Abstract
Tonic immobility and escape are adaptive anti-predator tactics used by many animals. Escape requires movement, whereas tonic immobility does not. If anti-predator tactics relate to weapon size, males with larger weapons may adopt tonic immobility, whereas males with smaller weapons may adopt escape. However, no study has investigated the relationship between weapon size and anti-predator tactics. In this study, we investigated the relationship between male weapon size and tonic immobility in the beetle Gnathocerus cornutus. The results showed that tonic immobility was more frequent in males with larger weapons. Although most studies of tonic immobility in beetles have focused on the duration, rather than the frequency, tonic immobility duration was not affected by weapon size in G. cornutus. Therefore, this study is the first, to our knowledge, to suggest that the male weapon trait affects anti-predator tactics.
Keywords: weapon, mating strategy, anti-predator strategy, tonic immobility, Gnathocerus cornutus
1. Introduction
Sexual selection is selection pressure for traits related to reproduction [1,2]. Because male–male combat occurs for copulation with females, sexual selection often favours males with larger weapons, which lead to victory in combat with rival males [2,3]. However, weapon size varies among males, and males may adopt alternative reproductive tactics depending on weapon size. For example, males with larger weapons often fight rival males, while males with smaller weapons adopt sneaking or dispersal strategies, for copulation with females [4–6].
Sexual dimorphism often affects predation risk, and males suffer higher predation pressure than do females in many animals [7–9]. For example, male Japanese rhinoceros beetles (Trypoxylus dichotomus) with larger weapons fall prey to more predators than do males with smaller weapons [9]. Moreover, males with larger weapons may be more aggressive against predators, not only rival males. Therefore, anti-predator strategies or tactics (phenotypic) may have coevolved with male weapon size. However, few studies have investigated the relationship between male weapon size and anti-predator tactics (but see [10]).
Tonic immobility, sometimes called thanatosis or death feigning, and escape are adaptive anti-predator strategies used by many animals [11,12]. More mobile individuals often adopt escape strategies, whereas less mobile individuals adopt tonic immobility [13]. That is, alternative anti-predator strategies may depend on mobility [13,14]. In males, alternative anti-predator tactics may be associated with weapon size. Because movement is often costly for males with larger weapons [4,15], these males may adopt tonic immobility rather than escape, as compared with males with smaller weapons. However, no study to our knowledge has investigated the relationship between male weapon size and anti-predator behaviour.
As an anti-predator behaviour, the frequency and duration of tonic immobility are important. Many studies of various beetle species have focused on the duration of tonic immobility [16–18]. However, the adaptive duration of tonic immobility may vary by predator species or density, and individuals that remain motionless for long durations will be at increased risk of being carried off by ants and predated [19]. Therefore, predation pressure may be affected by the frequency, and not only the duration, of tonic immobility.
This study examined male broad-horned flour beetles, Gnathocerus cornutus (Fabricius). Male G. cornutus have an exaggerated mandible that is often used as a weapon in male–male combat for copulation with females [20]. Males adopt alternative reproductive strategies based on mandible size: males with larger mandibles often combat rival males, whereas males with smaller mandibles often adopt dispersal to search for females [6]. Furthermore, male weapon size is correlated with mobility measured using a tracking system, such that males with larger mandibles are less mobile than males with smaller mandibles [20]. First, we conducted predation experiments using the jumping spider (Hasarius adansoni Audouin) as a model predator to test whether tonic immobility of G. cornutus is an adaptive anti-predator behaviour. We also investigated the relationship between weapon size and tonic immobility in G. cornutus.
We hypothesized that males with larger mandibles and lower mobility are more likely to adopt tonic immobility, whereas males with smaller mandibles and greater mobility are more likely to escape. We tested this hypothesis in male G. cornutus.
2. Material and methods
The G. cornutus individuals used in this study were obtained from a laboratory population reared in an incubator (Sanyo, Japan) maintained at 25°C under a 16 h/8 h light/dark cycle (7.00 lights on, 23.00 lights off) with food (19 : 1 wholemeal flour : brewer's yeast). This beetle was originally collected from a wild population in Japan, in June 1957 (see [20] for details).
The predation experiment used female H. adansoni (n = 10) captured indoors as model predators. This experiment followed a previous study [16]. One starved female H. adansoni and one male G. cornutus were placed in an 80 mm diameter, 20 mm high Petri dish and observed for 15 min. The experiment was replicated using 10 different predator–prey pairs.
