Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0244799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244799

The effects of viewing a winter forest landscape with the ground and trees covered in snow on the psychological relaxation of young Finnish adults: A pilot study

Ernest Bielinis 1,*, Emilia Janeczko 2, Norimasa Takayama 3, Anna Zawadzka 1, Alicja Słupska 1, Sławomir Piętka 1, Maija Lipponen 4, Lidia Bielinis 5
Editor: Marco Innamorati6
PMCID: PMC7790245  PMID: 33411751

Abstract

Forest bathing is an outdoor activity, and it might be a promising preventive treatment for social problems involving stress. A vast number of studies confirm the positive effects of this activity on people’s health. Nevertheless, little is known about the influence of winter forest bathing when conducted in an environment with snow cover on the ground and trees. Thus, a crossover experiment was designed in this study, with the participation of twenty-two healthy university students from Finland. During the experiment, a short exposition by a forest environment or landscape with buildings (as a control) was applied. Participants self-reported their psychological relaxation before and after the exposition, and the results were analyzed and compared. The mood, emotions, restorativeness, and subjective vitality were recorded as indices reflecting the psychological relaxation effect. The negative mood indices decreased significantly after exposition by the snow-covered environment, but the positive ‘vigor’ indices did not increase or decrease significantly. The level of negative emotions increased after the exposition with the control environment. Likewise, positive emotions decreased after the interaction with the control. Restorativeness was significantly increased after the exposition by the experimental forest but decreased after the viewing of the control buildings. The size of the effect in terms of restorativeness was the highest in this experiment. The subjective vitality was lowered as affected by the control, but it did not increase or decrease after the exposition with the experimental forest. There is probably an effect from the slight interruption in the process from the influence of the forest greens on participants because their vigor and vitality did not increase after the exposition with this environment in the study. However, snow might influence the participants as a calming and emotion-lowering component of the environment, but this idea needs to be further explored with the involvement of participants from other countries who would be viewing forest environments with snow cover and whose psychological relaxation could be measured.

Introduction

Forest bathing (taking in the forest atmosphere, or Shinrin-Yoku) is a recreational outdoor activity conducted in a forest environment to induce a restorative experience or reduce stress [1]. It is an important activity, and the participation of members of modern societies in this type of activity might have promising effects on their health, especially regarding problems with a stress [2, 3]. Forest bathing as conducted in an organized way or performed by the subjects on their own might have a salutary influence on these problems [38]. This activity also has therapeutic potential for treating mental health problems, such as depression, posttraumatic stress disorder or schizophrenia [913]. In addition, forest bathing has been reported to evoke anti-stress effects on psychological and physiological parameters [1, 8, 1321]. Other research showed its positive effect on anti-cancer protein levels in patients during cancer therapy [22].

A great deal of research in the area of forest bathing and other forms of nature-related recreation indicate that these activities might induce a psychological relaxation effect [2325]. This effect on subjects may be measured using self-reporting psychometric techniques, using concepts of mood, affect, restorativeness and vitality, such as the Profile of Mood States Questionnaire, Positive and Negative Affect Schedule questionnaire, Restorative Outcome Scale questionnaire, and Subjective Vitality Scale questionnaire [2629]. Thus, the psychological relaxation effect may be defined as a measurable, positive influence on psychological health and relaxation, and it could be measured using the abovementioned questionnaires [3037].

Forest bathing might also be conducted during the wintertime in the areas of the globe where four seasons occur, and where snow appears in the forest landscape. This winter condition also has a positive influence on the psychological relaxation of participants, just as forest bathing conducted during vegetative seasons did [25, 3840]. The identification of the forest recreation effect (including forest bathing) on psychological relaxation is also important in areas where winter prevails for a significant part of the year, including snow cover. For example, in Finland, a country where the winter lasts from early October to the middle of May (seven and a half months of winter in some parts of the Finnish Lapland), the importance of winter forest recreation is high [41, 42]. Outdoor recreation during the winter in these regions is also important due to economic concerns [43]. The occurrence of snow in the Lapland is also common; hence, outdoor recreation conducted in landscapes with snow is important as well. Thus, knowing how forest bathing conducted in a winter landscape with snow cover might influence the psychological relaxation of subjects involved in this activity is important for many societies, including Finnish ones. This activity is crucial because the subjects involved in this activity might experience psychological relaxation [39], which is crucial for their actual psychological health and for health prevention [44], and it also might be interesting for the market [45]; forest bathing in snow might be promoted as a product in these regions. Therefore, many Finish entrepreneurs are interested in knowing how being in nature affects their clients, and they want to use this information to market their nature-related products [46].

In the literature, there is not much research available on the influence of forest bathing with snow covered trees on psychological relaxation. There are some studies in which participants indicated some preferences for photographs presenting landscapes with snow-covered trees [45], but their psychological relaxation was not measured. In other studies, snow was visible on the ground, but not on the trees; in addition, this environment had a desired, positive influence on psychological relaxation [39]. Moreover, there is strong evidence that winter forest bathing without snow has a positive effect on participants [25]. The previous research, in which psychological relaxation was measured under winter conditions, was conducted with an ‘intense’ form of control, i.e., an urban road environment with intense urban traffic. For this reason, it will be more appropriate if a calmer, silent environment without greens and only with buildings is used as a control. Nevertheless, there are studies in which some ‘restraining factors’, such as the view of the urban buildings in the forest landscape matrix [47] or the use of a laptop during the recreational experience, can waste these experiences and have a negative influence on the participant response in comparison to the control. Snow that fell in the forest on the ground cover and on trees might be seen as a ‘restraining factor’, and this factor should be scientifically examined. It might influence the psychological relaxation of participants who view this landscape [39, 45].

Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine the influence of a winter forest landscape, with the ground and trees covered by snow, on the psychological relaxation of young adults. This investigation was conducted using a silent, calm control in the same winter environment; however, it was surrounded only by buildings in the landscape.

Materials and methods

Ethical statement

This study was ethically reviewed and approved by the Ethical Review Board at the University of Warmia and Mazury in Olsztyn. The number of the ethical statement is 06/2018. All the procedures were performed in this study in accordance with the ethical standards of the Polish Committee of Ethics in Science and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration´s later amendments.

Participants

Twenty-two undergraduate students of Finnish nationality from Häme University of Applied Sciences (11 women, 11 men) participated in this study, and their mean age (± SD) was 22.5 years (± 4.67). Participation in the study was voluntary, and the students confirmed their willingness to be involved in the research through a written consent form (Ethics Committee approval number: 06/2018). The participants were divided randomly into two groups, group A or group B, each of which consisted of 11 participants. They received some information from researchers about the experiment beforehand, but information about the expected results was only given to participants after the study. The gender of the study participants should be balanced between the group A and the group B. In this crossover study, the general female and male groups were the same in the A group and the B group overall. A statistical power analysis was conducted using G*Power 3.1.9.4 free software for Mac (Heinrich Hein University, Düsseldorf, Germany) [48]. The actual power (1–β error probability) was calculated at 0.775. A statistical test ‘ANOVA: repeated measure, within factors’ was used, and a power analysis ‘Post hoc: Compute achieved power’ was applied with an effect size of 0.25 and α error of probability of 0.05. The power of properly prepared experiments is 0.8 or higher; hence, the statistical power in this experiment was close to acceptable.

