et al., 2015). Planar image acquisition in MB calyx and surrounding soma (via MB-LexA) or DN1 clock neuron soma (via clk4.1-GAL4) was performed at 2 Hz. For each brain, both an AHL control and drug (ATP, PDF, or FSK) experimental trial was performed, each including 30 s of baseline AHL perfusion followed by 120 s of drug or vehicle perfusion. Thus, these groups were statistically compared by paired t test. For Figure 3E, an additional unpaired t test was used to compare the experimental group (pdf>P2X2,MB>EPAC +ATP) to the empty driver control (P2X2,MB>EPAC +ATP) which received 30 s of baseline AHL followed by 120 s of ATP perfusion. Data were processed before analysis by custom MATLAB scripts which normalized each trace to its baseline and then fit and subtracted out any linear trends present in baseline because of bleaching (available on GitHub: https://github.com/Griffith-Lab). Expression pattern imaging was performed as follows: brains were fixed, stained, mounted and embedded according to Janelia FlyLight protocols (Dissection and Fixation 2% PFA, IHC-Double Label, DPX Mounting; https://www.janelia.org/project-team/flylight/protocols). Primary antibodies included mouse monoclonal anti-GFP (1:200; Roche Applied Science, catalog #11814460001), and rabbit polyclonal anti-Ds-red (1:200; Clontech, catalog #632496). Secondary antibodies included Cy2 goat anti-mouse (1:400; The Jackson Laboratory, catalog #115-225-166) and Alexa Fluor 594 donkey anti-rabbit (1:400; The Jackson Laboratory, catalog #711-585-152) as recommended in Janelia protocols. Samples were imaged on a Zeiss LSM 880 microscope using a Plan-Apochromat 25×/0.8 Imm Corr DIC M27 objective. Data presented in Figure 3B,C, Movie 1 were imaged using 561/595 and 488/515 channels with a slice depth of 0.65 µm and subsequently processed with Airyscan. Data presented in Figure 3D, Movie 2 were acquired with a slice depth of 0.75 µm using 458/550 and 633/698 channels. 3D rendering was performed on Zeiss Zen 2.3 software.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

The MB is not a direct target of the PDF signaling pathway. A, Cartoon representation of LNv/MB anatomy. Purple PDF+ LNv soma (light: small, dark: large) are shown with other core clock cells in light gray. sLNvs send projections anterodorsomedially in close proximity to the MB (magenta) calyx, as indicated. Inset, Area shown in main panel indicated by dotted outline on whole brain. B–D, Confocal images of MB (magenta) and putative Pdfr (green) expression patterns. B, Maximum intensity projection (MIP) of calyx, with single planar slice shown in C where dotted outline shows the only cell with colocalization of markers seen in n = 3 brains (magnified, inset). D, MIP of MB lobe region. E–H, Data shows MB calyx or clock neuron DN1 intracellular cAMP as reported by EPAC1camps FRET signal imaged at 2 Hz (using MB-LexA or clk4.1-GAL4). After a 30-s baseline AHL perfusion, perfusion of drug (ATP, PDF, FSK) or AHL vehicle was bath applied for the remainder of the trial, as indicated by the black bar atop each panel. The mean signal of the terminal 10 s was used for statistical comparisons. Line mean, shadow ± SEM, N of each condition shown in parentheses. E, Calyx EPAC response to bath-applied 30 μm PDF, compared with AHL vehicle control, performed at ZT2–ZT6. Paired t test comparison is non-significant. F, LNv neurons were activated by pdf-GAL4 expression of the ATP-stimulated P2X2 channel and calyx EPAC responses were compared among AHL vehicle and empty driver controls. Data were pooled from five timepoints spaced throughout the 24-h cycle; t test comparisons are non-significant. G, Calyx EPAC response to bath-applied 15 μm forskolin (FSK) compared with AHL vehicle control, performed at ZT2–ZT6. Paired t test, p = 6.71E-06. H, Clock neuron DN1 somatic response to bath-applied 30 μm PDF compared with AHL vehicle control, performed at ZT2–ZT6. Paired t test, p = 3.09E-07.

Movie 1.

Putative PDFR expression is excluded from the MB KCs. Confocal image of MB calyx and KC soma, acquired with a slice depth of 0.65 µm, processed with Airyscan, and rendered as a fly through 5-fps video (posterior→anterior). MB calyx and KC soma are shown in magenta (fixed with dsRed antibody staining of VT030559-GAL4>UAS-mCD8-IVS-RFP); putative PDFR expression pattern is shown in green (fixed with GFP antibody staining of pdfR-2A-LexA>LexAop-mCD8-IVS-GFP).

Download video file (6.5MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0782-20.2020.video.1
Movie 2.

Putative PDFR expression is excluded from the MB lobe neuropil. Confocal image of MB lobe neuropil, acquired with a slice depth of 0.75 µm, and rendered as a 3D rotation around the x-axis. MB lobes are shown in magenta (fixed with dsRed antibody staining of VT030559-GAL4>UAS-mCD8-IVS-RFP); putative PDFR expression is shown in green (fixed with GFP antibody staining of pdfR-2A-LexA>LexAop-mCD8-IVS-GFP). The MB β/β' lobe tips hug the PDFR+ EB projection rings without overlap.

Download video file (1.5MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0782-20.2020.video.2

Experimental design and statistical analysis

Information regarding experimental design for both behavior and imaging can be found in those sections within Materials and Methods. All statistical analyses used are detailed in Results and figure legends and were performed using MATLAB R2019a (MathWorks). As the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for normality is known to be overly sensitive with small sample sizes, statistical approaches accommodating unequal variance or non-parametric data were used in cases (1) where maximum SD was equal or greater than three times' the minimum SD, or (2) there was a known ceiling/floor imposing non-normality. Within a figure, statistically similar groups (p > 0.05) are identified by the same letter assignment; statistically different groups (p < 0.05) can be identified by having differing letter group assignments. For all tests requiring post hoc comparisons (ANOVA, Kruskal–Wallis), the Bonferroni method was used. For any significant (p < 0.05) post hoc comparison p values of an analysis, the largest significant p value informed the selection of the closest standard p values ultimately reported in figure legends (i.e., p < 1E-4 in the case where the largest p value was 0.000045).

Results

WT appetitive olfactory STM is stable throughout the day

While it has previously been shown that there are TOD effects on some forms of olfactory memory in Drosophila (Lyons and Roman, 2009; Fropf et al., 2014, 2018; Chouhan et al., 2015), there has been no investigation of the role of the clock in appetitive olfactory STM. To address this, we first tested appetitive STM of Canton-Special WT flies entrained to a 12/12 LD cycle at six timepoints evenly spaced throughout the 24-h day (Fig. 1B, black). Grouped flies (100–200) were exposed to two neutral odors, one paired with sucrose and the other unpaired. Memory of this experience was assessed directly after training by allowing animals to choose between the two odors; WT flies prefer the odor previously paired with sucrose (Tempel et al., 1983). While a double-plot of the mean LI of each time point was best fit with a nonlinear one-term Fourier curve (R2 = 0.9889; linear fit R2 = 0.5400), there were no statistically significant TOD changes for WT flies (n = 8 per time point, two-way ANOVA, pZT = 0.0373; post hoc comparisons for WT×ZT, all p = 1.00). Thus, WT appetitive STM appears to be relatively stable throughout the 24-h circadian cycle.

