Abstract
Large-scale family studies on the co-occurrence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and borderline personality disorder (BPD) are lacking. Thus, we aimed to estimate the co-occurrence and familial co-aggregation of clinically ascertained ADHD and BPD diagnoses using the entire Swedish population. In a register-based cohort design we included individuals born in Sweden 1979–2001, and identified their diagnoses during 1997–2013; in total, 2,113,902 individuals were included in the analyses. We obtained clinical diagnoses of ADHD and BPD from inpatient and outpatient care. Individuals with an ADHD diagnosis had an adjusted (for birth year, sex, and birth order) odds ratio (aOR) of 19.4 (95% confidence interval [95% CI] = 18.6–20.4) of also having a BPD diagnosis, compared to individuals not diagnosed with ADHD. Having a sibling with ADHD also increased the risk for BPD (monozygotic twins, aOR = 11.2, 95% CI = 3.0–42.2; full siblings, aOR = 2.8, 95% CI = 2.6–3.1; maternal half-siblings, aOR = 1.4, 95% CI = 1.2–1.7; paternal half-siblings, aOR = 1.5, 95% CI = 1.3–1.7). Cousins also had an increased risk. The strength of the association between ADHD and BPD was similar in females and males, and full siblings showed similar increased risks regardless of sex. Among both males and females, ADHD and BPD co-occur within individuals and co-aggregate in relatives; the pattern suggests shared genetic factors and no robust evidence for etiologic sex differences was found. Clinicians should be aware of increased risks for BPD in individuals with ADHD and their relatives, and vice versa.
Subject terms: ADHD, Genetics
Introduction
Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder [1], with a general population prevalence of 5–10% in childhood [2] and about 2–5% in adulthood [3]. Comorbid disorders associated with ADHD include neurodevelopmental (e.g., autism spectrum disorders), externalizing (e.g., substance use disorders), and internalizing disorders (e.g., depression and anxiety disorders) [4–6]. Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a common personality disorder, with onset in adolescence, and prevalence in the adult general population of about 1–4% [7–9]. BPD is characterized by emotional dysregulation [10] leading to severely impaired interpersonal functioning, extensive use of health care, and a high risk of suicide [8]. Although it is increasingly recognized that emotional dysregulation is important also in ADHD, population-based data on the co-occurrence of ADHD and BPD within individuals and co-aggregation in families are still lacking [11]. A better understanding of this comorbidity is important. First, it may contribute to the understanding of shared mechanisms underlying both ADHD and BPD [10]. Second, if these disorders share underlying mechanisms, it would suggest that treatment strategies for one disorder may be effective for the other [10].
The association between ADHD and BPD has been addressed in epidemiological studies, suggesting that the disorders often co-occur in the same individuals [11], an association first highlighted in a study of self-reported childhood ADHD symptoms in BPD cases, compared to controls, by Fossati et al. in 2002 [12]. More recently, a Swedish study found that the prevalence of BPD in individuals with ADHD was 37.0% [13], and in a German sample of adult women with BPD, 41.5% were screen-positive for childhood history of ADHD of which 16.1% had ADHD symptoms that persisted into adulthood [14]. A study based on self-reports from approximately 34,000 US individuals found that 33.7% with ADHD also self-reported BPD as compared to 5.2% in the general population [15]. However, as these prior results were mainly based on either small clinical samples or self-reported data, it remains unclear if the co-occurrence of clinically diagnosed ADHD and BPD is present at the population level.
Family and twin studies suggest that susceptibility to ADHD is largely genetic in origin [16–19]. A number of studies have demonstrated genetic overlaps of ADHD with neurodevelopmental, e.g. [5, 20], externalizing, e.g. [21], and internalizing disorders, e.g. [22]. but little is known about the extent to which the genetic risk factors of ADHD are also shared with BPD. Only one twin study has investigated the genetic and environmental contributions to the association between ADHD- and BPD-traits. It showed that half of the association between ADHD and BPD was explained by genetic factors, while the remaining part of the association was explained by environmental factors unique to the individual [23]. Whether these findings generalize to individuals with clinically diagnosed ADHD and BPD and to non-twin samples remains to be investigated.
The main objective of the current study was to estimate the co-occurrence and familial co-aggregation between clinically diagnosed ADHD and BPD in a total population cohort.