Experimental stimuli

The participants were involved in four different types of activities in this study, and after each activity, psychometric questionnaires were administered. The first activity was held in a room environment before the participants went to see the forest environment (Pre: Forest), and it reflected the normal, current mental state of the participants. The room environment was a classroom in one of the Evo campus buildings at Häme University of Applied Sciences (southern Finland) (Fig 1A). The second activity involved viewing a forest environment in which the ground and trees were covered by snow (Post: Forest) (Fig 1B). During this exposition, the participants needed to take a 5-minute walk from the room on campus to the forest, in which they stayed or sat for 15 minutes and contemplated the winter forest landscape. The students were allowed to move during the experiment because the temperature was below zero; however, talking, smoking or using electronic devices was forbidden. Additionally, the participants stood at some distance from each other (but in a line) so they did not have a chance to interrupt their neighbors (Fig 1B and 1C). The forest was composed of 80 to 108-year-old Norway spruces (close to 80%) and silver birches of the same age (close to 20%). The third activity (Pre: Buildings) was to stay in a room before viewing a landscape interrupted by buildings (a classroom on the Evo campus) (Fig 1C). The fourth activity was to view an outdoor winter landscape interrupted by buildings on the Evo campus (control environment), which was a two-minute walk from the room. The task of the participants during these activities was to contemplate the landscape for 15 minutes in a sitting or standing position (Fig 1B and 1C). Talking, smoking or using electronic devices during this exposition was also forbidden. The order of participation in the activities of group A or B was reversed (crossover study) but staying in the room environment (activity 1 and 3) always occurred before exposition by each outdoor forest or building environment (activity 2 and 4).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Photos showing the building with the room on the Evo campus (south Finland) for the pre-test (A), participants in standing or sitting positions (depending on the will of the participant) during the exposition by the forest environment with the ground and trees covered by snow (B) and during an exposition by a landscape with buildings (control) (C).

Procedure

The crossover experiment with the engaged students as participants was conducted on the 29th of January, at the Evo campus. On this day, two groups of students (A and B) participated in the experiment in random order. Each participant was involved in each measurement four times, i) after staying in the room environment before viewing the forest, ii) after viewing the forest environment, iii) after being in the room environment and before viewing the landscape with buildings, and iv) after viewing the landscape with buildings. The order depended on the participant affiliation with group A or B. The weather during the experiment was typical of January in southern Finland; the temperature was close to –7°C (–7.23°C ± 0.32), the sky was partially cloudy, there was a very slight wind (1.13 ± 0.71 m/s), the humidity was 94.25 ± 1.39% and the atmospheric pressure was 1000.49 ± 0.19 (Meteorological Station Hämeenlinna Lammi Evo, latitude: 61.21660, longitude: 25.13283). There was a 20-25-cm layer of snow cover on the ground and on the trees. The snow cover layer was similar in both experimental sites (in the forest and in front of the buildings). The climatic conditions were similar in both experimental sites, because there was no strong wind on that day, the differences were not felt by the respondents (oral report). During the measurement, the participants filled in questionnaires (after each exposition), while each questionnaire contained four psychometric tools: Profile of Mood States (POMS), Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS), Restorative Outcome Scale (ROS), and Subjective Vitality Scale (SVS). The respondents were asked to complete the questionnaire at each survey site (on-site).

Measurements

POMS: The Profile of Mood States is a questionnaire used to measure six different mood states, namely tension-anxiety, depression-dejection, anger-hostility, fatigue, confusion, and vigor. The questionnaire is valid and commonly used [26]; a version with 65 items was applied in this research (with a 4-point Likert scale, from 0-not at all to 4-absolutely).

PANAS: The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) is a questionnaire used to measure two types of emotional affect, positive and negative. This questionnaire, which contains 20 items, is valid and reliable [27], and a version with a 5-point Likert scale was used in our study.

ROS: The Restorative Outcome Scale measures the restorative effect of each environment and contains six items. The scale is valid and reliable [49].

SVS: The Subjective Vitality Scale contains four items and measures vitality. The scale is valid and reliable [29].

All the questionnaires were given in English. The level of English proficiency among the Finnish students is very high, and in other studies, this method was used as well [50]. In case of any problems, the participants had a chance to ask bilingual academic teachers from the Evo campus to explain each confusing term in a questionnaire. The Cronbach’s alphas (Table 1) were calculated to estimate the internal consistency and evaluate the usefulness of data for reasoning.

Table 1. Reliability of the experiment and number of items for each (sub) scale.

Scales and Subscales Number of Items Cronbach's Alpha
POMS
Tension 9 0.832
Depression-dejection 15 0.924
Anger-hostility 12 0.899
Vigor 8 0.734
Fatigue 7 0.844
Confusion 7 0.809
PANAS
Positive 10 0.844
Negative 10 0.849
ROS 6 0.969
SVS 4 0.915

POMS: Profile of Mood States; PANAS: Positive and Negative Affect Schedule; ROS: Restorative Outcome Scale; and SVS: Subjective Vitality Scale.

Data and statistical analysis

Raw data from the questionnaires were used for the statistical analysis, and the mean values ± standard deviation were calculated for comparisons. Two-way repeated measures ANOVAs (within-factors) were applied for the analysis, and the primary effects of the ‘Condition’, ‘Time’ and ‘Condition × Time’ Interaction were analyzed for the results of all the psychometric tools used here. After each ANOVA, a post hoc Tukey-Kramer multiple comparisons test was applied. The JMP 15 Trial for Mac with ‘Full-factorial ANOVA Add-in’ (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) was applied for ANOVAs and post hoc calculations. During the pre-test in groups A and B, there were a few missing values (for whole measurements of each participant), and these values were replaced with the values generated automatically by the Expectation Maximization (EM) technique in SPSS software.

Results

POMS

Two-way repeated measures ANOVA (within-factors) was used to analyze the effect of different conditions (forest environment vs. landscape with buildings), the effect of exposure to different environments (pre vs. post), and the interaction among them, on six different subscales of the POMS scale (six different mood states) (Table 2). Regarding the primary effects, the conditions had a significant effect on tension-anxiety, anger-hostility, and vigor (there was a non-significant but marginal effect on confusion). The primary effect of time had a significant effect on vigor and fatigue.

Table 2. Results of two-way repeated measures ANOVA for the Profile of Mood States (mood).

POMS Primary effect Interaction
Condition: Time: Condition × Time
Buildings vs. Forest Pre vs. Post
F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2
Tension-anxiety 9.32 0.006 ** 0.307 3.07 0.094 0.128 43.74 p<0.001 *** 0.676
Depression-dejection 2.34 0.141 0.100 0.05 0.822 0.002 17.92 p<0.001 *** 0.460
Anger-hostility 10.93 0.003 ** 0.342 2.53 0.130 0.107 19.95 p<0.001 *** 0.487
Vigor 22.18 p<0.001 *** 0.514 7.40 0.013 * 0.260 5.51 0.029 * 0.208
Fatigue 1.40 0.249 0.063 21.90 p<0.001 *** 0.511 8.18 0.009 ** 0.280
Confusion 3.76 0.066 # 0.152 26.91 p<0.001 *** 0.562 23.59 p<0.001 *** 0.529

*** p < 0.001,

**p < 0.01,

*p < 0.05, and

# p < 0.1 two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

The results of Tukey-Kramer’s multiple test comparisons showed that five POMS indicators were significantly lower (except for vigor, non-significant differences) after participant exposure to the forest environment than before (Forest: Pre vs. Post) (Table 3). After an exposition viewing of buildings (Buildings: Pre vs. Post), an increase was observed in the level of all five negative mood indicators (except for vigor, which is a positive mood indicator, which decreased, and depression-dejection, which was non-significant but had a high p-value (p = 0.071), and confusion, which was non-significant). By contrast, none of the POMS indicators differed significantly before the viewing of the forest environment or the landscape with buildings (Pre: Forest vs. Buildings). After the exposition, there were significant differences between forest and building observations in terms of POMS indicators; the values of all the negative indicators were significantly lower after exposure to the forest environment (fatigue, marginally non-significant p = 0.053) or higher for the positive vigor subscale (Post: Forest vs. Buildings).

Table 3. Results of multiple comparison tests between forest and building (setting) and pre-post (exposure to the forest or the control) for the Profile of Mood States (mood).