The magnitude and TOD-independence of appetitive STM requires PDF/PDFR signaling

To determine whether the circadian clock has a role in regulation of appetitive STM, we asked whether the major molecular output of the core clock, the neuropeptide PDF (Renn et al., 1999; Shafer and Yao, 2014) and its receptor PDFR (Hyun et al., 2005; Lear et al., 2005; Mertens et al., 2005; Choi et al., 2009), affected this behavior. Mutants lacking PDF (pdf01) or PDFR (han5304) were tested for appetitive STM alongside WT flies (Fig. 1B). Both pdf01 and han5304 showed a clear STM deficit compared with WT at all time points (two-way ANOVA: pgenotype = 6.43E-23; Bonferroni post hoc comparisons: pWT,pdf = 3.62E-23, pWT,han = 4.31E-12, ppdf,han = 3.44E-05). All genotypes were responsive to odor, sugar and shock (see Materials and Methods; Table 1). Given the requirement found for both PDF and its receptor PDFR, we conclude that the PDF signaling pathway supports STM throughout the day.

These pdf01 and han5304 data were also well fit by nonlinear Fourier curves (R2 = 0.7812 and R2 = 0.8472, respectively; linear fit R2 = 0.0052 and R2 = 0.0987, respectively), but their apparent amplitudes of oscillation were larger than those of WT. This is made more obvious if the data are normalized to their means to allow direct comparison between genotypes (Fig. 1C). While this normalization dampens the amplitude of the WT oscillation, the pdf01 and han5304 curves show even larger excursions, implying that the PDF signaling pathway may in fact stabilize STM formation over the course of the day.

To test this, we assayed WT STM compared with pdf01 or han5304 at the TODs where we had previously observed largest deviations from the mean: dawn (ZT1) and dusk (ZT13; Fig. 1D,E). These experiments replicated the prior overall deficits seen in pdf01 and han5304 appetitive STM and confirmed that WT appetitive STM does not change with TOD [two-way ANOVA, post hoc comparisons: pWT1,13 = 1.00 (Fig. 1D); pWT1,13 = 1.00 (Fig. 1E)]. However, as predicted by our qualitative observations in Figure 1B,C, both pdf01 and han5304 had significantly lower learning scores at ZT1 relative to ZT13 [two-way ANOVA, post hoc comparisons: ppdf1,13 = 0.008 (Fig. 1D); phan1,13 = 0.003 (Fig. 1E)]. The loss of PDF signaling through PDFR therefore uncovers strong TOD effects on the ability to learn. We conclude that PDF and its receptor PDFR are generally necessary for STM but also have a critical role in ensuring that animals can learn equally well at all TODs.

Appetitive STM requires PDF signaling in adult neurons

In the mature adult fly, PDF is uniquely produced in the LNv neurons of the clock, but PDFR is present in both neurons and glia (Im and Taghert, 2010). To distinguish between a neuronal and a glial PDF target involved in STM, we asked whether neuron-specific expression of a Pdfr cDNA would rescue the han5304 appetitive learning deficit. Panneuronal expression of Pdfr on a han5304 background using the nsyb-GAL4 driver significantly increased STM compared with parental controls at ZT0–ZT2 (one-way ANOVA, p = 4.61E-06; post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 0.808, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 6.10E-06, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 2.07E-04; Fig. 2A). We excluded the possibility of neomorphic effects from the broad nsyb-GAL4 expression pattern by investigating the same panneuronal PDFR overexpression on a WT background, which failed to generate significant changes in STM compared with parental controls (data not shown). Therefore, we concluded that PDF/PDFR signaling in neurons is sufficient for normal appetitive STM.

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Adult-specific neuronal PDF-PDFR signaling is sufficient for appetitive STM formation. Appetitive olfactory STM tested between ZT0–ZT4 in han5304 mutants with panneuronal PDFR expression. A, Constitutive PDFR expression, compared with genetic controls, tested at ZT0–ZT2 and B adult-specific PDFR expression using the RU486-inducible GeneSwitch system, compared with EtOH vehicle and WT controls, tested at ZT0–ZT4. Mean LI scores are shown ± SEM, with individual datapoints (circles). Letter categorization indicates groups of statistical similarity (p > 0.05) or difference found by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons (all significant comparisons, A: p < 0.0005, B: p < 0.05).

To determine whether adult-specific PDF signaling was sufficient for normal STM, we used the drug-inducible driver elav-GS-GAL4 to panneuronally express Pdfr on the han5304 mutant background solely during adulthood. Flies were placed onto food containing RU486 or EtOH vehicle directly after eclosion to fully induce transgene expression (Osterwalder et al., 2001; Depetris-Chauvin et al., 2011). Under these conditions, han5304;;elav-GS>Pdfr flies fed with RU486 and tested at ZT0–ZT4 (Fig. 2B) showed significantly increased appetitive STM compared with their vehicle-fed sibling controls and had learning scores equivalent to the concurrently run WT control (one-way ANOVA, p = 8.0235e-04; post hoc comparisons pEtOH,RU486 = 0.0144, pEtOH,WT = 5.872e-04, pRU486,WT = 0.3288). Taken together, we conclude that PDFR in a population of adult neurons promotes appetitive STM.

Mushroom body (MB) Kenyon cells (KCs) are not direct targets of PDF

We began our search for the relevant PDFR+ neurons in the MB, a site of integration essential for olfactory learning and memory in the fly. Olfactory information is delivered to the MB neuropil by sparse random activation of subpopulations of roughly 2000 KCs whose soma surround the calyx of the MB. Within the compartmentalized MB neuropil lobes, KC axonal projections receive stimulus-specific information from dopaminergic inputs, allowing for time-space coincidence of odor-stimulus pairings (for review, see Busto et al., 2010). Dorsomedial projections from PDF+ sLNv clock cells skew posteriorly in close proximity to the MB KCs and calyx (Fig. 3A; Helfrich-Förster, 1995); while these projections contain small clear core vesicles (Yasuyama and Meinertzhagen, 2010) the recently published Drosophila adult brain connectome shows no direct synaptic routes between sLNv and MB KC neurons (https://neuprint.janelia.org and Xu et al., 2020) permitting us to exclusively focus on sLNv extrasynaptic dense core vesicle signaling as a communication mechanism between sLNvs and MBs. In fact, sLNv projections show TOD-dependent changes in PDF immunoreactivity (Park et al., 2000) and low levels of PDFR mRNA have been detected in some KC subpopulations (Crocker et al., 2016). These prior findings led us to ask whether KCs could be a target of PDF.

We labeled KCs with membrane-bound RFP using the VT030559-GAL4 driver and looked for colocalization with membrane-bound GFP expressed with Pdfr-2A-LexA (Deng et al., 2019), a gene-fusion in the Pdfr locus (Fig. 3B–D) which should recapitulate the endogenous expression pattern of Pdfr. Given the dense packing of the KC soma, we took particular care to maximize our confocal Z-resolution and visualize independent soma by using AiryScan deconvolution. A z-axis maximum image projection (MIP) of the KC soma and MB calyx (Fig. 3B) shows potential overlap of reporter signal; however, a flythrough of the stacked dataset (Movie 1) shows that the entirety of this “overlap” can be attributed to the extremely close proximity of the PDFR+ DN1 clock cell soma and PDFR+ DN1/LNv projections to the calyx and KC soma. In an examination of ∼6000 KCs from three independent brains we saw only a single cell that expressed both GFP and RFP (Fig. 3C, inset). We also examined the MB lobe region to detect any innervation of the neuropil by PDFR+ projections; again, though the z-axis MIP implies potential overlap (Fig. 3D), examination of 3D rotational data (Movie 2) revealed no detectable PDFR+ signal within the MB lobe neuropil. GFP signal closest in proximity belonged to EB-projecting PDFR+ cells.