Materials and methods
Study population
We linked Swedish registers through the unique personal identification number given to each Swedish citizen. The Medical Birth Register [24] provided sex and birth date; we included everyone born between 1 January 1979 and 31 December 2001 (2,319,694 individuals). We excluded stillbirths, congenital malformations, and deaths during infancy (108,079). Using the Cause of Death Register [25], we excluded individuals who died before their 12th birthday (6283). Via the Total Population Register [26] we excluded individuals who emigrated before their 12th birthday (72,337). Age 12 represents the latest age of first presentation of ADHD symptoms according to DSM-5. Finally, using the Multi-Generation Register [27], we linked individuals to their biological parents, and excluded individuals who did not have both parents known (19,093), yielding a sample of 2,113,902 individuals. The Multi-Generation Register and the Twin Register [28] allowed us to identify relatives: monozygotic twins (sharing essentially 100% of alleles), dizygotic twins (sharing on average 50% of co-segregating alleles), full siblings (50% of co-segregating alleles), maternal half-siblings (25%), paternal half-siblings (25%); cousins whose parents were full siblings (12.5%); cousins whose parents were maternal- and paternal half-siblings (6.25%).
The study was approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm (Dnr 2013/862–31/5); since it was a registry study no individual was contacted, and informed consent was waived. The validation sub-study was approved by the Swedish Central Ethics Board (Dnr Ö 27-2012).
Study variables
We linked the data to the Swedish National Patient Register [29] (NPR) and Prescribed Drug Register [30] (PDR). The NPR comprises diagnoses from inpatient health care, and from outpatient visits to specialist care from 2001 and onwards [29]; we used International Classification of Diseases 10th revision [31] (ICD-10) diagnoses from 1 January 1997 (when ICD-10 was introduced in Sweden) to 31 December 2013. We used data from PDR on all dispensed medications from 1 June 2005 to 31 December 2014.
We identified individuals with ADHD from the NPR (ICD-10 code F90; Hyperkinetic Disorder) and/or from the PDR via prescriptions of ADHD medications (methylphenidate, amphetamine, dexamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, or atomoxetine). Details supporting the validity of this definition are available in previous publications, including the validity of using ADHD medications as proxy for ADHD diagnosis [32, 33].
We identified individuals with BPD from the NPR (ICD-10 code F60.3; Emotionally Unstable Personality Disorder, the diagnosis code used in Swedish version of ICD-10 to correspond to DSM-IV-TR Borderline Personality Disorder) [34]. As the validity of the BPD diagnosis in the NPR had not been previously investigated, we performed a separate validation study, following a previously described methodology [35]. Briefly, we requested 100 randomly selected medical records for men and women with a BPD diagnosis in the NPR. Two clinically experienced, board-certified, adult psychiatrists independently examined the records employing DSM-IV-TR (very similar to the ICD-10 research criteria) when assessing diagnosis in the records. Evidence of diagnosis was considered to be present if (a) at least five of the nine criteria in BPD were endorsed in the record or whether a diagnostic instrument (SCID-II) had been employed to establish a diagnosis of BPD, or (b) the clinicians held an expert opinion that a BPD diagnosis was the most likely explanation for the patients symptoms given the information available. We received 82 of the requested records, and 70 had sufficient information to assess diagnosis. The agreement for endorsed diagnosis between examiners was good (93%; 95% confidence interval [95% CI], 84–98) as was the inter-rater reliability for number of endorsed criteria (Cohens kappa [36], 0.82; 95% CI, 0.74–0.90). Disagreements on endorsing diagnosis (5 records) were solved by using the least favorable rater (i.e., if one rated as “not BPD” then her rating had precedence). According to (a) 44 diagnoses could be confirmed, with the addition of (b) this number increased to 57 records, corresponding to positive predictive values (PPV) of 63% (95% CI = 50–74) and 81% (95% CI = 70–90).
Sex, birth year, and birth order are collectively referred to as the covariates below—included since they are potential confounders, associated with both ADHD and BPD. Birth year was included as a covariate to adjust for the secular changes affecting diagnostic practices through the follow-up period; for instance, incidence of ADHD diagnoses has seen a sharp increase. Birth order was included because the position among siblings may affect the likelihood of getting diagnoses.
Statistical analyses
We estimated associations using logistic regression, thus viewing ADHD and BPD as binary variables. The choice of logistic regression, even though age at diagnosis is accessible to us, reflects that we do not believe the date of diagnosis accurately captures the date of disease onset. Additionally this means that analyses of temporal order of diagnoses could be misleading. To estimate appropriate associations we instead rely on a carefully selected cohort with reasonable follow-up, adjustment for birth year, and sensitivity analyses.
Co-occurrence within individuals
We analyzed the within-individual association between ADHD (exposure) and BPD (outcome) to obtain crude and covariate-adjusted odds ratios (OR and aOR). Note though that the choice of which of diagnoses to view as exposure and outcome is arbitrary in the current design. We calculated proportions of BPD among those with and without ADHD in the total cohort and separately by sex (crudely and standardized over covariates) [37], and the corresponding proportions of ADHD in BPD.