Forest Buildings
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
Tension-anxiety 1.20 0.55 0.58 0.5 p<0.001 *** 1.04 0.29 1.38 0.44 0.023 *
Depression-dejection 0.76 0.45 0.43 0.58 0.035 * 0.56 0.28 0.86 0.57 0.071 #
Anger-hostility 0.62 0.40 0.41 0.5 0.003 ** 0.59 0.39 1.10 0.70 0.003 **
Vigor 1.68 0.46 1.63 0.53 0.975 1.54 0.28 1.11 0.46 0.009 **
Fatigue 1.47 0.82 0.71 0.66 0.005 ** 1.31 0.42 1.16 0.64 0.053 *
Confusion 1.65 0.45 0.95 0.59 p<0.001 *** 1.51 0.31 1.48 0.40 0.995
Pre Post
Forest Buildings Forest Buildings
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
Tension-anxiety 1.20 0.55 1.04 0.29 0.605 0.58 0.5 1.38 0.44 p<0.001 ***
Depression-dejection 0.76 0.45 0.56 0.28 0.844 0.43 0.58 0.86 0.57 0.003 **
Anger-hostility 0.62 0.40 0.59 0.39 0.994 0.41 0.5 1.10 0.70 p<0.001 ***
Vigor 1.68 0.46 1.54 0.28 0.980 1.63 0.53 1.11 0.46 p<0.001 ***
Fatigue 1.47 0.82 1.31 0.42 0.731 0.71 0.66 1.16 0.64 0.053 #
Confusion 1.65 0.45 1.51 0.31 0.676 0.95 0.59 1.48 0.40 0.001 **

*** p < 0.001,

**p < 0.01,

*p < 0.05,

# p < 0.1 ANOVA-Tukey-Kramer.

PANAS

A two-way repeated ANOVA of the PANAS data was applied, with the Condition and Time as effects and with the interaction of these two factors (Table 4). Regarding the primary effects, there was a significant effect of the Condition and Time on the positive aspect of PANAS. The Interaction Condition × Time was significant for both positive and negative aspects of PANAS.

Table 4. Results of two-way repeated measures ANOVA for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (emotion).

PANAS Primary effect Interaction
Condition: Time: Condition × Time
Building vs. Forest Pre vs. Post
F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2
Positive 16.48 0.001 ** 0.440 26.00 P < 0.001 *** 0.553 10.49 0.004 ** 0.333
Negative 0.28 0.601 0.013 0.02 0.898 0.001 18.76 p < 0.001 *** 0.472

*** p < 0.001,

**p < 0.01 two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

The test of multiple comparisons (Table 5) showed that there were no differences between the pre-test (room environment before forest) and post-test for the forest environment for both negative and positive aspects of PANAS (Forest: Pre vs. Post), and there were significant differences for both aspects in between the room environment between viewing buildings and after viewing buildings (Buildings: Pre vs. Post); the positive aspect decreased and the negative aspect increased after the exposition from viewing buildings. There was a significant difference between the exposition by forest or buildings, and viewing buildings decreased the level of the positive aspect of PANAS (Post: Forest vs. Buildings).

Table 5. Results of multiple comparison tests between the forest and buildings (setting) and pre-post results (exposure to the forest or control) for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (emotion).

Forest Buildings
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. p Mean S.D. Mean S.D. p
Positive 2.57 0.60 2.56 0.48 0.999 2.60 0.23 1.89 0.51 p<0.001 ***
Negative 1.61 0.28 1.39 0.59 0.186 1.44 0.18 1.62 0.50 0.271
Pre Post
Forest Buildings Forest Buildings
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. p Mean S.D. Mean S.D. p
Positive 2.57 0.60 2.60 0.233 0.997 2.56 0.48 1.89 0.51 p<0.001 ***
Negative 1.61 0.28 1.44 0.18 0.221 1.39 0.59 1.62 0.50 0.042 *

*** p < 0.001,

*p < 0.05, ANOVA-Tukey-Kramer.

ROS

For the ROS two-way repeated measure, an ANOVA was conducted with two factors, Condition and Time, and their interaction was calculated as well (Table 6). The primary effect of the Condition was significant in this analysis, and the other primary effect of Time was not significant. The Interaction effect Condition × Time was significant.

Table 6. Results of two-way repeated measures ANOVA for the Restorative Outcome Scale (subjective restorativeness).

ROS Primary effect Interaction
Condition: Time: Condition × Time
Building vs. Forest Pre vs. Post
F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2
35.53 p < 0.001 *** 0.629 1.77 0.198 0.780 28.90 p < 0.001 *** 0.579

*** p < 0.001,

**p < 0.01 two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

The Tukey-Kramer’s test of multiple comparisons (Table 7) showed that the ROS level significantly increased after participant exposure to the forest environment (Forest: Pre vs. Post). In addition, the ROS level decreased significantly after the exposition from the buildings (Buildings: Pre vs. Post). There was no significant difference between pre-tests before both experimental variants (Pre: Forest vs. Buildings). Furthermore, the ROS level increased significantly after the exposition by the forest environment and decreased after the exposition by buildings (Post: Forest vs. Buildings).

Table 7. Results of multiple comparison tests between forest and buildings (setting) and pre-post results (exposure to forest or buildings) for the Restorative Outcome Scale (subjective restorativeness).

Forest Buildings
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
ROS 3.37 1.11 4.75 1.42 0.001 ** 3.37 0.79 1.67 0.68 p < 0.001 ***
Pre Post
Forest Buildings Forest Buildings
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
ROS 3.37 1.11 3.37 0.79 1.000 4.75 1.42 1.67 0.68 p < 0.001 ***

*** p < 0.001.

** p < 0.01. ANOVA-Tukey-Kramer.

SVS

A two-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to compare the primary effects of the Condition and Time and the interaction between them for the SVS scores (Table 8).

Table 8. Results of two-way repeated measures ANOVA for the Subjective Vitality Scale (subjective vitality).

Primary effect Interaction
SVS Condition: Time: Condition × Time
Building vs. Forest Pre vs. Post
F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2 F P ƞ2
5.55 0.028 * 0.209 17.27 p < 0.001 *** 0.451 7.02 0.015 * 0.251

*** p < 0.001,

*p < 0.05 two-way repeated measures ANOVA.

There was a significant effect of the Condition and the Time on the SVS scores. The interaction was also significant.

The test of multiple comparisons (Table 9) showed that there was no difference between the pre-test (room environment before forest) and post-test (forest environment) results in the case of SVS (Forest: Pre vs. Post). A significant decrease was observed in the SVS values after participant exposure to the landscape with buildings (Buildings: Pre vs. Post). The room environments before viewing the forest environment did not differ from the room environment before viewing the landscape with buildings (Pre: Forest vs. Buildings). The viewing of the building had a significantly reduced SVS in comparison to the viewing of the forest environment (Post: Forest vs. Buildings).

Table 9. Results of multiple comparison tests between forests and buildings (setting) and pre-post results (exposure to forest or buildings) for the Subjective Vitality Scale (subjective vitality).

Forest Buildings
Pre Post Pre Post
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
SVS 4.42 0.78 4.36 0.97 0.999 4.39 0.70 3.57 0.91 0.002 **
Pre Post
Forest Buildings Forest Buildings
Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P Mean S.D. Mean S.D. P
SVS 4.42 0.78 4.39 0.70 0.999 4.36 0.97 3.57 0.91 0.011 *

** p < 0.01,

* p < 0.05, ANOVA-Tukey-Kramer.

Discussion

Mood states

Consistent with previous studies [2, 14, 18, 24, 25, 39, 40, 5153], this study has confirmed that participation in a 15-minute forest bathing session has a positive effect on the mood states of participants. Overall, the snow covering the ground and the trees were not a restraining factor in these cases. The results of the other studies, in which the influence on participants of a forest environment with a thin (5 cm) cover of snow was examined, also showed that snow is not a strong restraining factor during the recreational experience, and this environment had a positive influence on the moods of participants [39]. Nevertheless, not every mood indicator was easily changed by the forest environment in the present research, and the vigor level did not increase after the exposure to the forest environment (Table 2). By contrast, in research in which participants were involved in winter forest bathing, the level of vigor significantly increased after exposure to a forest environment without snow in the winter [25, 38]. It could be concluded that in some way, in this context, snow is a slight restraining factor because its presence stops the stimulation of participants in the areas responsible for vigor stimulation. The same observations were made in the study in which a thin snow cover occurred [39]. This means that, under certain circumstances, snow can suppress the feeling of vigor as it blocks the stimulating effect of green in the forest and the view of trees. This stimulating effect has been proven in other studies [25, 39]. These observations suggest that some greens that occurred in the forest environment (but were hidden under snow cover in these studies) might stimulate, in some specific way, the vigor of the participants. In addition, the other hypothesis is that the snow-covered environment might influence them not only by visual stimulation but also through a reduction in the concentration of volatiles in the air [54, 55], because in some other research, the vigor level increased after olfactory stimulation by etheric oil [56].