We also attempted to visualize a functional connection between the PDF+ LNvs and the MB. PDFR is a G-protein-coupled receptor which activates Gαs to increase intracellular cAMP (Duvall and Taghert, 2012, 2013). To allow us to see changes in cAMP we expressed the FRET-based reporter EPAC1cAMPs (Shafer et al., 2008) in the entirety of the MB with MB247-LexA, an approach which we have previously used (Haynes et al., 2015). We investigated the ability of LNvs to directly signal to the MB in two ways. First, we bath applied PDF to MB>EPAC brains (Fig. 3E). We failed to detect a significant EPAC response in the MB calyx after bath application of PDF as compared with vehicle controls, tested at ZT2–ZT6 (paired t test, p = 0.245; Fig. 3E). To rule out the need for some LNv-released co-transmitter, we also measured MB cAMP response after activation of LNvs. Activation was accomplished by expressing ATP-gated P2X2 channels under the control of pdf-GAL4 and perfusing dissected brains with ATP (Lima and Miesenböck, 2005; Yao et al., 2012). Although we performed experiments at five different timepoints spanning the 24-h circadian day, we failed to detect any significant response to ATP within the MB calyx or KC soma relative to vehicle or genetic controls (pooled data, Fig. 3F; paired t test, MB>EPAC,pdf>P2X2 ± ATP, p = 0.153; unpaired t test, MB>EPAC,pdf>P2X2+ATP to genetic controls, p = 0.054). Importantly, positive controls with 15 μm forskolin, a direct activator of adenylate cyclase, validate our ability to detect bona fide cAMP signals in MB (paired t test compared with vehicle, p = 6.71E-06; Fig. 3G). We also verified that the same PDF bath treatment induces a significant 30% average EPAC response in DN1 clock neurons, cells known to be PDF responsive (paired t test, p = 3.09E-07; Fig. 3H). The PDFR+ neuron(s) involved in the regulation of appetitive STM are thus likely to be extrinsic to the MB, acting as interneurons downstream of the PDF+ LNvs to regulate the memory center.

Intraclock PDF signaling is not sufficient for appetitive STM

The two most extensively characterized functions of PDF are regulation of daily locomotor activity timing and maintenance of clock circuit intracellular cycling (manifest by rhythmic locomotor activity in constant conditions). Both of these functions are conducted by PDF activation of PDFR in neurons of the core clock. Because we found that PDF is required for STM but does not directly signal to memory-relevant MB KCs, we next considered PDF signaling within the clock itself as an indirect regulator of memory formation. If cycling of the core clock was required to produce an STM-promoting non-PDF output or if circadian-regulated locomotion itself was a factor in memory formation, intraclock signaling might be memory relevant. We therefore asked whether PDFR expression sufficient for the rescue of canonical han5304 LD and DD locomotor phenotypes is also sufficient for STM rescue. To do this, we first needed to identify clock-related GAL4 lines that were capable of rescuing these phenotypes.

The han5304 LD locomotor activity phenotype has an advance in the timing of evening anticipation onset and an early evening peak activity (Hyun et al., 2005). We observed han5304 activity for three consecutive days in 12/12 LD using the DAM system and, consistent with prior reports, han5304 flies showed accelerated evening onset and evening peak activity compared with WT [Fig. 4A–C; Student's unpaired t test: p = 8.45E-14 (Fig. 4B); p = 4.38E-08 (Fig. 4C)]. We then rescued expression of Pdfr in han5304 using two different clock-related drivers: PdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 and clk856-GAL4, and recorded locomotor activity under similar conditions. The PdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 expression pattern (Fig. 4H) includes the ellipsoid body and one to two cells of each clock subset (Parisky et al., 2016). We found that Pdfr expression under the PdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 promotor was sufficient to delay the han5304 evening onset and return evening peak timing back to WT [Fig. 4E–G; Kruskal–Wallis with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons, p = 5.56E-10, post hoc comparisons: pgal4,UAS = 1.00, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 5.44E-05, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 2.67E-06, pgal4,WT = 3.86E-05, pUAS,WT = 1.89E-06, pUAS+gal4,WT = 1.00 (Fig. 4F); p = 9.60E-09, post hoc comparisons: pgal4,UAS = 1.00, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 2.86E-05, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 9.71E-08, pgal4,WT = 2.67E-02, pUAS,WT = 5.61E-04, pUAS+gal4,WT = 0.568 (Fig. 4G)]. Similarly, clock-limited PDFR expression with clk856-GAL4 (Fig. 4L; Gummadova et al., 2009) significantly delayed the accelerated han5304 evening onset and evening peak timing [Fig. 4I–K; Kruskal–Wallis with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons, p = 1.58E-10, post hoc comparisons: pgal4,UAS = 1.00, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 1.24E-07, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 3.40E-09 (Fig. 4J); p = 1.94E-13, post hoc comparisons: pgal4,UAS = 0.772, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 8.39E-09, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 2.96E-12 (Fig. 4K)].

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Clock-based PDFR supports normal locomotor activity. LD activity and appetitive STM for han5304 and cell-specific Pdfr rescue mutants. A, E, I, Population LD activity (mean ± SEM) shown in 30-min bins averaged from three consecutive days. A, Mean population values of evening anticipation onset (arrows) and evening peak (arrowheads) are shown. Evening anticipation onset (B, F, J) and the evening activity peak (C, G, K) were extracted for individual subjects (circle datapoints; overlaid box-and-whisker). D, In addition to other cells, core clock neurons (categorized by subtype DN, LNd, LPN, and LNv) and EB soma and ring neuropil in han5304 lack PDFR (indicated by empty outlined ROIs). H, L, Compared with D, filled ROIs demonstrate (H) PdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 and (L) clk856-GAL4 mediated PDFR rescue expression patterns on the han5304 background. Letter categorization indicates groups of statistical similarity (p > 0.05) or difference found by (B, C) Student's unpaired t test (p < 1E-7) and (F, G, J, K) Kruskal–Wallis with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons (all significant comparisons F: p < 0.0001; G: p < 0.05; J: p < 1E-7; K: p < 1E-9).

The other phenotype of the han5304 mutant is an inability to maintain circadian rhythmicity of locomotor activity after transfer to total darkness (Hyun et al., 2005). Although our STM assays use flies entrained to a 12/12 LD cycle, it was still possible that a PDFR+ component of the clock involved in maintenance of circadian rhythmicity was required for STM. We therefore examined the free-running activity of those flies in DD conditions, analyzing activity from DD days 2–8 (Table 2). Compared with WT, han5304 had significantly decreased RI (Student's unpaired t test, p = 2.84E-08) and periodicity (Welch's t test, p = 0.0363) and the overall population was much more arrhythmic (70.4% vs 95.6%). PDFR expression with the pdfR(10 kb) promotor failed to increase han5304 RI (one-way ANOVA, p = 4.75E-14, post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 0.19, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 0.0280, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 1, pgal4,WT = 4.38E-14, pUAS,WT = 1.05E-08, pUAS+gal4,WT = 5.575E-07), periodicity (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 2.31E-05, post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 1, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 1, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 1, pgal4,WT = 1.79E-04, pUAS,WT = 9.60E-05, pUAS+gal4,WT = 6.23E-03) or percent rhythmic values to WT levels. han5304;clk856>Pdfr flies had significantly increased RI (one-way ANOVA, p = 1.61E-08, post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 0.013, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 8.37E-04, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 8.16E-09) and percent rhythmic population values compared with their parental controls, while periodicity showed decreased variance but no rescue in mean values (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 1.49E-03, post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 0.263, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 9.27E-04, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 0.219).

