Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Pharmacol. 2021 Apr 1;32(2-#x000263):96–111. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000590

Table 1.

Rodent models of empathy, along with species tested, main behavioral findings, as well as any other manipulations performed and neurobiology discovered with each model.

Model of Empathic Behavior Species Studied Outcome
Emotional Contagion - Behaviors
Church et al., 1959 rats Attenuation of operant behavior once it became associated with aversive shock to a conspecific
Langford et al., 2006 mice Potentiation of pain-related behaviors tested in the presence of a familiar conspecific that also received painful stimulus
Smith et al., 2016 mice Hyperalgesia observed in a “bystander” animal tested in the same environment as a conspecific that received painful stimulus, even without the shared experience of the stimulus
Carnevali et al., 2017 rats
Li et al., 2018 rats Prosocial behaviors (allo-licking and -grooming) made by “bystander” directed at conspecific that received noxious stimulus
Du et al., 2019 rats
Li et al., 2014 rats Expression of emotional contagion may be modulated by circulating glucocorticoid levels, depending on the type/severity of noxious stimuli used
Martin et al., 2015 mice
Laviola et al., 2017 mice Low levels of emotional contagion (paw licking) correlated with potentiated oxytocin and vasopressin levels in behaviorally-relevant brain areas absent any changes in receptor density. Low emotional contagion also associated with impaired reactivity to external stressors
Emotional Contagion – Brain Areas Associated
Li et al., 2014 rats Prefrontal cortex (PFC)
Knapska et al., 2006 rats PFC and amygdala
Zaniboni et al., 2018 mice Insula
Observational Fear Learning - Behaviors
Bredy and Barad, 2009 mice Context-dependent modulation of the social transmission of fear learning to an observer from a conspecific
Guzman et al., 2009 mice
Bruchy et al., 2010 rats
Knapska et al., 2010 rats
Atsak et al., 2011 rats
Jeon and Shin, 2011 mice
Sanders et al., 2013 mice
Jeon et al., 2010 mice Vicarious freezing behavior due to affective transfer to observer from conspecific receiving footshocks
Jeon and Shin, 2011 mice
Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2020 rats Instrumental harm aversion tested when observers became directly responsible for the footshock of the conspecific. This behavior was shown to be subject to effort
Guzman et al., 2009 mice Social buffering, both passive and active, modulated observational fear learning in observer. This phenomenon is dependent on the level of familiarity with the fearful conspecific
Watanabe 2011; 2015 mice
Kiyokawa et al., 2014 rats
Pisansky et al., 2017 mice Oxytocin administration, or chemogenetic activation of oxytocin neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), causes enhanced sensitivity to distress of the conspecific
Guzman et al., 2014 mice Oxytocin infusion into Lateral septum (LS) accelerated social buffering of observational fear
Observational Fear Learning - Brain Areas Associated
Pisansky et al., 2017 mice PVN
Guzman et al., 2014 mice LS
Jeon et al., 2010 mice Anterior cingulate cortex
Carrillo et al., 2019 rats
Restraint Stress Model - Behaviors
Bartal et al., 2011 rats Animals learn to release a conspecific from a restraint tube
Ueno et al., 2019a mice
Bartal et al., 2014 rats Release of a conspecific is modulated by social experience and familiarity
Ueno et al., 2019b mice Underlying motivation for release behavior in mice is less clear and may be driven by social interest and curiosity with the apparatus
Blystad et al., 2019 rats Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on contents of the container
Silberberg et al., 2014 rats Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on social contact
Carvalheiro et al., 2019 rats Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on alternative choices afforded to the observer
Bartal et al., 2011 rats
Bartal et al., 2016 rats Midazolam administered to observers before the session reduced door openings
Restraint Stress Model – Brain Areas Associated
Tomek et al., 2020 rats Insula
Soaked Conspecific Model - Behaviors
Sato et al., 2015 rats Animals learn to release a “soaked conspecific” into a dry chamber. Release behavior specific to presence of conspecific, and previous experience in the distressing condition potentiates the helping behavior
Cox and Reichel, 2019 rats
Mild stress (footshock) reduces latency to release distressed conspecific
Karakilic et al., 2018 rats
Yuksel et al., 2019 mice 8 weeks of voluntary wheel running improved door opening latency in females
Yamagishi et al., 2019 rats Oxytocin injections enhanced early acquisition of helping behavior in single-housed, but not pair-housed rats
Ueno et al., 2019b mice Oxytocin had no effect on door opening
Kandis et al., 2018 rats Dose-response relationship observed between acetaminophen concentration administered and reduction in door opening latency
Targeted Helping Independent of Social Interaction-Behaviors
Cox and Reichel, 2019 rats Rats will release distressed conspecific from a pool of water independent of social interaction. Release behavior is specific to a distressed conspecific, modulated by previous experience, and is dependent on effort