Virgin 14–21-day-old G. cornutus males (n = 192) were randomly collected from the laboratory population, and we measured prothorax width and mandible length (figure 1) as indicators of body size and weapon size, respectively [20]. Each trait was measured to the nearest 0.01 mm using a dissecting microscope (VM-60; Olympus, Japan). To evaluate the effects of relative weapon size, we also used the residual mandible length for the analysis after regressing mandible length on prothorax width.
Figure 1.
Distribution of the duration of tonic immobility in males of Gnatocerus cornutus. For example, bar ‘20’ shows the number of beetles with a 1–20 s duration of tonic immobility. The black-filled bar shows beetles that did not exhibit tonic immobility. The straight lines in the beetle illustration indicate the mandible length (a) and prothorax width (b).
We observed the frequency and duration of tonic immobility in the male G. cornutus according to the method of Miyatake et al. [16]. To avoid interference from other beetles, each male was placed in a 17 mm diameter, 20 mm high plastic container with food until the observations. Each beetle was moved gently onto a small plate, and tonic immobility was induced by touching the abdomen of the beetle with a thin stick. When the beetle showed tonic immobility, we recorded its duration using a stopwatch. The duration of tonic immobility was defined as the length of time between touching the beetle and detecting its first movement. If the beetle did not respond, the stimulus was repeated up to three times. When the beetle showed tonic immobility, the duration was recorded. If the beetle was unresponsive to these stimulations, it was recorded as a non-responsive individual (see electronic supplementary material). All observations were conducted from 12.00 to 18.00 in a room maintained at 25°C.
The frequency of tonic immobility was analysed using a generalized linear model (GLM) with a binomial distribution and log link function, using prothorax width, absolute mandible length and residual mandible length as explanatory variables. To analyse the duration of tonic immobility, we used a GLM with a gamma distribution, using prothorax width, absolute mandible length and residual mandible length as explanatory variables. All analyses were conducted using R v. 3.4.3 [21].
3. Results
In all the 10 cases, the spider attacked the beetle when the beetle approached. The spider did not eat the beetle immediately, but observed its response for a while. If the prey showed tonic immobility after the spider attack, the predator lost interest after several seconds and the prey survived (n = 8 of 10; see electronic supplementary material, movie S1). If the beetle struggled with the spider or moved, the spider attacked it again, and beetle was eaten (n = 2 of 10; see electronic supplementary material, movie S2).
Figure 1 shows the distribution of tonic immobility duration in G. cornutus. Many beetles had shorter durations, while a few had longer durations (range 0–563.31 s, mean ± s.d. = 58.44 ± 73.27 s). Figure 2 shows the relationship between residual mandible length and the duration (figure 2a) or frequency (figure 2b) of tonic immobility. The GLMs did not show a significant correlation between the duration of tonic immobility and any explanatory variable, whereas the tonic immobility frequency was significantly correlated with mandible length and residual mandible length, but not prothorax width (table 1). Tonic immobility was more frequent in males with relatively long mandibles.
Figure 2.
Relationships of mandible length with the duration (a) and frequency (b) of tonic immobility in G. cornutus. In (b), ‘1’ indicates beetles with tonic immobility and ‘0’ beetles without tonic immobility.
Table 1.
Results of GLMs evaluating the effects of prothorax width, absolute mandible length, and residual mandible length on the frequency and duration of tonic immobility in G. cornutus. Values in bold are statistically significant (p < 0.05).
tonic immobility | factor | d.f. | χ2 | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
frequency | prothorax width | 1 | 0.72 | 0.3977 |
error | 190 | |||
absolute mandible length | 1 | 6.23 | 0.0126 | |
error | 190 | |||
residual mandible length | 1 | 6.10 | 0.0136 | |
error | 190 | |||
duration | prothorax width | 1 | 0.57 | 0.4499 |
error | 190 | |||
absolute mandible length | 1 | 0.00 | 0.9709 | |
error | 190 | |||
residual mandible length | 1 | 0.37 | 0.5406 | |
error | 190 |
4. Discussion
When G. cornutus encountered H. adansoni, beetles that showed tonic immobility survived the attack by the predator. In G. cornutus, males with longer mandibles performed tonic immobility significantly more frequently than did males with shorter mandibles. This suggests that males with longer mandibles adopt tonic immobility as an anti-predator tactic. Tonic immobility does not necessitate movement. Studies of G. cornutus found that longer-mandible males were less mobile than shorter-mandible males [6,22]. Therefore, because mandible size is correlated with mobility in males, anti-predator tactics may differ between males with longer and shorter mandibles in this beetle. This is the first study to our knowledge to report a relationship between a male weapon trait and anti-predator behaviour in insects.