Table 2.

Free-running circadian locomotor activity properties of han5304 mutants and rescue genotypes

Analysis group Genotype RI % Rhythmic Periodicity
1 han5304 0.267 ± 0.026 70.4 23.5 ± 0.281
WT 0.501 ± 0.021 95.6 24.1 ± 0.047
2 han5304;pdfR(10 kb)-GAL4/+ 0.218 ± 0.017 50.0 23.5 ± 0.233
han5304;;UAS-Pdfr/+ 0.275 ± 0.016 72.9 23.3 ± 0.137
han5304;pdfR(10 kb)-GAL4/+;UAS-Pdfr/+ 0.295 ± 0.021 73.9 23.6 ± 0.155
WT 0.445 ± 0.020 93.5 24.0 ± 0.071
3 han5304;clk856-GAL4/+ 0.308 ± 0.025 71.0 23.1 ± 0.154
han5304;;UAS-Pdfr/+ 0.216 ± 0.019 60.7 23.8 ± 0.293
han5304;clk856-GAL4/+;UAS-Pdfr/+ 0.422 ± 0.019 93.8 23.7 ± 0.055

Flies used in Figure 4 were released into free-running DD conditions for 8 d following Figure 4 LD recordings; circadian properties are calculated from DD days 2–8 and reported as mean ±SEM. Bold analysis groupings indicate independent experimental comparison groups. Statistical differences (p < 0.05) were found as follows. Analysis group 1 (RI: Student's unpaired t test, p = 2.84E-08; periodicity: Welch's t test, p = 0.0363); analysis group 2 (RI: one-way ANOVA, Bonferroni post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 0.0280, pgal4,WT = 4.38E-14, pUAS,WT = 1.05E-08, pUAS+gal4,WT = 5.575E-07; periodicity: Kruskal–Wallis post hoc comparisons pgal4,WT = 1.79E-04, pUAS,WT = 9.60E-05, pUAS+gal4,WT = 6.23E-03); analysis group 3 (RI: one-way ANOVA, post hoc comparisons pgal4,UAS = 0.013, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 8.37E-04, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 8.16E-09).

Taken together, we find that pdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 driven expression of PDFR rescues only LD activity phenotypes of han5304 mutants, while clk856-GAL4 PDFR expression is sufficient to rescue both LD and DD phenotypes. These drivers therefore allowed us to ask whether PDF's role in appetitive STM is mediated through core-clock-driven LD locomotor patterns or maintenance of circadian rhythmicity. Both of these hypotheses were disproven as we found that neither pdfR(10 kb)-GAL4 (tested at ZT4) nor clk856-GAL4 expression of PDFR (tested at ZT3–ZT10) was able to rescue the han5304 appetitive STM deficit [one-way ANOVA, p = 0.239 (Fig. 5A), p = 3.21E-05 (Fig. 5B), post hoc comparisons: pgal4,UAS = 4.19E-05, pgal4,UAS+gal4 = 0.879, pUAS,UAS+gal4 = 0.001]. Furthermore, as the clk856-GAL4 expression pattern includes nearly all core clock neurons, it is reasonable to entirely infer that clock-based PDFR expression alone is insufficient to support normal appetitive STM. However, to fully exclude any possible synergistic interaction of PDF with the clock and MB in STM behavior, we co-expressed PDFR in the clock and MB simultaneously using clk856-GAL4 and the strong KC-specific driver VT030559-GAL4 on a han5304 background (Fig. 5C). Tested at ZT1–ZT7, this manipulation not only failed to rescue appetitive STM compared with parental controls, but in fact further impaired learning, likely because of either genetic background effects or to a neomorphic effect of strong overexpression of PDFR in the MB (one-way ANOVA, p = 6.15E-03, post hoc comparisons: pVTgal4,UAS = 1.00, pVTgal4,clkgal4UAS+VTgal4 = 8.17E-03, pclkgal4UAS,clkgal4UAS+VTgal4 = 0.0379). These data imply that PDF must be acting on a PDFR+ neuronal target exclusive from both the core clock and the MB KCs to regulate appetitive STM. Our results also argue that the multiple behavioral roles of PDF signaling (on locomotor activity, DD rhythms, and memory) are independently controlled by the expression of PDFR in distinct PDF target neurons.

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

PDF signaling in the clock and MB does not support STM. Appetitive STM of han5304 mutants with cell-specific constitutive Pdfr expression. A–C, Mean LI scores are shown ± SEM, with individual datapoints (circles). Letter categorization within each panel indicates groups of statistical similarity (p > 0.05) or difference found by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons (all significant comparisons, B: p < 0.005; C: p < 0.05). Flies were tested at (A) ZT3–ZT5, (B) ZT3–ZT10, and (C) ZT1–ZT7.

Aversive olfactory STM requires PDF but not PDFR

Differential distribution of PDFR, the only known receptor for PDF, within and external to the clock allow it to have many different and independent behavioral roles. Other signaling pathways, however, often use an additional strategy for diversification: multiple receptors. We were struck by the difference in STM scores between pdf01 and han5304 mutants (Fig. 1B), the pdf01 STM phenotype is significantly and consistently more severe than han5304. Since both of these alleles are protein null (Hyun et al., 2005; Renn et al., 1999) in theory they should have the same magnitude of deficit if PDFR is the sole receptor for PDF. If, however, there were a second receptor for PDF, it could provide some PDF signaling in the han5304 mutant and result in a milder phenotype.

To further explore the learning and memory roles of PDF and PDFR and determine whether the disparity in STM deficits was common to other types of memory, we assayed appetitive LTM and aversive STM. We first investigated a possible difference between pdf01 and han5304 24 h appetitive LTM, tested between ZT2–ZT4 (Fig. 6A). While pdf01 flies were slightly more impaired than han5304 flies were, there was no statistically significant disparity between the mutants (one-way ANOVA, p = 8.00E-04, post hoc comparisons: pWT,pdf01 = 7.79E-04, pWT,han5304 = 0.0215, ppdf01,han5304 = 0.770). However, we found strong evidence for the existence of a second PDF receptor when we tested pdf01 and han5304 mutants for aversive STM (Fig. 6B). In this assay, flies are tested at ZT0–ZT2 for memory to an odor that was previously paired with shock; instead of approaching the paired odor (as in the appetitive assay), flies demonstrate memory by avoiding the paired odor. As before, pdf01 mutants had impaired aversive STM compared with WT, but quite surprisingly, han5304 mutants showed no aversive STM deficit at all (one-way ANOVA, p = 5.08E-06, post hoc comparisons: pWT,pdf01 = 4.90E-05, pWT,han5304 = 1.00, ppdf01,han5304 = 1.97E-05). To definitively confirm a requirement for PDF in aversive STM and rule out the influence of genetic background in pdf01, we crossed pdf01 flies to WT, pdf01, and the recently created pdfattP mutant (Deng et al., 2019) and tested progeny for aversive STM at ZT0–ZT3 (Fig. 6C). pdf01/+ heterozygotes were indistinguishable from WT for STM, confirming the recessive nature of the defect. pdf01 homozygotes and pdfattP/pdf01 transheterozygotes had significant STM defects (Kruskal–Wallis, p = 4.78E-04, post hoc comparisons: pCS/pdf01,pdfattP/pdf01 = 0.0269, pCS/pdf01,pdf01/pdf01 = 3.91E-04, ppdfattP/pdf01,pdf01/pdf01 = 0.676). Collectively, these data suggest that PDF may promote aversive STM by acting not on PDFR, but on a novel, and still unidentified, receptor.