Co-aggregation within relatives
We estimated the risk of BPD (outcome) in individuals according to their relatives’ ADHD (exposure): crude and adjusted for covariates, including covariates for the relative. Similarly as in the within-individual analyses the choice of exposure and outcome is arbitrary. Comparing estimates across relatives informs about the role of genetic and/or environmental risk factors shared by the disorders. If the association between monozygotic twins is weaker than the within-individual association, then factors not shared by family members are likely to influence the co-occurrence. If the association between full siblings is weaker than between monozygotic twins, and the association between maternal half-siblings is weaker than between full siblings, genetic factors are likely to influence the association. Maternal half-siblings have similar intra-uterine environment, and usually grow up in similar environments since children predominantly stay with the mother following parental separation [38, 39]. Hence, a stronger association in maternal than paternal half-siblings suggest shared environment.
Further, we calculated ORs in the full sibling subsample stratified by sex combinations, and calculated proportions with BPD diagnosis. We calculated similar proportions using ADHD as outcome and BPD as exposure.
All analyses were performed in R, adjusting the precision of estimates for dependencies within family clusters using cluster-robust sandwich estimators, employing the packages drgee [40] and stdReg [37].
Sensitivity analyses
Alternative diagnostic definitions
The main cohort covered many birth years to provide reasonable power for both the within individual and the relative analyses, which introduced a potential risk of bias due to both left truncation (i.e., we started following some birth cohorts later in life) and right censoring (i.e., individuals were not followed until death). To investigate if results were influenced by this we identified a sub-cohort where BPD was defined as present if an individual was eligible to get a diagnosis through high-risk age period from age 18 until at least age 23, taking migration and deaths into account, and was diagnosed with BPD at any time (including before age 18 and after age 23). If the individual did not have an uninterrupted period with possibility to get a diagnosis between ages 18 and 23—due to end of follow-up, not being alive, or not living in Sweden—we defined BPD as missing; else we defined it as “no BPD”. We defined ADHD correspondingly, with safeguarded observation from age 7 until age 12. Using these definitions, we investigated the robustness of associations.
Test of familial factors
Between full siblings, we adjusted analyses for ADHD occurrence in the outcome individual, yielding estimates not interpretable as familial risks since the adjustment introduces bias. However, this analysis helps determine whether a shared familial cause (e.g., genetic) for an association likely exists, regardless of if one disease causes the other directly. If the association remains despite such adjustment, this supports the notion of a liability for both disorders shared by relatives (see, e.g., [41] for further explanation).
Results
Co-occurrence within individuals
During follow-up 82,593 (3.9%) individuals were diagnosed with ADHD, while 9544 (0.5%) were diagnosed with BPD (Table 1). Males were more frequently diagnosed with ADHD, while BPD diagnoses were more common among females. Among individuals with an ADHD diagnosis, 3.6% had also been diagnosed with BPD (2952 out of 82,593; Table 2), with a higher proportion in females (8.0%) compared to males (1.0%; Table 2). Among individuals with a BPD diagnosis, 30.9% had also been diagnosed with ADHD (2952 out of 9544), with a higher proportion in males (40.1%) compared to females (29.6%; Table 2). Calculating proportions standardized over covariates suggest that the increase was not due to a skewed age- and/or sex distribution (Supplemental Table 1a and c). Having an ADHD diagnosis increased the odds of a BPD diagnosis 11.4 times (95% CI = 10.9–11.9; Table 3), and even more so after adjustment for covariates (aOR = 19.4, 95% CI = 18.6–20.4; Table 3). In Supplemental Table 2 separate adjustment for each covariate are presented; both for within individual and relatives analyses adjustment for birth year had the largest impact on the estimates. The association was stronger in women compared to men (aOR = 19.1 vs. 21.8; p = 0.047; Table 4), but this difference did not remain statistically significant in sensitivity analysis with observation ensured in high-risk periods (p = 0.720; Supplemental Table 3).
Table 1.