The development of mandible traits and fighting behaviour requires many resources [4,23–25]. Because tonic immobility does not consume energy compared with moving escape tactics [26], tonic immobility may be the optimal tactic for males with larger mandibles. Moreover, larger weapons may have higher physiological costs and consume more resources during development. Therefore, this correlation may also be an adaptive strategy for resource allocation.
The duration of tonic immobility was not correlated with mandible length. In many beetle studies, tonic immobility duration was considered an important anti-predator behaviour [16,27]. However, a longer duration of tonic immobility also increases physiological costs [28] and the risk of being discovered and carried off by ants [19]. Because we found that weapon size is correlated with the frequency but not duration of tonic immobility, the importance of the frequency of tonic immobility should be evaluated as an anti-predator strategy (see [29]).
Because male G. cornutus with shorter mandibles are more mobile [6,22], they may adopt an escape tactic rather than tonic immobility. Additional studies are needed to investigate anti-predator behaviour in shorter-mandible males. Although we expected that larger weapons lead to an increased predation risk, in some cases, the size or existence of weapons may have a positive relationship with predator defence [10]. Coevolution between weapon and predation avoidance may differ among species.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank Dr Christine W. Miller, Dr Devin M. O'Brien and two anonymous reviewers.
Ethics
The research conducted by the authors has been approved by the research ethics review of Kagawa University (AP0000353996) and Okayama University (no. 6433471). The beetle (G. cornutus) and the jumping spider (H. adansoni) used in this study are invertebrates and therefore not subject to animal ethics review. The study was conducted in a manner that avoided or minimized discomfort or distress to the laboratory animals, and efforts were made to ensure that the animals did not suffer unnecessarily at any stage of the experiment. We also used a sample size that minimizes the number of animals that experience discomfort or distress but is not statistically problematic in predation experiments. The G. cornutus beetle culture has been maintained in the laboratory of the National Food Research Institute, Japan for 50 years. They have been maintained at Okayama University for over 15 years. This population has been kept on wholemeal flour with yeast. We reared this population at 25°C, which is similar to the natural conditions for this insect. All animals in the study were handled carefully. The H. adansoni spiders used in the predation experiments were collected from Kagawa University and Okayama University premises using appropriate methods. After completion of the experiment, the animals were reared with food in an environment similar to the natural conditions for this spider.
Data accessibility
Movies of the predation experiment are provided as electronic supplementary material. Data are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.8gtht76mt [30].
Authors' contributions
K.M. conceived and designed the study. K.Y. performed the experiment. Y.F. performed analyses. All authors contributed to the interpretation of data. K.M. and T.M. drafted the manuscript. All authors critically revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final version and agree to be accountable for the content therein.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by Grant-in-Aid from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS) Fellows 20J00383 to K.M., and by KAKENHI 18H02510, MEXT, JSPS to T.M.
References
- 1.Darwin C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Andersson M. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Emlen DJ. 2008. The evolution of animal weapons. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 39, 387–413. ( 10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.39.110707.173502) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Emlen DJ. 2001. Costs and the diversification of exaggerated animal structures. Science 291, 1534–1536. ( 10.1126/science.1056607) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Simmons LW, Emlen DJ. 2006. Evolutionary trade-off between weapons and testes. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 16 346–16 351. ( 10.1073/pnas.0603474103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Yamane T, Okada K, Nakayama S, Mityatake T. 2010. Dispersal and ejaculatory strategies associated with exaggeration of weapon in an armed beetle. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 1705–1710. ( 10.1098/rspb.2009.2017) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Endler JA. 1980. Natural selection on color patterns in Poecilia reticulata. Evolution 34, 76–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Setsuda K, Tsuchida K, Watanabe H, Kakei Y, Yamada Y. 1999. Size dependent predatory pressure in the Japanese horned beetle, Allomyrina dichotoma L. (Coleoptera; Scarabaeidae). J. Ethol. 17, 73–77. ( 10.1007/BF02769300) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Kojima W, Sugiura S, Makihara H, Ishikawa Y, Takanashi T. 2014. Rhinoceros beetles suffer male-biased predation by mammalian and avian predators. Zool. Sci. 31, 109–115. ( 10.2108/zsj.31.109) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Metz MC, Emlen DJ, Stahler DR, MacNulty DR, Smith DW, Hebblewhite M. 2018. Predation shapes the evolutionary traits of cervid weapons. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2, 1619–1625. ( 10.1038/s41559-018-0657-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ohno T, Miyatake T. 2007. Drop or fly? Negative genetic correlation between death-feigning intensity and flying ability as alternative anti-predator strategies. Proc. R. Soc. B 274, 555–560. ( 10.1098/rspb.2006.3750) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Ruxton GD, Allen WL, Sherratt TN, Speed MP. 2019. Avoiding attack: the evolutionary ecology of crypsis, aposematism, and mimicry. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Matsumura K, Sasaki K, Miyatake T. 2016. Correlated responses in death-feigning behavior, activity, and brain biogenic amine expression in red flour beetle Tribolium castaneum strains selected for walking distance. J. Ethol. 34, 97–105. ( 10.1007/s10164-015-0452-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Miyatake T, Tabuchi K, Sasaki K, Okada K, Katayama K, Moriya S. 2008. Pleiotropic antipredator strategies, fleeing and feigning death, correlated with dopamine levels in Tribolium castaneum. Anim. Behav. 75, 113–121. ( 10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.04.019) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Oufiero CE, Garland T Jr. 2007. Evaluating performance costs of sexually selected traits. Funct. Ecol. 21, 676–689. ( 10.10.1111/j.1558-5646.1980.tb04790.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Miyatake T, Katayama K, Takeda Y, Nakashima A, Sugita A, Mizumoto M. 2004. Is death-feigning adaptive? Heritable variation in fitness difference of death-feigning behaviour. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 2293–2296. ( 10.1098/rspb.2004.2858) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Nakayama S, Miyatake T. 2010. Genetic trade-off between abilities to avoid attack and to mate: a cost of tonic immobility. Biol. Lett. 6, 18–20. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0494) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Sendova-Franks AB, Worley A, Franks NR. 2020. Post-contact immobility and half-lives that save lives. Proc. R. Soc. B 287, 20200881 ( 10.1098/rspb.2020.0881) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Miyatake T, Matsumura K, Kitayama R, Otsuki K, Yuhao J, Fujisawa R, Nagaya N. 2019. Arousal from tonic immobility by vibration stimulus. Behav. Genet. 49, 478–483. ( 10.1007/s10519-019-09962-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Okada K, Miyanoshita A, Miyatake T. 2006. Intra-sexual dimorphism in male mandibles and male aggressive behavior in the broad-horned flour beetle Gnatocerus cornutus (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae). J. Insect Behav. 19, 457–467. ( 10.1007/s10905-006-9038-z) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.R Core Team. 2017. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fuchikawa T, Okada K. 2013. Inter- and intrasexual genetic correlations of exaggerated traits and locomotor activity. J. Evol. Biol. 26, 1979–1987. ( 10.1111/jeb.12197) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nijhout HF, Emlen DJ. 1998. Competition among body parts in the development and evolution of insect morphology. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci USA 95, 3685–3689. ( 10.1073/pnas.95.7.3685) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Moczek AP, Nijhout HF. 2004. Trade-offs during the development of primary and secondary sexual traits in a horned beetle. Am. Nat. 163, 184–191. ( 10.1086/381741) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Tomkins JL, Kotiaho JS, LeBas NR. 2005. Phenotypic plasticity in the developmental integration of morphological trade-offs and secondary sexual trait compensation. Proc. R. Soc. B 272, 543–551. ( 10.1098/rspb.2004.2950) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Krams I, Kivleniece I, Kuusik A, Krama T, Freeberg TM, Mänd R, Vrublevska J, Rantala MJ, Mänd M. 2013. Predation selects for low resting metabolic rate and consistent individual differences in anti-predator behavior in a beetle. Acta Ethol. 16, 163–172. ( 10.1007/s10211-013-0147-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kuriwada T, Kumano N, Shiromoto K, Haraguchi D. 2011. Age-dependent investment in death-feigning behaviour in the sweetpotato weevil Cylas formicarius. Physiol. Entomol. 36, 149–154. ( 10.1111/j.1365-3032.2010.00777.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kiyotake H, Matsumoto H, Nakayama S, Sakai M, Miyatake T, Ryuda M, Hayakawa Y. 2014. Gain of long tonic immobility behavioral trait causes the red flour beetle to reduce anti-stress capacity. J. Insect Physiol. 60, 92–97. ( 10.1016/j.jinsphys.2013.11.008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Farkas TE. 2016. Body size, not maladaptive gene flow, explains death-feigning behaviour in Timema cristinae stick insects. Evol. Ecol. 30, 623–634. ( 10.1007/s10682-016-9832-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Matsumura K, Yumise K, Fujii Y, Hayashi T, Miyatake T. 2020. Data from: Anti-predator behaviour depends on male weapon size Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.8gtht76mt) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Matsumura K, Yumise K, Fujii Y, Hayashi T, Miyatake T. 2020. Data from: Anti-predator behaviour depends on male weapon size Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.8gtht76mt) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Movies of the predation experiment are provided as electronic supplementary material. Data are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.8gtht76mt [30].