Figure 6.

Figure 6.

Multiple receptor targets permit valence-specific support of STM by a single clock output. Mean LI scores are shown ± SEM, with individual datapoints (circles). A, Twenty-four-hour appetitive LTM of pdf01 and han5304 mutants. B, C, Aversive STM of WT and PDF pathway mutants. Letter categorizations indicate groups of statistical similarity (p > 0.05) or difference found by one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni post hoc comparisons (all significant comparisons, A: p < 0.05; B: p < 1E-4; C: p < 0.05). Flies were tested at (A) ZT2–ZT5 (B) ZT0–ZT2, and (C) ZT0–ZT3.

Discussion

A role for the circadian clock in cognition and memory has been demonstrated in many organisms, including humans (for review, see Gerstner and Yin, 2010; Smarr et al., 2014; Krishnan and Lyons, 2015). Efforts to understand these phenomena have largely involved manipulations of intracellular molecular oscillator components [e.g., period (per)] to abolish intracellular clock cycling. However, this type of manipulation is not fully representative of the nature of human circadian misalignment disorders, as a role for intercellular signaling has also been shown (Yamaguchi et al., 2013). In Drosophila, where investigators have tools to interrogate molecular processes in great mechanistic detail, manipulation of the signaling peptide PDF allowed us to examine the role of a key neuromodulatory output of the core clock circuit in cognition. Our work demonstrates a requirement for PDF in the formation of associative olfactory memory (schematized in Fig. 7). Putting our data in the context of what is known about the molecular clock's influence on cognition, we explore below the importance of TOD on WT and PDF mutant STM and suggest the possible existence of a second PDF receptor.

Figure 7.

Figure 7.

The core clock regulates distinct behaviors via discrete PDF targets: a proposed model. Localization of PDFR to the clock permits control of locomotor activity independently from control of memory. Signaling through PDFR in a population of interneurons extrinsic to both the clock and MB (here shown as IN1) permits regulation of appetitive memory. We propose that, in place of PDF-PDFR signaling, PDF activation of a novel unidentified receptor (here called PDFR2) in a separate population of interneurons (IN2) is required for aversive memory.

PDF acts on multiple targets to regulate behaviors

In the adult fly, we and others have shown that PDF released from the LNv neurons of the core clock circuit acts, via PDFR, both on the clock circuit itself and outside of the clock to drive key circadian features and rhythmicity of locomotor activity (Fig. 4; Table 2). Our data show that PDF and PDFR are also required for robust appetitive olfactory memory and for equalizing the ability to form memory across the day (Figs. 1, 2). An additional layer of valence-specific control is provided by the fact that PDF appears to act in aversive olfactory memory formation independently of PDFR, suggesting the possibility of a second receptor (Fig. 6). Surprisingly, intraclock circuit PDF signaling, which is sufficient for normal locomotor activity, plays no role in PDF's regulation of memory, demonstrating that PDF independently directs multiple behaviors depending on the location of its target receptor.

While regulation of associative olfactory memory requires PDF signaling outside both the clock and the MB KCs (Figs. 35), the identity of the PDF-responsive element is unknown. Examination of the recently completed hemibrain EM connectome shows no monosynaptic pathways between sLNvs and KCs (https://neuprint.janelia.org; Xu et al., 2020), but a recent study supports the existence of a functional connection since activation of LNvs was sporadically able to produce a GCaMP signal in KCs (Pírez et al., 2019). Given the EM data, this is likely because of sLNv regulation of an excitatory KC input neuron (Fig. 7).

The role of circadian rhythms themselves in olfactory STM formation is limited

In Drosophila, a requirement for molecular clock components like per in cognitive tasks has varied, dependent on the type of learning and stage of memory studied (Gailey et al., 1991; Sakai et al., 2004; Lyons and Roman, 2009; Le Glou et al., 2012; Fropf et al., 2014, 2018; Chouhan et al., 2015; Inami et al., 2020). Interestingly, in cases where the locus of action for per has been mapped, PER acts outside the core clock circuit to regulate memory, but these studies do not explore TOD effects. In studies examining TOD effects on cognition (Lyons and Roman, 2009; Fropf et al., 2014, 2018), per mutants or animals rendered arrhythmic by altered light conditions lose TOD differences. Thus, the role of the cycling core clock itself in learning and memory has been mostly restricted to TOD modulation, and PER, like PDF and PDFR, has an apparent role in memory that is independent of its function in the core clock circuit.

PDF signaling opposes a latent TOD rhythm in appetitive STM

Our data suggest that the requirement for PDF in supporting normal levels of appetitive STM is TOD-independent i.e., PDF is required at all TODs to form robust appetitive STM. PDF has been implicated in a wide variety of behaviors that are influenced by circadian time (Chen et al., 1999, 2016; Mertens et al., 2005; Chung et al., 2009; Keene et al., 2011; Kim et al., 2013; Krupp et al., 2013; Nagy et al., 2019), but its only previously known roles in learning and memory are courtship related (Inami et al., 2020). The PDF peptide is well-placed to be an agent of general learning enhancement since it can act on long-range targets via extrasynaptic release and diffusion, allowing it to provide neuromodulation over the entire circadian cycle. The fact that PDF has been characterized as an arousal-promoting signal (Parisky et al., 2008; Chung et al., 2009) may be germane to this role since arousal state is critical to attention in working memory (Ricker et al., 2018).

An intriguing feature of the pdf and Pdfr mutant phenotypes is the emergence of a latent TOD oscillation in STM (Fig. 1). Daily changes in sLNv structure and PDF accumulation at terminals (Park et al., 2000) have maxima and minima at ZT1 and ZT13 (Fernández et al., 2008; Gorostiza et al., 2014) which align with our observed peak and trough of appetitive STM in PDF signaling mutants. The role of PDF in morning arousal (Parisky et al., 2008) may explain the early day appetitive STM trough of pdf and Pdfr animals: since a heightened arousal state facilitates learning, an absence of PDF signaling reduces morning arousal and cripples the ability of animals to make associations with food-predictive odors. In the absence of measurements of the kinetics of learning, however, it is difficult to pinpoint which stage of the learning process is defective. But the latent nature of TOD effects in mutants relative to the stable nature of WT STM throughout the day allows us to infer that this stability is the sum of oscillating contributions from PDF signaling and one or more other cycling factors. In humans, multiple oscillators for working memory have been proposed (Folkard et al., 1983).