Notably, snow represents natural beauty, shelter (for animals and plants), and belongs to the nature in this area because of the four seasons. In addition, the white color might be perceived as an absence of aggressive colors [57]. Therefore, it seems reasonable that snow cover had no restraining effects on other mood variables. Nevertheless, the comparison of the POMS results with those from other regional studies conducted in forest environments was impossible, because this parameter has not been measured in this context in Finland before. The effect of calm control (environments with buildings not interrupted by entities, e.g., cars) on the POMS indices was significant, and the viewing of landscapes with buildings significantly decreased the mood of participants, but their confusion and depression-dejection were not significantly different.

In the study in which the POMS was measured during winter without snow cover, “the rough control” (control in the experiment in the city, related to i.e. car traffic), induced a negative effect on these two measures as well [25]; however, in the other study, when the snow partially covered the buildings, there was no significant effect on these parameters relative to “the rough control” [39]. The differences between the forest environment and the control environment are spectacular, and a good comparison is a pre-test, which was measured in the room environment and reflected the basic mood state of the participants.

Emotions

The positive and negative effects of the PANAS changed in response to the forest or control environments, respectively, as confirmed in other studies [14, 25, 39, 52, 58]. Only the control environment had a significant influence on the positive and negative aspects of PANAS; the positive aspect decreased after participants viewed the landscape with buildings, but the negative aspect increased in this case. In other studies, with the use of rush control, the effect on PANAS was quite similar; the negative aspect increased, while the positive aspect decreased (but not significantly) after participants viewed the urban environment (street) during a winter without snow cover [25] and similarly under a thin snow cover [39]. In Tyrväinen et al. [3], the positive aspect of PANAS increased in the forest environment but decreased in the urban environment. The negative aspect of PANAS increased in an urban environment during the vegetation season. In the present research, the increase of these two aspects of PANAS in the forest environment was not significant in the winter with snow cover. In the available literature, there are also not many positions with information about the effect of the forest environment on the PANAS level among participants, because these indices are rarely measured.

Restorativeness

The values of the Restorative Outcome Scale increased in the forest environment, similar to other studies [14, 25, 39, 52, 53, 58]. This psychological index is very sensitive at measuring the effects of forest environments on participants. In this study, the size of the effect was the highest among all the analyzed indices for interactions (ƞ2 = 0.579), and thus the effect of different environments on the ROS was high. In addition, forest with snow cover on the ground and on trees had a strong impact on this parameter, as strong as that in other studies [39], and the ROS has the largest effect of all the analyzed indices, but overall, the value of ƞ2 was lower in the cited study (ƞ2 = 0.228 for interaction). The reaction of the Finnish participants in the forest environment was previously measured with the ROS [58]. The ROS value in the control environment decreased, as in the rush control in the winter; in this study, it decreased to approximately 50% in comparison to the room environment, but in another study, this decrease was smaller in the rush control [39].

Subjective vitality

The Subjective Vitality Scale is also a sensitive measure for indicating the effect of the forest environment; it detected this effect well here, as in other studies [3, 14, 21, 25, 39, 53, 58]. The size of the effect on the interaction was relatively small in comparison to other indices used in this research, and the differences were significant only for the forest environment in comparison to the landscape with buildings, and thus, the influence of the forest environment with snow cover had a relatively slight impact on the SVS. This could be a similar situation to the subscale of POMS on vigor; snow is probably a slightly ‘restraining factor’, which halted the increase in vigor and subjective vitality, which needs to be further explored. The calm control significantly decreased the level of SVS, but it might be specific for the Finnish population because in the rush control in the experiment with snow cover, this effect did not occur [39], but this effect needs to be elucidated in further investigations.

In a previous study [25], the vitality was increased as well with Polish participants, in the country with a shorter winter. Populations of northern countries feel winter differently because it is longer, darker and often makes them depressed [59, 60]. For the Finnish people, being outside in the cold forest in the wintertime may also appear as a life-threatening situation, which could also affect the other indices (mood, emotions, and restorativeness).

Limitations

In order to improve the statistical power, it is proposed to perform further studies in which a larger number of respondents and also non-students will participate. In this study, the number of participants was relatively small, but the power of the test was statistically acceptable. In subsequent studies, it is proposed to perform a power analysis of the experiment before collecting data, to obtain information about how large a group of subjects will be sufficient.

In future studies involving non-student participants, the socio-demographic variables should be considered and balanced for each group. Additionally, the assessment of the contact and attitude of the subjects towards the snow environment should also be measured, for example, the level of pleasure induced by snow in the subjects and the experience of sports in the snow may be variables and covariates in this study.

In the current study, a pilot study was carried out on the impact of the snow environment on the subjects. The approach here planned was to expose the subjects to 15 minutes of exposition. However, this is only one type of activity that can be considered, true outdoor recreation often lasts longer and provides a variety of stimuli, what should be considered in future research. It can be imagined that in future studies the experimental group would participate in a two-hour walk in the snow in the forest, or be involved in sports activities in this environment. As a result, it will be possible to obtain results on a more natural form of activity in a forest environment with snow cover.

Another limitation of this work is the use of research in the winter season, but a good reference point would be to perform additional experiments in the growing season. Such an experimental setup will be implemented in future research. We were interested in the winter aspect, mainly because this period in quite a large part of Phenoscandia lasts extremely long, and the long periods of snow cover are inextricably linked with this region of Europe. Hence, the results of the research may have very important regarding implementations for the inhabitants of Finland.

Another limitation of the research was the differentiation of the way of psychological relaxation—the subjects could rest while sitting or standing and looking at the forest. This variable (sitting or standing during relaxation in the forest) was not controlled in the experiment. Another limitation was the fact that the climatic conditions of the environment were not accurately measured in each experimental site (in the forest and in front of the buildings). Both of these limitations will be considered and eliminated in future experiments.

The main limitation of these studies is the low power of the experiment resulting from the small number of subjects (22 people), but to verify the correctness of the research, we calculated the power of the experiment in this study and it is statistically acceptable. In other studies [61, 62] the number of participants was 12–15, which has been considered sufficient to draw preliminary conclusions. Research of this type is characterized by its specificity, which means that they are not conducted in large samples, additionally, coefficients are used to ensure repeatability of the results, i.e. Cronbach's alpha.

Conclusions

This study examined the effect of winter forest bathing in a snow-covered environment, with snow on the ground and on the trees compared with a calm landscape with buildings (no cars, no traffic) used as a control on the psychological relaxation of young Finnish students at HAMK University. The results showed that the level of negative mood indicators (subscales of POMS scale) among the participants primarily significantly decreased after their exposure to a snow-covered forest environment, and a positive indicator of ‘vigor’ did not change this time. The level of positive emotions (PANAS Positive) decreased after participants viewed a calm control with buildings, and in this environment, the level of negative emotions (PANAS Negative) increased. In the forest environment, these two indicators did not increase significantly in comparison to a pre-test (normal, expected state of participants). The restorativeness of the snow-covered environment was high, the ROS indicator increased significantly, and the effect size was the highest of all the psychological indicators included in this study. The calm control significantly decreased the level of ROS at the same level as the rush control did in other studies. The subjective vitality (SVS) did not increase after participant exposure to the snow-covered forest environment but decreased after their exposure to the calm control. In other studies, this indicator increased after exposure to a forest environment in a vegetative state. It is possible that the snow cover is a slightly restraining factor in the environment, which halted the impact of the green on the vigor and vitality of participants, but this idea needs to be elucidated in further investigations.