Total sample N (column percent) |
ADHD N (row percent) |
BPD N (row percent) |
ADHD and BPD N (row percent) |
|
---|---|---|---|---|
Sample | 2,113,902 (100%) | 82,593 (3.9%) | 9544 (0.5%) | 2952 (0.1%) |
Covariates | ||||
Sex | ||||
Female | 1,031,987 (48.8%) | 30,571 (3.0%) | 8307 (0.8%) | 2456 (0.2%) |
Male | 1,081,915 (51.2%) | 52,022 (4.8%) | 1237 (0.1%) | 496 (<0.1%) |
Birth years | ||||
1979–1984 | 505,457 (23.9%) | 10,763 (2.1%) | 3761 (0.7%) | 1091 (0.2%) |
1985–1989 | 483,733 (22.9%) | 13,989 (2.9%) | 3601 (0.7%) | 1102 (0.2%) |
1990–1994 | 543,344 (25.7%) | 24,095 (4.4%) | 2079 (0.4%) | 711 (0.1%) |
1995–2001 | 581,368 (27.5%) | 33,746 (5.8%) | 103 (<0.1%) | 48 (<0.1%) |
Birth order by mother | ||||
1 | 874,369 (41.4%) | 35,204 (4.0%) | 4179 (0.5%) | 1316 (0.2%) |
2 | 772,164 (36.5%) | 28,406 (3.7%) | 3143 (0.4%) | 974 (0.1%) |
3 | 332,840 (15.7%) | 12,512 (3.8%) | 1505 (0.5%) | 422 (0.1%) |
4 | 94,430 (4.5%) | 4380 (4.6%) | 504 (0.5%) | 159 (0.2%) |
5 | 26,228 (1.2%) | 1398 (5.3%) | 140 (0.5%) | 57 (0.2%) |
>5 | 13,873 (0.7%) | 693 (5.0%) | 73 (0.5%) | 24 (0.2%) |
Table 2.
No. of individuals |
Proportion of BPD—outcome Percent (95% CI) |
Proportion of ADHD—outcome Percent (95% CI) |
|||
No ADHD | ADHD | No BPD | BPD | ||
Within individual | |||||
Both sexesa | 2,113,902 | 0.3% (0.3–0.3) | 3.6% (3.4–3.7) | 3.8% (3.8–3.8) | 30.9% (30.0–31.9) |
Females onlya | 1,031,987 | 0.6% (0.6–0.6) | 8.0% (7.7–8.3) | 2.7% (2.7–2.8) | 29.6% (28.6–30.5) |
Males onlya | 1,081,915 | 0.1% (0.1–0.1) | 1.0% (0.9–1.0) | 4.8% (4.7–4.8) | 40.1% (37.4–42.8) |
Full siblings | No. of pairsb | ||||
Both sexes | 2,211,396 | 0.4% (0.4–0.4) | 0.9% (0.9–1.0) | 3.4% (3.4–3.4) | 7.8% (7.2–8.4) |
Female outcome female exposure | 523,464 | 0.7% (0.7–0.7) | 1.9% (1.6–2.1) | 2.6% (2.5–2.6) | 6.6% (5.8–7.4) |
Female outcome male exposure | 552,516 | 0.7% (0.7–0.8) | 1.5% (1.3–1.7) | 2.6% (2.6–2.7) | 8.6% (6.2–11.0) |
Male outcome female exposure | 552,516 | 0.1% (0.1–0.1) | 0.3% (0.2–0.4) | 4.2% (4.2–4.3) | 8.4% (7.6–9.3) |
Male outcome male exposure | 582,900 | 0.1% (0.1–0.1) | 0.2% (0.2–0.3) | 4.1% (4.0–4.2) | 10.6% (7.9–13.3) |
Half-siblings | Pairsb | ||||
Both maternal and paternal | 663,566 | 0.9% (0.8–0.9) | 1.3% (1.2–1.3) | 7.7% (7.6–7.8) | 10.6% (9.9–11.4) |
Notes: 95% CI 95% confidence intervals
aCluster-robust standard errors based on mothers as clusters
bNumber of unique ways of combining pairs, i.e., a pair may be included twice, first with A as outcome person and B as exposure person, then with B as outcome person and A as exposure person
Table 3.
No. of individuals | Crude odds ratio (95% CI) |
Adjusted odds ratioa (95% CI) |
|
---|---|---|---|
Within individualb | 2,113,902 | 11.4 (10.9–11.9) | 19.4 (18.6–20.4) |
Relatives | No. of pairsc | ||
Monozygotic twins | 9130 | 5.0 (1.5–16.9) | 11.2 (3.0–42.2) |
Dizygotic twins | 17,350 | 0.6 (0.1–4.7) | 1.0 (0.1–7.3) |
Full siblings | 2,211,396 | 2.4 (2.2–2.6) | 2.8 (2.6–3.1) |
Maternal half-siblings | 332,486 | 1.4 (1.2–1.6) | 1.4 (1.2–1.7) |
Paternal half-siblings | 331,080 | 1.5 (1.3–1.6) | 1.5 (1.3–1.7) |
Cousins parents full siblings | 6,456,848 | 1.4 (1.3–1.5) | 1.5 (1.4–1.6) |
Cousins parents maternal half-siblings | 472,212 | 1.2 (1.0–1.4) | 1.3 (1.1–1.5) |
Cousins parents paternal half-siblings | 466,836 | 1.2 (1.0–1.4) | 1.2 (1.0–1.4) |
Notes: 95% CI 95% confidence intervals
aAdjusted for sex, sex of relative, birth year, birth year of relative, birth order, and birth order of relative, wherever applicable
bCluster-robust standard errors based on mothers as clusters
cNumber of unique ways of combining pairs, i.e., a pair may be included twice, first with A as outcome person and B as exposure person, then with B as outcome person and A as exposure person
Table 4.