TOD-independent appetitive memory has survival value

The diversity of roles for PDF in behavior illustrates how essential the output of the clock is for optimal function and survival. But why, in the context of appetitive STM (which is ultimately TOD-independent), is an organism's capacity for appetitive learning so critically linked to its timekeeping capabilities? Although behaviorally-diverse prior work may, on some levels, appear conflicting regarding TOD influence on associative STM (Lyons and Roman, 2009; Fropf et al., 2018), TOD effects are consistently found in associative LTM (Lyons and Roman, 2009; Fropf et al., 2014, 2018; Chouhan et al., 2015, 2017). The need for stability of appetitive STM may be because of the fact that at the time of training, an organism lacks information necessary to evaluate the benefit of devoting metabolic resources to consolidation of that learning, i.e., future food availability is unknown. Though the most widely-used appetitive STM paradigm requires starvation for expression of memory immediately after training, significant expression of appetitive LTM requires starvation only at the time of retrieval but not at the time of acquisition (Krashes et al., 2009; Chouhan et al., 2017). Furthermore, feeding after appetitive training prompts the decay of appetitive STM within 10–30 min, while LTM can still be observed whether animals are re-starved (Krashes et al., 2009). In light of a STM requirement for PDF throughout the day, we hypothesize that PDF may help maximize information acquisition at the front end of the process, to allow memory to be dispensed with or consolidated in a manner dependent on future internal metabolic state information.

PDF signaling in aversive memory may use a novel receptor

PDF is also required for aversive olfactory STM. Surprisingly, however, there is no requirement for PDFR, the only characterized receptor for this peptide. For pdf mutants, it is unlikely that aversive STM loss is because of a second site mutation since it appears in transheterozygous animals as well. And though a valence-specific compensatory mutation in the han5304 line could mediate this phenomenon, it is hard to envision a mechanism for this. Aversive associative STM is therefore the only behavior known for which PDF is required but PDFR is dispensable. In other organisms, circadian output peptides have multiple receptors. Three GPCRs for the C. elegans homolog of PDF have been identified, each of which is highly similar to Drosophila PDFR and related to VIPR1 and VIPR2, the two known mammalian receptors for VIP (a mammalian peptide with similar clock roles; Janssen et al., 2008, 2009). We suggest that receptor diversity allows the valence-specific regulation of associative STM by regionally segregated and distinct receptors. Since the circuitry involved in acquisition of appetitive versus aversive memory involves different sets of neurons (Riemensperger et al., 2005; Liu et al., 2012; Yamazaki et al., 2018), this implies that PDF will act at circuit nodes in each pathway that are valence-specific and is consistent with our finding that PDF does not act on KCs, the final common substrate of memory. Ultimately, identification and characterization of the second PDF receptor will be required to fully understand this peptide's multiple roles in behavior.

Footnotes

This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01 MH067284 (to L.C.G.), F31 NS110273 (to E.J.R.-R.), and T32 NS007292. We thank Dr. Timothy D. Wiggin for his original versions of EPAC data analysis scripts and his excellent feedback on the project overall.

The authors declare no competing financial interests.

