Abstract
The Nogo signal is involved in impairment of memory formation. We previously reported the lateral olfactory tract usher substance (LOTUS) as an endogenous antagonist of the Nogo receptor 1 that mediates the inhibition of axon growth and synapse formation. Moreover, we found that LOTUS plays an essential role in neural circuit formation and nerve regeneration. However, the effects of LOTUS on synapse formation and memory function have not been elucidated. Here, we clearly showed the involvement of LOTUS in synapse formation and memory function. The cultured hippocampal neurons derived from lotus gene knockout (LOTUS-KO) mice exhibited a decrease in synaptic density compared with those from wild-type mice. We also found decrease of dendritic spine formation in the adult hippocampus of LOTUS-KO mice. Finally, we demonstrated that LOTUS deficiency impairs memory formation in the social recognition test and the Morris water maze test, indicating that LOTUS is involved in functions of social and spatial learning and memory. These findings suggest that LOTUS affects synapse formation and memory function.
Subject terms: Neuroscience, Psychology
Introduction
Overcoming the higher brain dysfunction that causes memory disorders, such as dementia, is an important issue currently being addressed worldwide. It is well known that cognitive decline occurs with aging in various higher brain functions, including spatial memory function1. The myelin-associated inhibitors (MAI), such as Nogo-A, myelin-associated glycoprotein (MAG), and oligodendrocyte myelin glycoprotein (OMgp), are potent inhibitors of axon regrowth2–5. The Nogo receptor-1 (NgR1) has been identified as a common receptor of these MAIs and adopts a co-receptor structure with the p75 neurotrophin receptor (p75NTR) and leucine-rich repeat and immunoglobin-like domain-containing Nogo receptor-interacting protein 1 (LINGO-1) or the tumor necrosis factor receptor superfamily (TROY), thus inducing a structural change in axons and spines via actin depolymerization through the activation of the Ras homolog family member A (RhoA)6–8. Nogo signaling inhibits axon regrowth through growth cone collapse, thereby inhibiting neural regeneration in the central nervous system via NgR19. Conversely, Nogo signaling was also recently reported to reduce synaptic density in hippocampal primary cultured neurons10,11. Similarly, in the adult brain, NgR1-overexpressing mice exhibit a reduction of spine density and impairment of memory function12,13. In contrast, it was reported that the inhibition of Nogo signaling in hippocampal primary cultured neurons increased the number of synapses14. Furthermore, it has also clarified that spine density and memory function are increased in nogo gene KO mice15. Through such actions, Nogo signaling is considered to be a physiological factor that reduces memory function via the elimination of neural plasticity. Moreover, age-dependent increases in MAI and NgR1 expression has been reported in rats, which were accompanied by a decrease in memory function16,17. Recently, the leucine-rich glioma inactivated 1 (LGI1) and lateral olfactory tract (LOT) user substance (LOTUS) have been identified as antagonists of NgR118,19. LGI1 was reported to be a secreted protein expressed in the CNS that contributes to synapse formation by inhibiting Nogo signaling20–22. LGI1 deficiency causes epileptic seizures, eventually leading to death by postnatal day 21. On the other hands, we previously discovered that LOTUS is a key factor involved in the formation of LOT, which is the secondary projection pathway for olfaction, and found that LOTUS functions as an endogenous antagonist of NgR119,23–25. Although LOTUS is widely and abundantly expressed in the adult brain, its expression level in the hippocampus of rats decreases with age, and the memory function is reduced in correlation with the decrease in LOTUS expression26,27. However, the physiological role of LOTUS in synapse formation and memory function has not been clarified. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine the involvement of LOTUS in synapse formation, spine morphology, and memory function.