No. of individuals | Crude odds ratio (95% CI) |
Adjusted odds ratioa (95% CI) |
p-Valueb | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Within femalesc | 1,031,987 | 14.9 (14.2–15.6) | 19.1 (18.2–20.1) | 0.047 |
Within malesc | 1,081,915 | 13.4 (11.9–15.0) | 21.8 (19.3–24.5) | |
Full siblings | No. of pairsd | |||
Female outcome female exposure | 523,464 | 2.7 (2.3–3.1) | 2.9 (2.6–3.4) | 0.087 |
Female outcome male exposure | 552,516 | 2.1 (1.9–2.3) | 2.5 (2.3–2.8) | |
Male outcome female exposure | 552,516 | 3.5 (2.6–4.7) | 3.9 (2.9–5.4) | 0.683 |
Male outcome male exposure | 582,900 | 2.8 (2.1–3.7) | 3.6 (2.7–4.8) |
Notes: 95% CI 95% confidence intervals
aAdjusted for birth year, birth year of relative, birth order, and birth order of relative, where applicable
bFor pairwise comparison of adjusted estimates
cCluster-robust standard errors based on mothers as clusters
dNumber of unique ways of combining pairs, i.e., a pair may be included twice, first with A as outcome person and B as exposure person, then with B as outcome person and A as exposure person
Co-aggregation within relatives
Having a relative with an ADHD diagnosis increased the odds of a BPD diagnosis (Table 3). The aOR for monozygotic twins (11.2, 95% CI = 3.0–42.2) was lower than within individuals, which indicate that factors not shared by family members are likely to influence the co-occurrence. The aOR for dizygotic twins was 1.0, with a wide CI (95% CI = 0.1–7.3)—reflecting the low power for these relatives. Among full siblings, the aOR was 2.8 (95% CI = 2.6–3.1), significantly lower compared to monozygotic twins (p = 0.041), suggesting that at least part of the association between BPD and ADHD is explained by genetic factors. The aORs were lower in both maternal (1.4, 95% CI = 1.2–1.7) and paternal half-siblings (1.5, 95% CI = 1.3–1.7) compared to full siblings (both p < 0.001), again supporting genetic contributions to the association. Further, maternal and paternal half-sibling estimates did not differ significantly from each other (p = 0.671), indicating that shared environmental effects have little or no impact on the association. Cousins whose parents were full siblings had an increased risk of having a diagnosis of BPD, aOR, 1.5 (95% CI = 1.4–1.6), while cousins whose parents were half-siblings had even lower aOR.
If etiological sex differences were present (e.g., partly different genetic factors being responsible for the overlap between the disorders in males and females) the increased risk for BPD would be expected to differ depending on sex. That is, a same-sex sibling with ADHD should increase the risk for BPD more compared to having an opposite-sex sibling with ADHD. Further, it has been suggested that females’ ADHD diagnoses possibly represent a stronger genetic load compared to males [42]. If this genetic load was also associated with BPD, siblings of females with ADHD should have an increased risk of BPD compared to siblings of males with ADHD. In full siblings, females having a sister with ADHD had an increased risk of having a BPD diagnosis compared to females having a brother with ADHD (aOR = 2.9 vs. 2.5; Table 4). Males with a sister diagnosed with ADHD also exhibited increased risk compared to males with a brother with ADHD (aOR = 3.9 vs 3.6; Table 4). However, neither of these differences were statistically significant (p = 0.087 in females and p = 0.683 in males; Table 4). Thus, etiological sex differences were not robustly supported.
Sensitivity analyses
Alternative diagnostic definitions
Analyses safeguarding follow-up during high-risk periods in ADHD and BPD revealed similar patterns of proportions of diagnoses, although with slightly higher percentages, 5.1% for ADHD and 0.7% for BPD (Supplemental Table 4). Analyses suggested ORs largely similar to those found in the main analysis, both within individuals and relatives (Supplemental Table 3). Corresponding results for proportion with diagnoses also followed the same pattern as seen with the original diagnostic definitions (Supplemental Table 1b and d).