References

  1. Agosto J, Choi JC, Parisky KM, Stilwell G, Rosbash M, Griffith LC (2008) Modulation of GABAA receptor desensitization uncouples sleep onset and maintenance in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 11:354–359. 10.1038/nn2046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aton SJ, Colwell CS, Harmar AJ, Waschek J, Herzog ED (2005) Vasoactive intestinal polypeptide mediates circadian rhythmicity and synchrony in mammalian clock neurons. Nat Neurosci 8:476–483. 10.1038/nn1419 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Busto GU, Cervantes-Sandoval I, Davis RL (2010) Olfactory learning in Drosophila. Physiology (Bethesda) 25:338–346. 10.1152/physiol.00026.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Chen B, Meinertzhagen IA, Shaw SR (1999) Circadian rhythms in light-evoked responses of the fly's compound eye, and the effects of neuromodulators 5-HT and the peptide PDF. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 185:393–404. 10.1007/s003590050400 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chen J, Reiher W, Hermann-Luibl C, Sellami A, Cognigni P, Kondo S, Helfrich-Förster C, Veenstra JA, Wegener C (2016) Allatostatin A signalling in Drosophila regulates feeding and sleep and is modulated by PDF. PLoS Genet 12:e1006346 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006346 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Choi C, Fortin JP, McCarthy EV, Oksman L, Kopin AS, Nitabach MN (2009) Cellular dissection of circadian peptide signals with genetically encoded membrane-tethered ligands. Curr Biol 19:1167–1175. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.06.029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Chouhan NS, Wolf R, Helfrich-Förster C, Heisenberg M (2015) Flies remember the time of day. Curr Biol 25:1619–1624. 10.1016/j.cub.2015.04.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chouhan NS, Wolf R, Heisenberg M (2017) Starvation promotes odor/feeding-time associations in flies. Learn Mem 24:318–321. 10.1101/lm.045039.117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Chung BY, Kilman VL, Keath JR, Pitman JL, Allada R (2009) The GABA(A) receptor RDL acts in peptidergic PDF neurons to promote sleep in Drosophila. Curr Biol 19:386–390. 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Crocker A, Guan X-J, Murphy CT, Murthy M (2016) Cell-type-specific transcriptome analysis in the Drosophila mushroom body reveals memory-related changes in gene expression. Cell Rep 15:1580–1596. 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.04.046 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Deng B, Li Q, Liu X, Cao Y, Li B, Qian Y, Xu R, Mao R, Zhou E, Zhang W, Huang J, Rao Y (2019) Chemoconnectomics: mapping chemical transmission in Drosophila. Neuron 101:876–893.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Depetris-Chauvin A, Berni J, Aranovich EJ, Muraro NI, Beckwith EJ, Ceriani MF (2011) Adult-specific electrical silencing of pacemaker neurons uncouples molecular clock from circadian outputs. Curr Biol 21:1783–1793. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Duvall LB, Taghert PH (2012) The circadian neuropeptide PDF signals preferentially through a specific adenylate cyclase isoform AC3 in M pacemakers of Drosophila. PLoS Biol 10:e1001337. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Duvall LB, Taghert PH (2013) E and M circadian pacemaker neurons use different PDF receptor signalosome components in Drosophila. J Biol Rhythms 28:239–248. 10.1177/0748730413497179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernández MP, Berni J, Ceriani MF (2008) Circadian remodeling of neuronal circuits involved in rhythmic behavior. PLoS Biol 6:e69. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Folkard S, Wever RA, Wildgruber CM (1983) Multi-oscillatory control of circadian rhythms in human performance. Nature 305:223–226. 10.1038/305223a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Fropf R, Zhang J, Tanenhaus AK, Fropf WJ, Siefkes E, Yin JCP (2014) Time of day influences memory formation and dCREB2 proteins in Drosophila. Front Syst Neurosci 8:43. 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00043 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Fropf R, Zhou H, Yin JCP (2018) The clock gene period differentially regulates sleep and memory in Drosophila. Neurobiol Learn Mem 153:2–12. 10.1016/j.nlm.2018.02.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Gailey DA, Villella A, Tully T (1991) Reassessment of the effect of biological rhythm mutations on learning in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 169:685–697. 10.1007/BF00194897 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Gerstner JR, Yin JCP (2010) Circadian rhythms and memory formation. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:577–588. 10.1038/nrn2881 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Goodwin PR, Meng A, Moore J, Hobin M, Fulga TA, Van Vactor D, Griffith LC (2018) MicroRNAs regulate sleep and sleep homeostasis in Drosophila. Cell Rep 23:3776–3786. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.05.078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gorostiza EA, Depetris-Chauvin A, Frenkel L, Pírez N, Ceriani MF (2014) Circadian pacemaker neurons change synaptic contacts across the day. Curr Biol 24:2161–2167. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.07.063 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gummadova JO, Coutts GA, Glossop NRJ (2009) Analysis of the Drosophila Clock promoter reveals heterogeneity in expression between subgroups of central oscillator cells and identifies a novel enhancer region. J Biol Rhythms 24:353–367. 10.1177/0748730409343890 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Haynes PR, Christmann BL, Griffith LC (2015) A single pair of neurons links sleep to memory consolidation in Drosophila melanogaster. ELife 4 10.7554/eLife.03868 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Helfrich-Förster C. (1995) The period clock gene is expressed in central nervous system neurons which also produce a neuropeptide that reveals the projections of circadian pacemaker cells within the brain of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 92:612–616. 10.1073/pnas.92.2.612 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hyun S, Lee Y, Hong ST, Bang S, Paik D, Kang J, Shin J, Lee J, Jeon K, Hwang S, Bae E, Kim J (2005) Drosophila GPCR Han is a receptor for the circadian clock neuropeptide PDF. Neuron 48:267–278. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Im SH, Taghert PH (2010) PDF receptor expression reveals direct interactions between circadian oscillators in Drosophila. J Comp Neurol 518:1925–1945. 10.1002/cne.22311 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Inami S, Sato S, Kondo S, Tanimoto H, Kitamoto T, Sakai T (2020) Environmental light is required for maintenance of long-term memory in Drosophila. J Neurosci 40:1427–1439. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1282-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Janssen T, Husson SJ, Lindemans M, Mertens I, Rademakers S, Ver Donck K, Geysen J, Jansen G, Schoofs L (2008) Functional characterization of three G protein-coupled receptors for pigment dispersing factors in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Biol Chem 283:15241–15249. 10.1074/jbc.M709060200 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Janssen T, Husson SJ, Meelkop E, Temmerman L, Lindemans M, Verstraelen K, Rademakers S, Mertens I, Nitabach M, Jansen G, Schoofs L (2009) Discovery and characterization of a conserved pigment dispersing factor-like neuropeptide pathway in Caenorhabditis elegans. J Neurochem 111:228–241. 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06323.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Keene AC, Mazzoni EO, Zhen J, Younger MA, Yamaguchi S, Blau J, Desplan C, Sprecher SG (2011) Distinct visual pathways mediate Drosophila larval light avoidance and circadian clock entrainment. J Neurosci 31:6527–6534. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6165-10.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kim WJ, Jan LY, Jan YN (2013) A PDF/NPF neuropeptide signaling circuitry of male Drosophila melanogaster controls rival-induced prolonged mating. Neuron 80:1190–1205. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kott J, Leach G, Yan L (2012) Direction-dependent effects of chronic “jet-lag” on hippocampal neurogenesis. Neurosci Lett 515:177–180. 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.03.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Krashes MJ, DasGupta S, Vreede A, White B, Armstrong JD, Waddell S (2009) A neural circuit mechanism integrating motivational state with memory expression in Drosophila. Cell 139:416–427. 10.1016/j.cell.2009.08.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Krishnan HC, Lyons LC (2015) Synchrony and desynchrony in circadian clocks: impacts on learning and memory. Learn Mem 22:426–437. 10.1101/lm.038877.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Krupp JJ, Billeter J-C, Wong A, Choi C, Nitabach MN, Levine JD (2013) Pigment-dispersing factor modulates pheromone production in clock cells that influence mating in Drosophila. Neuron 79:54–68. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Le Glou E, Seugnet L, Shaw PJ, Preat T, Goguel V (2012) Circadian modulation of consolidated memory retrieval following sleep deprivation in Drosophila. Sleep 35:1377–1384B. 10.5665/sleep.2118 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lear BC, Merrill CE, Lin J-M, Schroeder A, Zhang L, Allada R (2005) A G protein-coupled receptor, groom-of-PDF, is required for PDF neuron action in circadian behavior. Neuron 48:221–227. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Liang X, Holy TE, Taghert PH (2016) Synchronous Drosophila circadian pacemakers display nonsynchronous Ca2+ rhythms in vivo. Science 351:976–981. 10.1126/science.aad3997 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Lima SQ, Miesenböck G (2005) Remote control of behavior through genetically targeted photostimulation of neurons. Cell 121:141–152. 10.1016/j.cell.2005.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lin Y, Stormo GD, Taghert PH (2004) The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor coordinates pacemaker interactions in the Drosophila circadian system. J Neurosci 24:7951–7957. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2370-04.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Liu C, Plaçais P-Y, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, Friedrich AB, Siwanowicz I, Rubin GM, Preat T, Tanimoto H (2012) A subset of dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in Drosophila. Nature 488:512–516. 10.1038/nature11304 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lyons LC, Roman G (2009) Circadian modulation of short-term memory in Drosophila. Learn Mem 16:19–27. 10.1101/lm.1146009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Mertens I, Vandingenen A, Johnson EC, Shafer OT, Li W, Trigg JS, De Loof A, Schoofs L, Taghert PH (2005) PDF receptor signaling in Drosophila contributes to both circadian and geotactic behaviors. Neuron 48:213–219. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.09.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Nagy D, Cusumano P, Andreatta G, Anduaga AM, Hermann-Luibl C, Reinhard N, Gesto J, Wegener C, Mazzotta G, Rosato E, Kyriacou CP, Helfrich-Förster C, Costa R (2019) Peptidergic signaling from clock neurons regulates reproductive dormancy in Drosophila melanogaster. PLoS Genet 15:e1008158. 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Osterwalder T, Yoon KS, White BH, Keshishian H (2001) A conditional tissue-specific transgene expression system using inducible GAL4. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:12596–12601. 10.1073/pnas.221303298 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Parisky KM, Agosto J, Pulver SR, Shang Y, Kuklin E, Hodge JJL, Kang K, Kang K, Liu X, Garrity PA, Rosbash M, Griffith LC (2008) PDF cells are a GABA-responsive wake-promoting component of the Drosophila sleep circuit. Neuron 60:672–682. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Parisky KM, Agosto Rivera JL, Donelson NC, Kotecha S, Griffith LC (2016) Reorganization of sleep by temperature in Drosophila requires light, the homeostat, and the circadian clock. Curr Biol 26:882–892. 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Park JH, Helfrich-Förster C, Lee G, Liu L, Rosbash M, Hall JC (2000) Differential regulation of circadian pacemaker output by separate clock genes in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 97:3608–3613. 10.1073/pnas.070036197 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Peng Y, Stoleru D, Levine JD, Hall JC, Rosbash M (2003) Drosophila free-running rhythms require intercellular communication. PLoS Biol 1:E13. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0000013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Pírez N, Bernabei-Cornejo SG, Fernandez-Acosta M, Duhart JM, Ceriani MF (2019) Contribution of non-circadian neurons to the temporal organization of locomotor activity. Biol Open 8:bio039628 10.1242/bio.039628 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Pitman JL, Huetteroth W, Burke CJ, Krashes MJ, Lai S-L, Lee T, Waddell S (2011) A pair of inhibitory neurons are required to sustain labile memory in the Drosophila mushroom body. Curr Biol 21:855–861. 10.1016/j.cub.2011.03.069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Rawashdeh O, Jilg A, Jedlicka P, Slawska J, Thomas L, Saade A, Schwarzacher SW, Stehle JH (2014) PERIOD1 coordinates hippocampal rhythms and memory processing with daytime. Hippocampus 24:712–723. 10.1002/hipo.22262 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Reinhart RMG, Nguyen JA (2019) Working memory revived in older adults by synchronizing rhythmic brain circuits. Nat Neurosci 22:820–827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Renn SC, Park JH, Rosbash M, Hall JC, Taghert PH (1999) A pdf neuropeptide gene mutation and ablation of PDF neurons each cause severe abnormalities of behavioral circadian rhythms in Drosophila. Cell 99:791–802. 10.1016/S0092-8674(00)81676-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Ricker TJ, Nieuwenstein MR, Bayliss DM, Barrouillet P (2018) Working memory consolidation: insights from studies on attention and working memory. Ann NY Acad Sci 1424:8–18. 10.1111/nyas.13633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Riemensperger T, Völler T, Stock P, Buchner E, Fiala A (2005) Punishment prediction by dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Curr Biol 15:1953–1960. 10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Ruby NF, Fernandez F, Garrett A, Klima J, Zhang P, Sapolsky R, Heller HC (2013) Spatial memory and long-term object recognition are impaired by circadian arrhythmia and restored by the GABAA antagonist pentylenetetrazole. PLoS One 8:e72433. 10.1371/journal.pone.0072433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Sakai T, Tamura T, Kitamoto T, Kidokoro Y (2004) A clock gene, period, plays a key role in long-term memory formation in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:16058–16063. 10.1073/pnas.0401472101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Shafer OT, Yao Z (2014) Pigment-dispersing factor signaling and circadian rhythms in insect locomotor activity. Curr Opin Insect Sci 1:73–80. 10.1016/j.cois.2014.05.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Shafer OT, Kim DJ, Dunbar-Yaffe R, Nikolaev VO, Lohse MJ, Taghert PH (2008) Widespread receptivity to neuropeptide PDF throughout the neuronal circadian clock network of Drosophila revealed by real-time cyclic AMP imaging. Neuron 58:223–237. 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Smarr BL, Jennings KJ, Driscoll JR, Kriegsfeld LJ (2014) A time to remember: the role of circadian clocks in learning and memory. Behav Neurosci 128:283–303. 10.1037/a0035963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Snider KH, Obrietan K (2018) Modulation of learning and memory by the genetic disruption of circadian oscillator populations. Physiol Behav 194:387–393. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.06.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Stoleru D, Peng Y, Nawathean P, Rosbash M (2005) A resetting signal between Drosophila pacemakers synchronizes morning and evening activity. Nature 438:238–242. 10.1038/nature04192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Tempel BL, Bonini N, Dawson DR, Quinn WG (1983) Reward learning in normal and mutant Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 80:1482–1486. 10.1073/pnas.80.5.1482 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Tully T, Quinn WG (1985) Classical conditioning and retention in normal and mutant Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 157:263–277. 10.1007/BF01350033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Videnovic A, Lazar AS, Barker RA, Overeem S (2014) 'The clocks that time us'—circadian rhythms in neurodegenerative disorders. Nat Rev Neurol 10:683–693. 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Wang LM, Dragich JM, Kudo T, Odom IH, Welsh DK, O'Dell TJ, Colwell CS (2009) Expression of the circadian clock gene period2 in the hippocampus: possible implications for synaptic plasticity and learned behaviour. ASN Neuro 1:AN20090020 10.1042/AN20090020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Wardlaw SM, Phan TX, Saraf A, Chen X, Storm DR (2014) Genetic disruption of the core circadian clock impairs hippocampus-dependent memory. Learn Mem 21:417–423. 10.1101/lm.035451.114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Weingarten JA, Collop NA (2013) Air travel: effects of sleep deprivation and jet lag. Chest 144:1394–1401. 10.1378/chest.12-2963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Wittmann M, Dinich J, Merrow M, Roenneberg T (2006) Social jetlag: misalignment of biological and social time. Chronobiol Int 23:497–509. 10.1080/07420520500545979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Wright KP, Lowry CA, Lebourgeois MK (2012) Circadian and wakefulness-sleep modulation of cognition in humans. Front Mol Neurosci 5:50. 10.3389/fnmol.2012.00050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Wright KP, Bogan RK, Wyatt JK (2013) Shift work and the assessment and management of shift work disorder (SWD). Sleep Med Rev 17:41–54. 10.1016/j.smrv.2012.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Xu CS, Januszewski M, Lu Z, Takemura S, Hayworth KJ, Huang G, Shinomiya K, Maitin-Shepard J, Ackerman D, Berg S, Blakely T, Bogovic J, Clements J, Dolafi T, Hubbard P, Kainmueller D, Katz W, Kawase T, Khairy KA, Leavitt L, et al. (2020) A connectome of the adult Drosophila central brain. BioRxiv 2020.01.21.911859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Yamaguchi Y, Suzuki T, Mizoro Y, Kori H, Okada K, Chen Y, Fustin J-M, Yamazaki F, Mizuguchi N, Zhang J, Dong X, Tsujimoto G, Okuno Y, Doi M, Okamura H (2013) Mice genetically deficient in vasopressin V1a and V1b receptors are resistant to jet lag. Science 342:85–90. 10.1126/science.1238599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Yamazaki D, Hiroi M, Abe T, Shimizu K, Minami-Ohtsubo M, Maeyama Y, Horiuchi J, Tabata T (2018) Two parallel pathways assign opposing odor valences during Drosophila memory formation. Cell Rep 22:2346–2358. 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Yao Z, Macara AM, Lelito KR, Minosyan TY, Shafer OT (2012) Analysis of functional neuronal connectivity in the Drosophila brain. J Neurophysiol 108:684–696. 10.1152/jn.00110.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yasuyama K, Meinertzhagen IA (2010) Synaptic connections of PDF-immunoreactive lateral neurons projecting to the dorsal protocerebrum of Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Neurol 518:292–304. 10.1002/cne.22210 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Yoshii T, Wülbeck C, Sehadova H, Veleri S, Bichler D, Stanewsky R, Helfrich-Förster C (2009) The neuropeptide pigment-dispersing factor adjusts period and phase of Drosophila's clock. J Neurosci 29:2597–2610. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5439-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Zhang Y, Liu Y, Bilodeau-Wentworth D, Hardin PE, Emery P (2010) Light and temperature control the contribution of specific DN1 neurons to Drosophila circadian behavior. Curr Biol 20:600–605. 10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.044 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Movie 1.