Results
LOTUS is distributed in the synapse region of cultured hippocampal neurons
NgR1 has been reported to be expressed at synapses and to negatively regulate synaptic morphology and density via the Nogo − NgR1 signal10,12,14,15,28,29. Here, first we examined the distribution of LOTUS in cultured hippocampal neurons using fluorescent immunostaining. We found that LOTUS was expressed in cellular regions that were co-stained with the Bassoon (a presynaptic marker) and postsynaptic density-95 (PSD-95, a postsynaptic marker), indicating that LOTUS is expressed in the synapse region of cultured hippocampal neurons (Fig. 1a − d). To assess the localization of LOTUS at the synapse, we compared LOTUS expression against that of Bassoon and PSD-95. We found that 33.5 ± 2.0% of PSD-95-positive puncta showed colocalization of PSD-95 and LOTUS, and 20.7 ± 3.1% of Bassoon-positive puncta showed colocalization of Bassoon and LOTUS (Fig. 1g). Next, we examined the localization of NgR1 and LOTUS at the post-synapse using fluorescent immunocytochemistry (Fig. 1e,f). We found that NgR1 was expressed in PSD-95-positive puncta, and that 28.6 ± 1.6% of PSD-95-positive puncta showed colocalization of LOTUS with NgR1. Moreover, 88.1 ± 6.1% of NgR1-positive, PSD-95-positive puncta showed colocalization of LOTUS with NgR1 (Fig. 1g). These findings suggest that LOTUS may be predominantly localized to the PSD-95-positive postsynapse and colocalizes with NgR1. Thus, LOTUS seems to be distributed in the synapse region of cultured hippocampal neurons.
Loss of LOTUS decreases synaptic density in cultured hippocampal neurons
To investigate the function of LOTUS in synapse formation, we measured the synaptic density via simultaneous immunostaining of the postsynaptic marker PSD-95 and the presynaptic marker Bassoon in cultured hippocampal neurons. The number of positive staining deposits was measured (Fig. 2a − d). The number of synapses was decreased in LOTUS-KO mice compared with WT mice (Fig. 2e). These data suggest that LOTUS contributes to synapse formation in cultured hippocampal neurons.
Loss of LOTUS decreases dendritic spine density in the hippocampus of adult mice
Because LOTUS-KO mice showed a decrease in synaptic density in cultured hippocampal neurons (Fig. 2), next we investigated role of LOTUS in dendritic spine morphology in the adult hippocampus. Mice in which dendritic spines can be visualized were created using Thy1-EGFP mice. Thus, the spine density of EGFP-positive dendrites was examined in the hippocampus of adult Thy1-EGFP WT and Thy1-EGFP LOTUS-KO mice. The apical dendritic spine density in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons was significantly decreased in LOTUS-KO mice compared with WT mice. In particular, the number of mushroom-type and thin-type spines was significantly decreased in LOTUS-KO mice. However, no difference was observed in the number of stubby-type spines in these mice (Fig. 3a,b). Similarly, the same measurement was performed in basal dendrite, which yielded similar results to those obtained for apical dendrites (Fig. 3c,d). These results indicate that loss of LOTUS reduces the number of thin and mushroom-type spines in hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons, as well as the total spine density, suggesting that LOTUS may contribute to hippocampal synapse formation.
Loss of LOTUS impairs hippocampus-dependent memory formation
To investigate whether LOTUS deficiency affects learning and memory, we first evaluated the ability of social cognitive memory formation in WT and LOTUS-KO mice. The social recognition test is a behavioral analysis that evaluates hippocampal-dependent social cognitive memory formation in mice. In this experiment, a mature test mouse encounters a juvenile mouse as a stranger for 3 min; 24 h later, it is determined whether the test mouse remembers the juvenile mouse. A significant reduction in the investigation time was observed in WT mice (n = 9), while no significant decrease was observed in LOTUS-KO mice (n = 10) (Fig. 4a). Furthermore, the results of the recognition index, which indicates the ratio of the social investigation times during the Day2 and Day1, meant that LOTUS-KO mice showed a significantly worse recognition compared with WT mice (Fig. 4b), suggesting that LOTUS-KO mice have impaired memory compared with WT mice. Thus, these data show that loss of LOTUS impairs social-recognition-related memory. Next, to evaluate the ability of spatial learning and memory, we performed the Morris water maze test, which is a behavioral analysis that evaluates hippocampal-dependent spatial memory formation in mice (Fig. 5a). In 6 days training, the escape latency was significantly longer in LOTUS-KO mice (n = 15) compared with WT mice (n = 13; two-way repeated-measures ANOVA). In addition, LOTUS-KO mice had significantly longer escape latency at Day 2 and Day 4 compared to WT mice (Student’s unpaired t-test, Fig. 5b). In test 1, after training twice a day for 3 days, the WT and LOTUS-KO mice did not show any difference in the staying time in the target quadrant (TQ) (by χ2 test, Fig. 5c), where the platform was set. In test 2, after training for 6 days, the staying time in the TQ was significantly increased in WT mice, whereas no difference was observed in LOTUS-KO mice (by χ2 test, Fig. 5d). Furthermore, the ratio of time spent in the TQ during the probe test was not significantly different between WT and LOTUS-KO mice in Test 1, whereas a significant reduction in this ratio was observed in LOTUS-KO mice in Test 2 compared with WT mice (Fig. 5e). No difference was observed in the body weight and swimming speed of LOTUS-KO mice compared with WT mice (Fig. S2a,b). These results suggest that LOTUS-KO mice show an impairment of spatial learning and memory. Taken together, the results imply that loss of LOTUS impairs the ability to form hippocampus-dependent memory, such as social cognitive and spatial memories, and that LOTUS may be required for memory formation.
Discussion
Nogo signaling inhibits neurite outgrowth with growth cone collapse, thereby inhibiting axonal regeneration after injury in the central nervous system via NgR12,3,6,9. MAIs such as Nogo-A, MAG, and OMgp are potent inhibitors of axon regrowth as ligands of NgR14,5. NgR1 adopts a co-receptor structure with p75NTR and LINGO-1 or TROY and induces structural changes in the cytoskeleton through activation of RhoA7,8. In this context, it was reported that Nogo signaling reduces synaptic density in the hippocampus14,15. Furthermore, it was also reported that the β-amyloid protein (Aβ), which is believed to be a causal protein of Alzheimer’s disease, also binds to NgR1 and reduces synaptic density, thereby inducing defective memory function30. In each case, NgR1 leads to the inhibition of synapse formation via the RhoA − ROCK signal. As an NgR1 antagonist, LGI1 has been reported to contribute to synapse formation by inhibiting Nogo signaling18,20–22. Therefore, we hypothesized that LOTUS, an endogenous antagonist of NgR1, is also involved in synapse formation and memory formation. To address this issue, we first examined the location of LOTUS expression in cultured hippocampal neurons using fluorescent immunocytochemistry. The data showed that LOTUS was distributed along dendrites and in synapse regions of hippocampal neurons. It has been reported that inhibition of NgR1 by shRNA increased synaptic density in cultured hippocampal neurons14. Moreover, the application of Nogo to cultured hippocampal neurons has been reported to reduce synaptic density10,11. In the present study we observed LOTUS co-localizing with NgR1 at PSD-95-positive synaptic sites, indicating that the interaction between NgR1 and LOTUS in the synapses of hippocampal neurons may suppress Nogo signaling and affect synapse formation. This idea is supported by data showing that the loss of LOTUS decreases synapse density in cultured hippocampal neurons and decreases dendritic spine density in the adult hippocampus.
Interestingly, LOTUS-KO mice exhibited a decreased density of thin- and mushroom-type spines compared with WT mice. These data suggest that LOTUS may influence synaptic morphology. As Nogo signaling is known to regulate actin dynamics7, it is possible that LOTUS plays a role in maintaining the synaptic actin assembly, supporting synaptic maturation and morphology.
It has also been reported that suppression of Nogo signaling increases hippocampal-dependent long-term memory function, and that enhancement of Nogo signaling decreases memory formation12,15. Based on these findings, we evaluated hippocampal-dependent long-term memory formation in LOTUS-KO mice; we found that social cognitive memory and spatial learning and memory were impaired in the absence of LOTUS in these animals, suggesting that LOTUS may contribute to hippocampus-dependent memory formation. Future studies of activity-dependent dendritic spine dynamics in LOTUS-KO mice are required to fully understand the contribution of LOTUS to synaptic plasticity.
It has been reported that the increase in Nogo signal16,17,27 or the decrease of LOTUS expression26 in the hippocampus according to aging causes memory impairment. Both processes may have a synergistic effect on age-dependent memory impairment. It would be interesting to examine whether LOTUS overexpression or the blockade of the decrease in LOTUS expression suppress the age-dependent memory impairment. Conversely, NgR1 has been reported as a receptor of the Aβ protein, a causative protein of Alzheimer’s disease; moreover, Aβ binding to NgR1 suppresses synapse formation, and Aβ action through NgR1 may affect synaptic plasticity and cause memory impairment eventually30,31. Recently, we found that LOTUS also binds to the paired immunoglobulin-like receptor B (PirB) and suppresses Nogo-induced PirB function32. PirB also acts as an Aβ receptor, and Aβ binding to PirB impairs memory function33. Therefore, whether LOTUS is involved in a regulatory function in the binding of Aβ to NgR1 or PirB is a fascinating subject for future research. Further investigations are required to identify the effective functions of LOTUS in the development of preventive and therapeutic approaches for senile amnesia and Alzheimer’s disease.
Methods
Animals
C57BL/6 J mice were purchased from Charles River Co. (Japan, Inc.), and the lotus/crtac1b gene knockout mice (Acc. No. CDB0599K,(http://www.cdb.riken.jp/arg/mutant%20mice%20list.html) were generated as previously described19 (http://www.cdb.riken.jp/arg/Methods.html). The heterozygous Thy1-EGFP mice were maintained by crossing with wild-type (WT) C57BL/6 J mice34. The mice were housed in a standard mouse facility and were provided autoclaved diet and water. Throughout the experimental procedures, all efforts were made to minimize the number of animals used and their suffering. The experimental procedures were approved by the institutional animal care and use ethical committee of Yokohama City University and were carried out in accordance with the recommended guidelines. The lotus/crtac1b mutants were assessed on the C57BL/6 J background.
Cell culture of hippocampal neurons
The hippocampal nerve cell primary culture method was partially modified from the original protocol35. The hippocampus was excised from embryos (E17.5) of WT and LOTUS-KO mice. Pregnant mice of each genotype were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane (Pfizer) and the embryos were removed. The hippocampus was dissected and dispersed using 0.25% trypsin and 100 µg/ml DNase at 37 °C for 12 min. Dispersed cells were immersed in a 24-well dish (Greiner Bio-One). The glass cover slips (φ, 12 mm; Matsunami) were coated with 10 μg/ml of polyethyleneimine and 10 μg/ml of laminin, and the surface was seeded with 0.5 × 105 cells/dish. Neurobasal medium (Gibco) containing 10% fetal bovine serum (Biowest) was used as the plating medium, and 1 × B-27 (Gibco), 1 × Glutamax (Gibco), and Neurobasal medium (Gibco) were used as the culture medium.
Immunohistochemistry
After washing with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), the hippocampal primary cultured neurons (DIV 14 days) were fixed with methanol at − 20 °C for 8 min, then washed with PBS and treated with 1% bovine serum albumin (BSA) (Nacalai Tesque) in PBS for 20 min. The primary antibodies (monoclonal hamster antibodies against LOTUS (H24G11-MAB) at a dilution of 5 µg/ml (Fig. 1c,d) or 1 µg/ml (Fig. 1a,b,e,f) and goat anti-NgR1 (1/500; R&D) were applied before fixation, as described previously19, followed by cell fixation with 4% paraformaldehyde at 37 °C for 10 min and at RT for 10 min. Mouse anti-PSD-95 (1/1000; Invitrogen), rabbit anti-Bassoon (1/1000; Synaptic systems), and chicken anti-MAP2 (1/1000; Abcam) antibodies diluted with 1% BSA/PBS were used as primary antibodies. Cells were incubated with the primary antibody for 1 h. After washing with PBS, Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG (1/2000; Invitrogen) diluted with 1% BSA/PBS, Alexa Fluor 488-conjugated donkey anti-goat IgG (1/1000; Invitrogen), Alexa Fluor 532-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1/100; Invitrogen) (Fig. 1c), Alexa Fluor 532-conjugated goat anti-mouse IgG (1/100; Invitrogen) (Fig. 1d), Alexa Fluor 568-conjugated goat anti-hamster IgG (1/100; Invitrogen) (Fig. 1c − d), Alexa Fluor 594-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1/2000; Invitrogen), Alexa Fluor 594-conjugated sheep anti-hamster IgG (1/1000), Alexa Fluor 647-conjugated donkey anti-mouse IgG (1/2000) and Alexa Fluor 647-conjugated goat anti-chicken IgY (1/2000; Invitrogen) were used as the secondary antibodies and incubated with the cells at room temperature for 1 h. Subsequently, the cells were washed with PBS and mounted with cover slips using Fluoromount (CosmoBio). Fluorescence images were obtained using a TCS SP8 microscope (Leica) equipped with a 100 × oil-immersion objective lens (NA, 1.4) and LAS X software (Leica). Super-resolution images were acquired using STED mode; confocal images were acquired using normal confocal mode. All images were captured at a resolution of 1024 × 1024 pixels with a z-step of 0.5 µm (Fig. 1a–f). The analysis for synaptic marker quantification was performed with Leica LAS X and analyzed on flattened Z-projections. All captured images were subjected to a luminance histogram threshold with LAS X (luminance for PSD-95: 30–150; Bassoon: 30–150; LOTUS: 50–120; NgR1: 30–120). The positive clusters with PSD-95 and LOTUS colocalization, Bassoon and LOTUS colocalization, NgR1, LOTUS and PSD-95 colocalization, and PSD-95 and NgR1 colocalization within 40 µm from the branch point close to the cell body were counted, respectively.