Test of familial factors
Analyses of full siblings with additional adjustment of ADHD in the outcome person found OR significantly higher than 1 (aOR = 1.4, 95% CI = 1.2–1.5). Altogether, findings support that familial factors are partly responsible for the observed co-aggregation within relatives, and not an artifact of one disorder causing the other within individuals.
Discussion
This is the largest study of ADHD and BPD co-occurrence, in a total population of over 2.1 million individuals, and the only one exploring familial co-aggregation of clinically diagnosed ADHD and BPD. We found that individuals with a diagnosis of ADHD had a 19.4 times higher odds of BPD diagnosis than individuals not diagnosed with ADHD. Furthering a previous twin study on the relation between ADHD- and BPD-like traits [23], the pattern of familial co-aggregation of ADHD and BPD across different types of relatives indicated that genetic factors play a role in ADHD and BPD co-occurrence. Our results should contribute to improved understanding of the mechanisms underlying both ADHD and BPD [10].
Emotional dysregulation has since long been hypothesized to underlie BPD, but also suggested to be an important aspect of ADHD [43–45]. This is in line with our results indicating that the shared genetic origin of ADHD and BPD may reflect a load of emotional dysregulation processes involved in both disorders, that the processes underlying the disorders are separate but correlated, or a combination thereof [10].
Although genetic factors were implied, the estimated associations between siblings compared to within individuals indicated that not all of the association was explained by these factors. Similarly to a previous study [23], our results indicate that individually unique factors, including a potential direct (phenotypic/causal) effect of ADHD diagnosis on the probability of getting a BPD diagnosis, are likely to explain parts of the association.
We found that BPD was more common among females, while ADHD was more so among males. In contrast, the data supported no, or only minor, differences in etiological factors responsible for the observed associations between ADHD and BPD in males and females. This may suggest that differences in the co-occurrence rates between males and females are probably explained by other factors, such as detection bias or diagnostic traditions—supported by lack evidence of sex differences in prevalence when assessed by face-to-face interviews of adults in the US [46].
Limitations of this study include under-detection; the observed diagnoses are likely underestimations of the true proportion of individuals with symptom levels and impairments corresponding to diagnoses if assessed by healthcare professionals. Some individuals may have a diagnosis registered in sources not accessible by us, e.g. outpatient care prior to 2001, or just do not seek help. Under-detection would be likely to bias estimates towards null. Since ADHD and BPD are likely to suffer from different detection biases—including different likelihood of detection depending on during which ages the individuals were followed up in available registries—we performed sensitivity analyses in a subsample with better coverage in high-risk periods for both diagnoses; no major differences in results appeared (Supplemental Table 3). Another limitation, likely to bias estimates upwards, is correlated detection bias; individuals with a diagnosis, or their relatives, will be in closer contact with health care and therefore have a higher probability of receiving another diagnosis compared to individuals with similar symptom- and impairment levels without other diagnoses. Another potential source of bias is misdiagnosis, where, for instance, BPD may be misdiagnosed as ADHD by a clinician and later more correctly diagnosed as BPD, thus introducing a spurious association between the disorders. Such diagnostic issues are plausible because of overlaps in the diagnostic symptom criteria between ADHD and BPD [11]. Another limitation is that the register diagnoses used are set by clinicians without enforced standardized procedures in place. This may increase the heterogeneity of symptom profiles sharing the same diagnosis and are likely to result in less specificity of diagnoses. However, it also represents a more naturalistic result of the relation between diagnoses given in the clinical environment. For our ADHD-definition we did not include ICD-10 diagnosis F98.8—“Other specified behavioral and emotional disorders with onset usually occurring in childhood and adolescence”—which include “Attention-deficit disorder without hyperactivity” (ADD). This because of inability to discern this important sub-diagnosis from other sub-diagnoses (i.e., excessive masturbation, nail-biting, nose-picking, and thumb-sucking), as well as it being questionable if this sub-diagnosis is consistently used for ADD in concurrent clinical practice. Thus, we may be missing to identify some individuals with ADD, while avoiding incorrectly identifying individuals without ADHD problems as having ADHD diagnosis. However, in our data, only 739 individuals had F98.8 without ADHD according to our definition, and a post-hoc analysis including F98.8 did not change our results (Supplemental Table 5). We did not follow individuals throughout their lives, thus they were at risk of receiving a diagnosis outside the follow-up period. However, when enforcing follow-up through high-risk periods for ADHD and BPD, associations remained robust.