Data Availability

All data files (Bielinis et al. 2020 Data.xlsx) are available from the Mendeley database: Ernest Bielinis(2020), “Psychological well-being in the winter forest: A pilot study”, Mendeley Data, V1, doi: 10.17632/jprp23jn68.1.

Funding Statement

The publication costs of this article were covered by the Forest Department of the Warsaw University of Life Sciences (SGGW) and from the Department of General Pedagogy at the University of Warmia and Mazury in Olsztyn.

References

  • 1.Tsunetsugu Y, Park BJ, Miyazaki Y. Trends in research related to “shinrin-yoku” (taking in the forest atmosphere or forest bathing) in Japan. Environ Health Prev Med. 2010;15: 27–37. 10.1007/s12199-009-0091-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ochiai H, Ikei H, Song C, Kobayashi M, Miura T, Kagawa T, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of a forest therapy program on middle-aged females. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12: 15222–15232. 10.3390/ijerph121214984 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Tyrväinen L, Ojala A, Korpela K, Lanki T, Tsunetsugu Y, Kagawa T. The influence of urban green environments on stress relief measures: A field experiment. J Environ Psychol. 2014;38: 1–9. 10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.12.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Park BJ, Tsunetsugu Y, Kasetani T, Morikawa T, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y. Physiological effects of forest recreation in a young conifer forest in Hinokage Town, Japan. Silva Fenn. 2009;43: 291–301. 10.14214/sf.213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Morita E, Fukuda S, Nagano J, Hamajima N, Yamamoto H, Iwai Y, et al. Psychological effects of forest environments on healthy adults: Shinrin-yoku (forest-air bathing, walking) as a possible method of stress reduction. Public Health. 2007;121: 54–63. 10.1016/j.puhe.2006.05.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Konijnendijk CC. Urban Forests and Trees. Urban Forests and Trees. 2005. 10.1007/3-540-27684-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Bell S, Tyrväinen L, Sievänen T, Pröbstl U, Simpson M. Outdoor Recreation and Nature Tourism: A European Perspective. Living Rev Landsc Res. 2007;1 10.12942/lrlr-2007-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mcpherson EG, Schroeder HW. Assessing the Benefits and Costs of the. 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bielinis E, Jaroszewska A, Łukowski A. The E ff ects of a Forest Therapy Programme on Mental Hospital Patients with A ff ective and Psychotic Disorders.: 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Poulsen DV, Stigsdotter UK, Djernis D, Sidenius U. ‘Everything just seems much more right in nature’: How veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder experience nature-based activities in a forest therapy garden. Heal Psychol Open. 2016;3 10.1177/2055102916637090 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lee I, Choi H, Bang KS, Kim S, Song MK, Lee B. Effects of forest therapy on depressive symptoms among adults: A systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14 10.3390/ijerph14030321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Shin WS, Shin CS, Yeoun PS. The influence of forest therapy camp on depression in alcoholics. Environ Health Prev Med. 2012;17: 73–76. 10.1007/s12199-011-0215-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Chun MH, Chang MC, Lee SJ. The effects of forest therapy on depression and anxiety in patients with chronic stroke. Int J Neurosci. 2017;127: 199–203. 10.3109/00207454.2016.1170015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Takayama N, Morikawa T, Bielinis E. Relation between psychological restorativeness and lifestyle, quality of life, resilience, and stress-coping in forest settings. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2019;16 10.3390/ijerph16081456 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kim MH, Wi AJ, Yoon BS, Shim BS, Han YH, Oh EM, An KW. The influence of forest experience program on physiological and psychological states in psychiatric inpatients. J Korean Soc Forest Scie. 2015;104: 133–139. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Ochiai H, Ikei H, Song C, Kobayashi M, Takamatsu A, Miura T, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of forest therapy on middle-aged males with high-normal blood pressure. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12: 2532–2542. 10.3390/ijerph120302532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lee J, Park BJ, Tsunetsugu Y, Ohira T, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y. Effect of forest bathing on physiological and psychological responses in young Japanese male subjects. Public Health. 2011;125: 93–100. 10.1016/j.puhe.2010.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Tsunetsugu Y, Lee J, Park BJ, Tyrväinen L, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y. Physiological and psychological effects of viewing urban forest landscapes assessed by multiple measurements. Landsc Urban Plan. 2013;113: 90–93. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.01.014 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Song C, Ikei H, Igarashi M, Takagaki M, Miyazaki Y. Physiological and psychological effects of a walk in Urban parks in fall. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2015;12: 14216–14228. 10.3390/ijerph121114216 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hansen MM, Jones R, Tocchini K. Shinrin-yoku (Forest bathing) and nature therapy: A state-of-the-art review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14 10.3390/ijerph14080851 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Elsadek M, Liu B, Lian Z, Xie J. The influence of urban roadside trees and their physical environment on stress relief measures: A field experiment in Shanghai. Urban For Urban Green. 2019;42: 51–60. 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.05.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kim BJ, Jeong H, Park S, Lee S. Forest adjuvant anti-cancer therapy to enhance natural cytotoxicity in urban women with breast cancer: A preliminary prospective interventional study. Eur J Integr Med. 2015;7: 474–478. 10.1016/j.eujim.2015.06.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ikei H, Komatsu M, Song C, Himoro E, Miyazaki Y. The physiological and psychological relaxing effects of viewing rose flowers in office workers. J Physiol Anthropol. 2014;33: 1–5. 10.1186/1880-6805-33-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Park SA, Song C, Oh YA, Miyazaki Y, Son KC. Comparison of Physiological and Psychological Relaxation Using Measurements of Heart Rate Variability, Prefrontal Cortex Activity, and Subjective Indexes after Completing Tasks with and without Foliage Plants. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14: 1–12. 10.3390/ijerph14091087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bielinis E, Takayama N, Boiko S, Omelan A, Bielinis L. The effect of winter forest bathing on psychological relaxation of young Polish adults. Urban For Urban Green. 2018;29: 276–283. 10.1016/j.ufug.2017.12.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. Profile of mood states: manual. EdITS; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Crawford JR, Henry JD. The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sample. Br J Clin Psychol. 2004;43: 245–265. 10.1348/0144665031752934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Korpela KM, Ylén M, Tyrväinen L, Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Health Place. 2008;14: 636–652. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ryan RM, Frederick C. On Energy, Personality, and Health: Subjective Vitality as a Dynamic Reflection of Well-Being. J Pers. 1997;65: 529–565. 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1997.tb00326.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Smith JC. The psychology of relaxation. Princ Pract Stress Manag. 2007;3: 38–52. 10.1016/j.ctcp.2006.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Smith JC. The new psychology of relaxation and renewal. Biofeedback. 2007;35: 85–89. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Smith JC, Wedell AB, Kolotylo CJ, Lewis JE, Byers KY, Segin CM. ABC relaxation theory and the factor structure of relaxation states, recalled relaxation activities, dispositions, and motivations. Psychol Rep. 2000;86: 1201–1208. 10.2466/pr0.2000.86.3c.1201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Smith JC. Relaxation, meditation, & mindfulness: A mental health practitioner’s guide to new and traditional approaches. Springer Publishing Company; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Smith JC. ABC relaxation theory: An evidence-based approach. Springer Publishing Company; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ohe Y, Ikei H, Song C, Miyazaki Y. Evaluating the relaxation effects of emerging forest-therapy tourism: A multidisciplinary approach. Tour Manag. 2017;62: 322–334. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ochiai H, Song C, Jo H, Oishi M, Imai M, Miyazaki Y. Relaxing Effect Induced by Forest Sound in Patients with Gambling Disorder. Sustainability. 2020;12: 5969. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Doimo I, Masiero M, Gatto P. Forest and wellbeing: Bridging medical and forest research for effective forest-based initiatives. Forests. 2020;11: 791. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Bielinis E, Omelan A, Boiko S, Bielinis L. The restorative effect of staying in a broad-leaved forest on healthy young adults in winter and spring. Balt For. 2018;24: 218–227. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bielinis E, Lukowski A, Omelan A, Boiko S, Takayama N, Grebner DL. The effect of recreation in a snow-covered forest environment on the psychological wellbeing of young adults: Randomized controlled study. Forests. 2019;10:1–17. 10.3390/f10100827 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Song C, Joung D, Ikei H, Igarashi M, Aga M, Park BJ, et al. Physiological and psychological effects of walking on young males in urban parks in winter. J Physiol Anthropol. 2013;32: 1–5. 10.1186/1880-6805-32-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tuulentie S, Heimtun B. New Rural Residents or Working Tourists? Place Attachment of Mobile Tourism Workers in Finnish Lapland and Northern Norway. Scand J Hosp Tour. 2014;14: 367–384. 10.1080/15022250.2014.967998 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Mäkinen TM, Raatikka VP, Rytkönen M, Jokelainen J, Rintamäki H, Ruuhela R, et al. Factors affecting outdoor exposure in winter: Population-based study. Int J Biometeorol. 2006;51: 27–36. 10.1007/s00484-006-0040-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Tyrväinen L, Uusitalo M, Silvennoinen H, Hasu E. Towards sustainable growth in nature-based tourism destinations: Clients’ views of land use options in Finnish Lapland. Landsc Urban Plan. 2014;122: 1–15. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.10.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Shephard RJ. Exercise and relaxation in health promotion. Sport Med. 1997;23: 211–217. 10.2165/00007256-199723040-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Tyrväinen L, Silvennoinen H, Hallikainen V. Effect of the season and forest management on the visual quality of the nature-based tourism environment: a case from Finnish Lapland. Scand J For Res. 2017;32: 349–359. 10.1080/02827581.2016.1241892 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Neuvonen M, Riala M, Nummelin T, Sievänen T, Tuulentie S. Future perspectives on outdoor recreation in Finland. Leisure/Loisir. 2018;42: 365–388. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hauru K, Lehvävirta S, Korpela K, Kotze DJ. Closure of view to the urban matrix has positive effects on perceived restorativeness in urban forests in Helsinki, Finland. Landsc Urban Plan. 2012;107: 361–369. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2012.07.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39: 175–191. 10.3758/bf03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Kalevi K, Matti Y, Liisa T, Silvennoinen H. Determinants of restorative experiences in everyday favorite places. Heal Place. 2008;14: 636–652. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2007.10.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.El Ansari W, Suominen S, Samara A. Eating habits and dietary intake: Is adherence to dietary guidelines associated with importance of healthy eating among undergraduate university students in Finland? Cent Eur J Public Health. 2015;23: 306–313. 10.21101/cejph.a4195 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Park BJ, Furuya K, Kasetani T, Takayama N, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y. Relationship between psychological responses and physical environments in forest settings. Landsc Urban Plan. 2011;102: 24–32. 10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.03.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Takayama N, Saito H, Fujiwara A, Horiuchi M. The effect of slight thinning of managed coniferous forest on landscape appreciation and psychological restoration. Prog Earth Planet Sci. 2017;4 10.1186/s40645-017-0129-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Takayama N, Korpela K, Lee J, Morikawa T, Tsunetsugu Y, Park BJ, et al. Emotional, restorative and vitalizing effects of forest and urban environments at four sites in Japan. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2014;11: 7207–7230. 10.3390/ijerph110707207 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Meneguzzo F, Albanese L, Bartolini G, Zabini F. Temporal and Spatial Variability of Volatile Organic Compounds in the Forest Atmosphere. Preprints. 2019;2019110246 10.3390/ijerph16244915 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Ikei H, Song C, Miyazaki Y. Effects of olfactory stimulation by α-pinene on autonomic nervous activity. J Wood Sci. 2016;62: 568–572. 10.1007/s10086-016-1576-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Matsumoto T, Asakura H, Hayashi T. Effects of olfactory stimulation from the fragrance of the Japanese citrus fruit yuzu (Citrus junos Sieb. ex Tanaka) on mood states and salivary chromogranin A as an endocrinologic stress marker. J Altern Complement Med. 2014;20: 500–506. 10.1089/acm.2013.0425 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Elliot AJ, Maier MA. Color Psychology: Effects of Perceiving Color on Psychological Functioning in Humans. Annu Rev Psychol. 2014;65: 95–120. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115035 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Simkin J, Ojala A, Tyrväinen L. Restorative effects of mature and young commercial forests, pristine old-growth forest and urban recreation forest—A field experiment. Urban For Urban Green. 2020;48: 126567 10.1016/j.ufug.2019.126567 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Hiltunen P, Jokelainen J, Ebeling H, Szajnberg N, Moilanen I. Seasonal variation in postnatal depression. J Affect Disord. 2004;78: 111–118. 10.1016/s0165-0327(02)00239-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Saarijärvi S, Lauerma H, Helenius H, Saarilehto S. Seasonal affective disorders among rural Finns and Lapps. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1999;99: 95–101. 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb07206.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Takayama N, Fujiwara A, Saito H, Horiuchi M. Management effectiveness of a secondary coniferous forest for landscape appreciation and psychological restoration. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2017;14 10.3390/ijerph14070800 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Takayama N, Saito K, Fujiwara A, Tsutsui S. Influence of Five-day Suburban Forest Stay on Stress Coping, Resilience, and Mood States. J Environ Inf Sci. 2018;2017: 49–57. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Marco Innamorati