Putative PDFR expression is excluded from the MB KCs. Confocal image of MB calyx and KC soma, acquired with a slice depth of 0.65 µm, processed with Airyscan, and rendered as a fly through 5-fps video (posterior→anterior). MB calyx and KC soma are shown in magenta (fixed with dsRed antibody staining of VT030559-GAL4>UAS-mCD8-IVS-RFP); putative PDFR expression pattern is shown in green (fixed with GFP antibody staining of pdfR-2A-LexA>LexAop-mCD8-IVS-GFP).

Download video file (6.5MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0782-20.2020.video.1
Movie 2.

Putative PDFR expression is excluded from the MB lobe neuropil. Confocal image of MB lobe neuropil, acquired with a slice depth of 0.75 µm, and rendered as a 3D rotation around the x-axis. MB lobes are shown in magenta (fixed with dsRed antibody staining of VT030559-GAL4>UAS-mCD8-IVS-RFP); putative PDFR expression is shown in green (fixed with GFP antibody staining of pdfR-2A-LexA>LexAop-mCD8-IVS-GFP). The MB β/β' lobe tips hug the PDFR+ EB projection rings without overlap.

Download video file (1.5MB, mp4)
DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0782-20.2020.video.2

Articles from The Journal of Neuroscience are provided here courtesy of Society for Neuroscience

RESOURCES