Important strengths of the current study are its population-based cohort, sample size, and clinically diagnosed ADHD and BPD. Although both ADHD and BPD are likely to include heterogeneous presentations, the diagnoses are likely to capture the DSM-IV-criteria accurately—the validation of the ICD-10 BPD codes showed adequate agreement (93%) and acceptable precision (PPV = 81%). A further strength is the use of population registers, where data are gathered prospectively and primarily for administrative purposes, and, importantly, totally independently from the current study.
ADHD and BPD co-occur in individuals, and co-aggregate in relatives—reflecting both shared genetic risk factors and individually unique risk factors. Although ADHD is more common in males and BPD in females, the strength of the individual and familial associations between the disorders is similar across sexes, indicating that etiologic sex differences are unlikely. Clinicians need to be aware of increased risk for BPD in individuals with a diagnosis of ADHD, as well as in their relatives, and vice versa.
Disclaimer
The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Electronic supplementary material
Acknowledgements
This research was supported by the Swedish Prison and Probation Service (Kuja-Halkola and Lind Juto).
Compliance with ethical standards
Conflict of interest
HL has served as a speaker for Eli Lilly and Shire and has received a research grant from Shire. PL has served as a speaker for Medicine, all outside the submitted work. The remaining authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Preliminary results were presented at the Behavior Genetics Annual meeting in San Diego, USA, 2015.
Electronic supplementary material
The online version of this article (10.1038/s41380-018-0248-5) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
References
- 1.Thapar A, Cooper M. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. 2016;387:1240–50. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00238-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Polanczyk GV, Willcutt EG, Salum GA, Kieling C, Rohde LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:434–42. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9:490–9. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Biederman J. Impact of comorbidity in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2004;65(Suppl. 3):3–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ghirardi L, Brikell I, Kuja-Halkola R, Freitag CM, Franke B, Asherson P et al. The familial co-aggregation of ASD and ADHD: a register-based cohort study. Mol Psychiatry. 2018;23:257–262. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 6.Friedrichs B, Igl W, Larsson H, Larsson JO. Coexisting psychiatric problems and stressful life events in adults with symptoms of ADHD—a large Swedish population-based study of twins. J Atten Disord. 2012;16:13–22. doi: 10.1177/1087054710376909. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.James LM, Taylor J. Revisiting the structure of mental disorders: borderline personality disorder and the internalizing/externalizing spectra. Br J Clin Psychol. 2008;47(Pt 4):361–80. doi: 10.1348/014466508X299691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Leichsenring F, Leibing E, Kruse J, New AS, Leweke F. Borderline personality disorder. Lancet. 2011;377:74–84. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(10)61422-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Torgersen S, Kringlen E, Cramer V. The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58:590–6. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Petrovic P, Castellanos FX. Top-down dysregulation—from ADHD to emotional instability. Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:70. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00070. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Matthies SD, Philipsen A. Common ground in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD)—review of recent findings. Bord Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. 2014;1:3. doi: 10.1186/2051-6673-1-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Fossati A, Novella L, Donati D, Donini M, Maffei C. History of childhood attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and borderline personality disorder: a controlled study. Compr Psychiatry. 2002;43:369–77. doi: 10.1053/comp.2002.34634. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Anckarsater H, Stahlberg O, Larson T, Hakansson C, Jutblad SB, Niklasson L, et al. The impact of ADHD and autism spectrum disorders on temperament, character, and personality development. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1239–44. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.7.1239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Philipsen A, Limberger MF, Lieb K, Feige B, Kleindienst N, Ebner-Priemer U, et al. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as a potentially aggravating factor in borderline personality disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2008;192:118–23. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.035782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Bernardi S, Faraone SV, Cortese S, Kerridge BT, Pallanti S, Wang S, et al. The lifetime impact of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) Psychol Med. 2012;42:875–87. doi: 10.1017/S003329171100153X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Brikell I, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H. Heritability of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2015;168:406–13. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Chang Z, Lichtenstein P, Asherson PJ, Larsson H. Developmental twin study of attention problems: high heritabilities throughout development. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:311–8. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ, Holmgren MA, et al. Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57:1313–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Larsson H, Chang Z, D’Onofrio BM, Lichtenstein P. The heritability of clinically diagnosed attention deficit hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Psychol Med. 2014;44:2223–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002493. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Faraone SV, Ghirardi L, Kuja-Halkola R, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H. The familial co-aggregation of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and intellectual disability: a register-based family study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56:167–74.e161. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kuja-Halkola R, Lichtenstein P, D’Onofrio BM, Larsson H. Codevelopment of ADHD and externalizing behavior from childhood to adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56:640–7. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12340. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Cole J, Ball HA, Martin NC, Scourfield J, McGuffin P. Genetic overlap between measures of hyperactivity/inattention and mood in children and adolescents. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48:1094–101. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b7666e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Distel MA, Carlier A, Middeldorp CM, Derom CA, Lubke GH, Boomsma DI. Borderline personality traits and adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms: a genetic analysis of comorbidity. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2011;156B:817–25. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Cnattingius S, Ericson A, Gunnarskog J, Kallen B. A quality study of a medical birth registry. Scand J Soc Med. 1990;18:143–8. doi: 10.1177/140349489001800209. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.National Board of Health and Welfare. Dödsorsaker 2013 - Causes of death 2013. Stockholm, Sweden: National Board of Health and Welfare, 2015.