14 Jul 2020

PONE-D-20-06816

The Effects of Viewing Winter Forest Landscape With Ground and Trees Covered By Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Bielinis,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Aug 28 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marco Innamorati

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent. In the Methods section, please ensure that you have specified what type of consent you obtained (for instance, written or verbal) and whether the ethics committee approved this consent procedure. If verbal consent was obtained please state why it was not possible to obtain written consent and how verbal consent was recorded. If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please amend either the abstract on the online submission form (via Edit Submission) or the abstract in the manuscript so that they are identical.

5. Please amend either the title on the online submission form (via Edit Submission) or the title in the manuscript so that they are identical.

6. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

7. We note that Figure [B & C] includes an image of a [patient / participant / in the study]. 

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review the manuscript titled: “The Effects of Viewing Winter Forest Landscape With Ground and Trees Covered By Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults”. The manuscript under review deals with the relationships between contact with nature and psychological wellbeing outcomes, such as emotion, restorativeness and subjective vitality. The topic of the manuscript would seem to be relevant and deserves attention. Nevertheless, I think a major revision of the manuscript is needed. I strongly recommend to test such relationships through a second study collecting a bigger sample and including non-student participants. The current sample is very small and includes only student participants. It is a fine pilot study. In a second study, an a priori statistical power analysis should be carried out before the collecting data. Participants gender needs to be balanced between experimental and control groups. In the current manuscript, it is not clear the composition of each group concerning the gender variable. A second study involving non-student participants would need to take into account socio-demographic variables and such variables have to be balanced for each group. Even though the study has been carried out in Finland a measure of confidence with snowy environments (e.g., pleasantness of snowy environments, skilled skier) should be assessed in order to control such a variable as a covariate. Authors mention such an aspect, in the discussion section, when referring to Finland winter and this variable could address this issue. In the discussion more can be done to provide suggestions concerning applied interventions as well as future researches.

Minor comments:

The current version of the manuscript needs to be edited because there are some typos. For example, p.12 row 250, in the following statement: “with the Condition as … and Time” a word is missing. There are other typos which need to be edited throughout the manuscript.

Authors should replace “males” and “females” with “women” and “men”. Even though females and males have been widely used in the past, women and men are more suitable terms for current academic writing because it takes out gendered stereotypes.

Reviewer #2: Authors highlight an interesting approach. Notwithstanding this aspect, the paper shows several limitations. Firstly, to investigate the role of snow in positive effect on psychological aspects such as relaxation they should complete the experiment by introducing another condition about a landscape without snow. Secondly, the paper miss several bibliographic references (i.e., there are numerous phrases missing of reference). The major concern is about the lack of power due to the small sample size

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0244799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244799.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


15 Sep 2020

Response to Editors’s:

Ad. 1. Done

Ad 2. Additional details were added.

Ad. 3. Data have private information about participants and cannot be spread online.