Analysis of synapse density in cultured hippocampal neurons
All fluorescence immunostaining images were acquired using a confocal microscope (TCS SP8; Leica) and the LasX software (Leica). The segment was imaged at 1–3 × magnification. All images were taken by using a resolution of 1024 × 1024 pixels with a z-step of 0.5 µm. Independently observable immunostaining with anti-PSD-95, anti-Bassoon, and anti-MAP2 antibodies was examined to identify the synapse sites (Fig. 2a,b). All captured images were subjected to a luminance histogram threshold with LAS X (luminance for PSD-95: 30–150; Bassoon: 30–150; MAP2: 10–200). The positive clusters with PSD-95 and Bassoon colocalization within 40 µm from the branch point close to the cell body of each MAP2-positive dendrite were counted, and the positive deposits within 10 µm of the proximal dendrite were measured.
Analysis of dendritic spine density in the adult hippocampus
Mice (male, 2 months old) were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane (Pfizer) and perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde. The brain was then removed and fixed overnight in the same fixative. Subsequently, the fixed brain was immersed in 30% sucrose and stored at − 80 °C. Coronal Sects. (30 µm) were prepared using a cryostat. Fluorescence images were acquired using a confocal microscope (TCS SP8; Leica) equipped with a 63 × (NA, 1.4) oil-immersion objective and the LAS X software (Leica). Images were captured at a resolution of 512 × 512 pixels with a z-step of 0.5 µm. The confocal stack was semi-automatically analyzed with the Neuron Studio software36 (http://research.mssm.edu/cnic/). Spine density was calculated as the number of spines divided by the length of the dendrite segment. Stubby spines were identified by a head-to-neck diameter ratio less than 1.1. Thin spines were determined by a head-to-neck diameter ratio greater than 1.1 and a maximum head diameter less than 0.35 µm. Mushroom spines were determined by a head-to-neck diameter ratio greater than 1.1 and a maximum head diameter greater than 0.35 µm. Spine density was quantified on the first branching site of apical or basal dendrites from hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons.
Behavioral tests
Before performing behavioral analysis, 3 min handling was performed for 5 days. The social recognition test and the Morris water maze test were performed using different mice, as described below.
Social recognition test
The social recognition test is a behavioral analysis that measures social cognitive memory, which is a hippocampal-dependent type of memory37,38. First, as a training session, juvenile mice (male, 2 − 3 weeks old) were used as strangers who had never met the test mature test mouse (male, 2 months old); the test mouse encountered the juvenile mouse for 3 min, during which the contact between the two mice via sniffing was measured as the time required for individual recognition. Twenty-four hours later, the investigation time was measured again in the same combination of mature and juvenile mice, as a test. When the investigation time at the time of testing was significantly decreased compared with that at the time of training, we considered that the mature test mouse remembered and recognized the juvenile mouse.
Morris water maze test
The Morris water maze test is used to examine whether a test mouse undergoes spatial learning39. The mice (male, 2 months old) were trained with two trials per day at an interval of 1 min for 6 days. The mice were trained at approximately the same time every day. In the probe test, at 24 h after training on days 3 and 6, the platform was removed, and the mice were allowed to swim for 1 min. The time spent in each quadrant (opposite [OP], adjacent right [AR], target quadrant [TQ], and adjacent left [AL]) was measured and compared.
Statistical analysis
The J-STAT software was used for statistical analysis. All data are expressed as the mean ± standard error. The colocalization ratio was analyzed by one-way ANOVA post hoc Tukey–Kramer (Fig. 1g). The escape latency of the Morris water maze was analyzed by two-way repeated ANOVA (Fig. 5b). The staying time in the TQ of the Morris water maze was analyzed by χ2 test and one-way ANOVA with post-hoc Steel − Dwass test (Fig. 5c,d). Differences were considered significant at P < 0.05.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by grants-in-aid from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sport, Science and Technology of Japan (to S.K. and K.T.). The authors thank Dr. Daiki Miura for technical advice regarding behavioral test and Ms. Eriko Saito for general technical assistance.
Author contributions
K.T. and R.N. originally designed the study, and K.T. and S.K. supervised the experiments, data analysis, and interpretation. R.N., Y.K., J.M. and R.I. carried out experiments and data analysis. R.N. and K.T. wrote the paper. K.T. had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-84106-y.
References
- 1.Hedden T, Gabrieli JD. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2004;5:87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cafferty WB, Strittmatter SM. The nogo–nogo receptor pathway limits a spectrum of adult cns axonal growth. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:12242–12250. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3827-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Akbik F, Cafferty WB, Strittmatter SM. Myelin associated inhibitors: a link between injury-induced and experience- dependent plasticity. Exp. Neurol. 2012;235:43–52. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.06.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Zhang L, et al. Identification of blys (b lymphocyte stimulator), a non-myelin-associated protein, as a functional ligand for nogo-66 receptor. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:6348–6352. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5040-08.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Wang KC, et al. Oligodendrocyte-myelin glycoprotein is a nogo receptor ligand that inhibits neurite outgrowth. Nature. 2002;417:941–944. doi: 10.1038/nature00867. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Fournier AE, GrandPre T, Strittmatter SM. Identification of a receptor mediating nogo-66 inhibition of axonal regeneration. Nature. 2001;409:341–346. doi: 10.1038/35053072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Yiu G, He Z. Glial inhibition of cns axon regeneration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2006;7:617–627. doi: 10.1038/nrn1956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Yamashita T, Tohyama M. The p75 receptor acts as a displacement factor that releases rho from rho-gdi. Nat. Neurosci. 2003;6:461–467. doi: 10.1038/nn1045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Akbik FV, Bhagat SM, Patel PR, Cafferty WB, Strittmatter SM. Anatomical plasticity of adult brain is titrated by nogo receptor 1. Neuron. 2013;77:859–866. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.An H, et al. Soluble lilra3 promotes neurite outgrowth and synapses formation through a high-affinity interaction with nogo 66. J. Cell Sci. 2016;129:1198–1209. doi: 10.1242/jcs.182006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Raiker SJ, et al. Oligodendrocyte-myelin glycoprotein and nogo negatively regulate activity-dependent synaptic plasticity. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:12432–12445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0895-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Karlsson TE, et al. Ngr1: a tunable sensor regulating memory formation, synaptic, and dendritic plasticity. Cereb. Cortex. 2016;26:1804–1817. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Karlén A, et al. Nogo receptor 1 regulates formation of lasting memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:20476–20481. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905390106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wills ZP, et al. The nogo receptor family restricts synapse number in the developing hippocampus. Neuron. 2012;73:466–481. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Zagrebelsky M, et al. Nogo-a regulates spatial learning as well as memory formation and modulates structural plasticity in the adult mouse hippocampus. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2017;138:154–163. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.VanGuilder HD, et al. Hippocampal dysregulation of synaptic plasticity-associated proteins with age-related cognitive decline. Neurobiol. Dis. 2011;43:201–212. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2011.03.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.VanGuilder HD, Bixler GV, Sonntag WE, Freeman WM. Hippocampal expression of myelin-associated inhibitors is induced with age-related cognitive decline and correlates with deficits of spatial learning and memory. J. Neurochem. 2012;121:77–98. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2012.07671.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Senechal KR, Thaller C, Noebels JL. Adpeaf mutations reduce levels of secreted lgi1, a putative tumor suppressor protein linked to epilepsy. Hum. Mol. Genet. 