- 26.Ludvigsson JF, Almqvist C, Bonamy AK, Ljung R, Michaelsson K, Neovius M, et al. Registers of the Swedish total population and their use in medical research. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31:125–36. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0117-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ekbom A. The Swedish Multi-generation Register. Methods Mol Biol. 2011;675:215–20. doi: 10.1007/978-1-59745-423-0_10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Magnusson PK, Almqvist C, Rahman I, Ganna A, Viktorin A, Walum H, et al. The Swedish Twin Registry: establishment of a biobank and other recent developments. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16:317–29. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Ludvigsson JF, Andersson E, Ekbom A, Feychting M, Kim JL, Reuterwall C, et al. External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:450. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-11-450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Wettermark B, Hammar N, Fored CM, Leimanis A, Otterblad Olausson P, Bergman U, et al. The new Swedish Prescribed Drug Register—opportunities for pharmacoepidemiological research and experience from the first six months. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2007;16:726–35. doi: 10.1002/pds.1294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.World Health Organization. International statistical classification of diseases and related health problems. 10th revision edn. Geneva: World Health Organization; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Larsson H, Ryden E, Boman M, Langstrom N, Lichtenstein P, Landen M. Risk of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia in relatives of people with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203:103–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.120808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Zetterqvist J, Asherson P, Halldner L, Langstrom N, Larsson H. Stimulant and non-stimulant attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder drug use: total population study of trends and discontinuation patterns 2006-9. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2013;128:70–7. doi: 10.1111/acps.12004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.National Board of Health and Welfare. Psychiatric codes. http://www.socialstyrelsen.se/klassificeringochkoder/diagnoskodericd-10/psykiatrikoder, Accessed Feb 1, 2018.
- 35.Ruck C, Larsson KJ, Lind K, Perez-Vigil A, Isomura K, Sariaslan A, et al. Validity and reliability of chronic tic disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder diagnoses in the Swedish National Patient Register. BMJ Open. 2015;5:e007520. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2014-007520. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Cohen J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ Psychol Meas. 1960;20:37–46. doi: 10.1177/001316446002000104. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Sjolander A. Regression standardization with the R package stdReg. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31:563–74. doi: 10.1007/s10654-016-0157-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Statistics Sweden. Children and their families. Stockholm, Sweden: Statistics Sweden, 2006.
- 39.Statistics Sweden. Different families live in different ways : a survey on residence and support of children after a separation. Stockholm, Sweden: Statistics Sweden, 2014.
- 40.Zetterqvist J, Sjolander A. Doubly robust estimation with the R Package drgee. Epidemiol Meth. 2015;4:69–86.
- 41.Yao S, Kuja-Halkola R, Thornton LM, Runfola CD, D’Onofrio BM, Almqvist C, et al. Familial liability for eating disorders and suicide attempts: evidence from a population registry in Sweden. JAMA Psychiatry. 2016;73:284–91. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Taylor MJ, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H, Anckarsater H, Greven CU, Ronald A. Is there a female protective effect against attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Evidence from two representative twin samples. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55:504–12.e502. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Merwood A, Chen W, Rijsdijk F, Skirrow C, Larsson H, Thapar A, et al. Genetic associations between the symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and emotional lability in child and adolescent twins. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53:209–20.e204. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Musser ED, Nigg JT. Emotion dysregulation across emotion systems in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2017; e-pub ahead of print 19 January 2017; 10.1080/15374416.2016.1270828.
- 45.Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014;171:276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Grant BF, Chou SP, Goldstein RB, Huang B, Stinson FS, Saha TD, et al. Prevalence, correlates, disability, and comorbidity of DSM-IV borderline personality disorder: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2008;69:533–45. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n0404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.