Ad. 4. Done

Ad. 5. Done

Ad. 6. Done

Ad. 7. We shaded faces and elements responsible for recognition in participants on the photographs, so as we think, now this photograph can be officially publish.

Review Comments to the Author

� Reviewer #1: Thank you for the opportunity to review the manuscript titled: “The Effects of Viewing Winter Forest Landscape With Ground and Trees Covered By Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults”. The manuscript under review deals with the relationships between contact with nature and psychological wellbeing outcomes, such as emotion, restorativeness and subjective vitality. The topic of the manuscript would seem to be relevant and deserves attention.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. We are grateful that Reviewer # 1 appreciated our work and our topic selection.

� Nevertheless, I think a major revision of the manuscript is needed. I strongly recommend to test such relationships through a second study collecting a bigger sample and including non-student participants. The current sample is very small and includes only student participants. It is a fine pilot study

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. The presented studies are the result of preliminary studies conducted in Finland in winter. These are, of course, a pilot study, and to underline this we decided to add the statement "pilot study" in the title of this manuscript.

Since this is a preliminary study, there are some limitations that we take into account, so we have added a "limitations" section to this manuscript. It would be difficult to carry out additional research in the near future, due to the time of the year, which will be in a few months (winter) and the constraints related to the pandemic, and the topic is important and in our opinion, the latest report on the impact of snow on the respondents deserves publication.

So if the reviewer kindly allows, we will highlight in this manuscript the fact that this is a pilot study (by adding the statement "pilot study" in the title) and refer to the individual improvements proposed by the reviewer, these references will be included in the "limitations" chapter. In our opinion, thanks to this solution, it will be possible to publish the manuscript with these results, but it will be known to the reader what the limitations of the research are. We believe that this will be a compromise between the requirements of Reviewer # 1 and our team's ability to conduct another study at this point.

In the "limitations" chapter we added:

In order to improve the statistical power, it is proposed to perform further studies in which a larger number of respondents and also non-students will participate. In this study, the number of participants was relatively small, but the power of the test was statistically acceptable.

� In a second study, an a priori statistical power analysis should be carried out before the collecting data.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. In the "limitations" chapter we added:

In subsequent studies, it is proposed to perform a power analysis of the experiment before collecting data, to obtain information about how large a group of subjects will be sufficient.

� Participants gender needs to be balanced between experimental and control groups. In the current manuscript, it is not clear the composition of each group concerning the gender variable.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. In the chapter "materials and methods" we added:

The gender of the study participants should be balanced between the experimental group and the control group. In this crossover study, the general female and male groups were the same in the control group and the experimental group overall.

� A second study involving non-student participants would need to take into account socio-demographic variables and such variables have to be balanced for each group.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. In the "limitations" chapter we added:

In future studies involving non-student participants, the socio-demographic variables should be considered and balanced for each group.

� Even though the study has been carried out in Finland a measure of confidence with snowy environments (e.g., pleasantness of snowy environments, skilled skier) should be assessed in order to control such a variable as a covariate. Authors mention such an aspect, in the discussion section, when referring to Finland winter and this variable could address this issue.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. In the "limitations" chapter we added:

In future studies, the assessment of the contact and attitude of the subjects towards the snow environment should also be measured, for example, the level of pleasure induced by snow in the subjects and the experience of sports in the snow may be variables and covariates in this study.

� In the discussion more can be done to provide suggestions concerning applied interventions as well as future researches.

Thank you to reviewer # 1 for this comment. In the "limitations" chapter we added:

In the current study, a pilot study was carried out on the impact of the snow environment on the subjects. The approach here planned was to expose the subjects to 15 minutes of stimulation. However, this is only one type of activity that can be considered, true outdoor recreation often lasts longer and provides a variety of stimuli, and in future research these natural activities should be considered as experimental and tested stimulation. It can be imagined that in future studies the experimental group would participate in a two-hour walk in the snow in the forest, or be involved in sports activities in this environment. As a result, a more natural form of activity could be tested in a forest environment with snow cover.

� Minor comments:

The current version of the manuscript needs to be edited because there are some typos. For example, p.12 row 250, in the following statement: “with the Condition as … and Time” a word is missing. There are other typos which need to be edited throughout the manuscript.

Authors should replace “males” and “females” with “women” and “men”. Even though females and males have been widely used in the past, women and men are more suitable terms for current academic writing because it takes out gendered stereotypes.

Thanks to reviewer # 1 for these comments. We included these minor changes to the manuscript.

� Reviewer #2: Authors highlight an interesting approach.

Thank you to reviewer # 2 for this comment. We are grateful that Reviewer # 1 appreciated our work and our topic selection.

� Notwithstanding this aspect, the paper shows several limitations.

Firstly, to investigate the role of snow in positive effect on psychological aspects such as relaxation they should complete the experiment by introducing another condition about a landscape without snow.

Thank you to reviewer # 2 for this comment. As this is a pilot study, the number of options has been reduced as much as possible, we will include this approach in future studies. If the reviewer allows, we will present this approach as a limitation of our research, while proposing future research. At the same time, we would like to draw attention to the fact that in many studies the type of experiment was presented as control + experimental variant, then the influence of forest areas during the growing season on the participants of the experiment was examined, and in this type of research there were no additional variants or additional control. We adopted a similar course of action in our experiment.

We have added the following in the "limitations" section:

Another limitation of this work is the use of research in the winter season, but a good reference point would be to perform additional experiments in the growing season. Such an experimental setup will be implemented in future research. We were interested in the winter aspect, mainly because this period in quite a large part of Phenoscandia lasts extremely long, and the long periods of snow cover are inextricably linked with this region of Europe. Hence, the results of the research may have very important implementations for the inhabitants of Finland

� Secondly, the paper miss several bibliographic references (i.e., there are numerous phrases missing of reference).

Thank you to reviewer # 2 for this comment. After careful verification of the text, we added the missing literature items.

� The major concern is about the lack of power due to the small sample size.

Thank you to reviewer # 2 for this comment. This is obviously a limitation of this research. We have added the following in the "limitations" section:

The main limitation of these studies is the low power of the experiment resulting from the small number of subjects (22 people), but to control this fact, we calculated the power of the experiment in this study and it is statistically acceptable. In other studies (including Takayma et al.), The number of participants was 12-15, which was sufficient to draw meaningful conclusions. Research of this type is characterized by its specificity, which means that they are not conducted in large samples, additionally, coefficients are used to ensure repeatability of the results, i.e. Cronbach's alpha.

Decision Letter 1

Marco Innamorati

2 Nov 2020

PONE-D-20-06816R1

The Effects of Viewing a Winter Forest Landscape with the Ground and Trees Covered in Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults: A Pilot Study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Bielinis,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Dec 17 2020 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marco Innamorati

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: No

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: In the current version of the manuscript, Authors recognize several limitations of the research and they use a more cautious title. Nevertheless, there is a statements that should be dampened. The statement, "In other studies [56,57] the number of participants was 12-15, which was sufficient to draw meaningful conclusions" should be reworded in the following way: "In other studies [56,57] the number of participants was 12-15, which HAS BEEN CONSIDERED sufficient to draw PRELIMINARY conclusions." Previous underpowered studies are not a justification to carry out future studies underpowered.

Even though the manuscript report limitations, Authors recognize such limitations in the discussion section and provide some suggestions for future studies. Moreover, the topic is somewhat original and might stimulate future research. Thus, in my view, the manuscript could be accepted as a pilot study after this round of minor revision by which Authors dampen the statement above reported.

Reviewer #2: Part of my concerns have been adressed by authors. Unfortunately, there are important issues that continue to be present in the paper like uncorrect use of some terms and not clear statements that have to be corrected or clarified. In general, the study should be described with greater methodological rigor.

Examples are:

-in my opinion, the term "stimulation" used to describe experimental conditions should be substituted with "exposition"

- you define the sample as "healthy" but this statement is not supported with any test (line 128)

- if groups are equal in composition and experimental conditions they received, why did you define two groups as experimental and control (e.g., line 138)? In statistical analyses section you use a within design. You should clarify this aspect

- In lines 73-74, bibliographic references are necessary. It is a crucial point of the paper because you operatively define the "psychological relaxation effect".