2005;14:1613–1620. doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddi169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Sato Y, et al. Cartilage acidic protein–1b (lotus), an endogenous nogo receptor antagonist for axon tract formation. Science. 2011;333:769–773. doi: 10.1126/science.1204144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Thomas RA, et al. The nogo receptor ligand lgi1 regulates synapse number and synaptic activity in hippocampal and cortical neurons. eNeuro. 2018 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0185-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Thomas R, et al. Lgi1 is a nogo receptor 1 ligand that antagonizes myelin-based growth inhibition. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:6607–6612. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5147-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Fukata Y, et al. Disruption of lgi1–linked synaptic complex causes abnormal synaptic transmission and epilepsy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107:3799–3804. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0914537107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Kurihara Y, et al. The carboxyl-terminal region of crtac1b/lotus acts as a functional domain in endogenous antagonism to nogo receptor-1. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 2012;418:390–395. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2012.01.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kurihara Y, et al. Lotus suppresses axon growth inhibition by blocking interaction between nogo receptor-1 and all four types of its ligand. Mol. Cell. Neurosci. 2014;61:211–218. doi: 10.1016/j.mcn.2014.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Iketani M, et al. Axonal branching in lateral olfactory tract is promoted by nogo signaling. Sci. Rep. 2016;6:1–9. doi: 10.1038/srep39586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Starkey HDV, Bixler GV, Sonntag WE, Freeman WM. Expression of ngr1-antagonizing proteins decreases with aging and cognitive decline in rat hippocampus. Cell. Mol. Neurobiol. 2013;33:483–488. doi: 10.1007/s10571-013-9929-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.VanGuilder Starkey HD, Sonntag WE, Freeman WM. Increased hippocampal ngr1 signaling machinery in aged rats with deficits of spatial cognition. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2013;37:1643–1658. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Wang X, et al. Localization of nogo-a and nogo-66 receptor proteins at sites of axon–myelin and synaptic contact. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:5505–5515. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-13-05505.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lee H, et al. Synaptic function for the nogo-66 receptor ngr1: regulation of dendritic spine morphology and activity- dependent synaptic strength. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:2753–2765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5586-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zhao Y, et al. Amyloid beta peptides block new synapse assembly by nogo receptor-mediated inhibition of t-type calcium channels. Neuron. 2017;96:355–372. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.09.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Smith LM, Kostylev MA, Lee S, Strittmatter SM. Systematic and standardized comparison of reported amyloid-β receptors for sufficiency, affinity, and alzheimer’s disease relevance. J. Biol. Chem. 2019;294:6042–6053. doi: 10.1074/jbc.RA118.006252. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Kurihara Y, Takai T, Takei K. Nogo receptor antagonist lotus exerts suppression on axonal growth-inhibiting receptor pir-b. J. Neurochem. 2020 doi: 10.1111/jnc.15013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kim T, et al. Human lilrb2 is a β -amyloid receptor and its murine homolog pirb regulates synaptic plasticity in an alzheimer’s model. Science. 2013;341:1399–1404. doi: 10.1126/science.1242077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Feng G, et al. Imaging neuronal subsets in transgenic mice expressing multiple spectral variants of gfp. Neuron. 2000;28:41–51. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00084-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Banker GA, Cowan WM. Rat hippocampal neurons in dispersed cell culture. Brain Res. 1977;126:397–425. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(77)90594-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Rodriguez A, Ehlenberger DB, Dickstein DL, Hof PR, Wearne SL. Automated three-dimensional detection and shape classification of dendritic spines from fluorescence microscopy images. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e1997. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001997. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Silva AJ. Long-term memory underlying hippocampus-dependent social recognition in mice. Hippocampus. 2000;10:47–56. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(2000)10:1<47::AID-HIPO5>3.0.CO;2-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Ishikawa R, Kim R, Namba S, Uchino S, Kida S. Time-dependent enhancement of hippocampus-dependent memory after treatment with memantine: Implications for enhanced hippocampal adult neurogenesis. Hippocampus. 2014;24:784–793. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Morris RG. Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learn. Motiv. 1981;12:239–260. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90020-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.