- in both buildings and forest conditions subjects could stay stayed or sat. Given the importance of these two condition on "psychological relaxation effect", this aspect should to be controlled.

-"sample size of 0.25" should be corrected with "effect size of 0.25" (line 143)

-lines 136-138 are not clear

- you should clarify if the climate features (e.g., temperature) are the same in "forest" and "building" conditions. Indeed, the snow was a prominent feature of forest condition, but you didn't report the state of snow in building condition.

-lines 349-354 are not clear, you should explain better these concepts and support them with bibliografic references

-in line 371 what do you mean with "rough control"?

-lines 480-481 should be clarified

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0244799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244799.r004

Author response to Decision Letter 1


10 Nov 2020

Reviewer #1:

We thank the reviewer for these comments, we hope our responses to these comments are sufficient, and appreciate the reviewer's contribution to improving the quality of this work.

In the current version of the manuscript, Authors recognize several limitations of the research and they use a more cautious title. Nevertheless, there is a statements that should be dampened. The statement, "In other studies [56,57] the number of participants was 12-15, which was sufficient to draw meaningful conclusions" should be reworded in the following way: "In other studies [56,57] the number of participants was 12-15, which HAS BEEN CONSIDERED sufficient to draw PRELIMINARY conclusions." Previous underpowered studies are not a justification to carry out future studies underpowered.

Thanks to reviewer # 1 for this comment, of course we made some corrections.

Even though the manuscript report limitations, Authors recognize such limitations in the discussion section and provide some suggestions for future studies. Moreover, the topic is somewhat original and might stimulate future research. Thus, in my view, the manuscript could be accepted as a pilot study after this round of minor revision by which Authors dampen the statement above reported.

Thank you to the reviewer for appreciating this topic and our corrections.

Reviewer #2:

Part of my concerns have been adressed by authors. Unfortunately, there are important issues that continue to be present in the paper like uncorrect use of some terms and not clear statements that have to be corrected or clarified. In general, the study should be described with greater methodological rigor.

We thank the reviewer for these comments, we hope our responses to these comments are sufficient, and appreciate the reviewer's contribution to improving the quality of this work.

Examples are:

-in my opinion, the term "stimulation" used to describe experimental conditions should be substituted with "exposition"

Thank you for this comment, we've made some corrections.

- you define the sample as "healthy" but this statement is not supported with any test (line 128)

Thank you for this comment, we have removed this adjective.

- if groups are equal in composition and experimental conditions they received, why did you define two groups as experimental and control (e.g., line 138)? In statistical analyses section you use a within design. You should clarify this aspect

We thank the reviewer for this comment. Of course we meant group A and group B. It was a crossover study (control and experimental groups were exchanging, so this is the same as group A and group B). We have supplemented this in the text.

- In lines 73-74, bibliographic references are necessary. It is a crucial point of the paper because you operatively define the "psychological relaxation effect".

We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have added needed literature here.

- in both buildings and forest conditions subjects could stay stayed or sat. Given the importance of these two condition on "psychological relaxation effect", this aspect should to be controlled.

We thank the reviewer for this comment. Unfortunately, we have not controlled this effect, so we consider it a research limitation (if the reviewer kindly allows). We added an explanation:

“Another limitation of the research was the differentiation of the way of psychological relaxation - the subjects could rest while sitting or standing and look at the forest. However, this variable, ie sitting or standing, was not controlled in the experiment.”

\\

-"sample size of 0.25" should be corrected with "effect size of 0.25" (line 143)

We thank the reviewer for this comment. Yes, we have corrected it.

-lines 136-138 are not clear

We thank the reviewer for this comment. We clarified it, it was about groups A and B ( which are interchangeably control and corossover study group).

- you should clarify if the climate features (e.g., temperature) are the same in "forest" and "building" conditions. Indeed, the snow was a prominent feature of forest condition, but you didn't report the state of snow in building condition.

We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have added the following text:

„The snow cover layer was similar in both experimental sites (in the forest and in front of the buildings). The climatic conditions were similar in both experimental sites, because there was no strong wind on that day, the differences were not felt by the respondents (oral report).”

and in limitations:

“Another limitation may be the fact that the climatic conditions of the environment were not accurately measured in each experimental site (in the forest and in front of buildings). Both of these limitations will be considered and eliminated in the next experiment.”

-lines 349-354 are not clear, you should explain better these concepts and support them with bibliografic references

Thank you to recent for this comment. We have added the following comment:

“This means that we believe that, under certain circumstances, snow can suppress the feeling of vigor as it blocks the stimulating effect of green in the forest and the view of trees. This stimulating effect has been proven in other studies”

-in line 371 what do you mean with "rough control"?

Thank you to recent for this comment. We've added the following text, and used this statement in quotation marks.

“(control in an experiment in the city, related to, inter alia, car traffic).”

-lines 480-481 should be clarified

Thank you to recent for this comment. We dropped that sentence - it was deleted.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Review PLOS ONE 09.11.2020.docx

Decision Letter 2

Marco Innamorati

11 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-06816R2

The Effects of Viewing a Winter Forest Landscape with the Ground and Trees Covered in Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults: A Pilot Study

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Bielinis,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 25 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Marco Innamorati

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

Reviewer #3: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Comments and concerns have been adressed. Limitations have been properly highlighted. I suggest to authors a further English check.

Reviewer #3: - It has been already said that the experimental subjects were randomly assigned to the two groups (line 133), there is no need to repeat it in line 179

- In lines 359-36: "the other strong hypothesis is that the snow covered environment might influence them not only by visual stimulation but also through a reduction in the concentration of volatiles in the air". This aspect is not directly measured, so it may be more appropriate to re-evaluate the term "strong hypothesis" used in the paper

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 Jan 7;16(1):e0244799. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0244799.r006

Author response to Decision Letter 2


15 Dec 2020

Response to reviewers:

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reply:

We thank the reviewer for this comment.

Reviewer #2: Comments and concerns have been addressed. Limitations have been properly highlighted. I suggest to authors a further English check.

Reply:

We would like to thank the reviewer for this comment, we have made language corrections as a result of an additional language check two times by qualified proofreader and also proofreading services.

Reviewer #3:

- It has been already said that the experimental subjects were randomly assigned to the two groups (line 133), there is no need to repeat it in line 179

Reply:

We thank the reviewer for this comment, we have removed the repetitive fragment of the text.

- In lines 359-36: "the other strong hypothesis is that the snow covered environment might influence them not only by visual stimulation but also through a reduction in the concentration of volatiles in the air". This aspect is not directly measured, so it may be more appropriate to re-evaluate the term "strong hypothesis" used in the paper

Reply:

We thank the reviewer for this comment, we reevaluated the meaning of this sequence, removed the word "strong".

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 3

Marco Innamorati

17 Dec 2020

The Effects of Viewing a Winter Forest Landscape with the Ground and Trees Covered in Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults: A Pilot Study

PONE-D-20-06816R3

Dear Dr. Bielinis,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Marco Innamorati

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #3: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #3: Comments and concerns have been addressed. Repetitive fragment on the text has been removed. The re-evaluation of the meaning of the sequence: "the other strong hypothesis is that the snow covered environment might influence them not only by visual stimulation but also through a reduction in the concentration of volatiles in the air" has been completed

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #3: No

Acceptance letter

Marco Innamorati

21 Dec 2020

PONE-D-20-06816R3

The Effects of Viewing a Winter Forest Landscape with the Ground and Trees Covered in Snow on the Psychological Relaxation of Young Finnish Adults: A Pilot Study

Dear Dr. Bielinis:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Marco Innamorati

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Review PLOS ONE 09.11.2020.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All data files (Bielinis et al. 2020 Data.xlsx) are available from the Mendeley database: Ernest Bielinis(2020), “Psychological well-being in the winter forest: A pilot study”, Mendeley Data, V1, doi: 10.17632/jprp23jn68.1.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES