Emotional Contagion - Behaviors |
Church et al., 1959 |
rats |
Attenuation of operant behavior once it became associated with aversive shock to a conspecific |
Langford et al., 2006 |
mice |
Potentiation of pain-related behaviors tested in the presence of a familiar conspecific that also received painful stimulus |
Smith et al., 2016 |
mice |
Hyperalgesia observed in a “bystander” animal tested in the same environment as a conspecific that received painful stimulus, even without the shared experience of the stimulus |
Carnevali et al., 2017 |
rats |
Li et al., 2018 |
rats |
Prosocial behaviors (allo-licking and -grooming) made by “bystander” directed at conspecific that received noxious stimulus |
Du et al., 2019 |
rats |
Li et al., 2014 |
rats |
Expression of emotional contagion may be modulated by circulating glucocorticoid levels, depending on the type/severity of noxious stimuli used |
Martin et al., 2015 |
mice |
Laviola et al., 2017 |
mice |
Low levels of emotional contagion (paw licking) correlated with potentiated oxytocin and vasopressin levels in behaviorally-relevant brain areas absent any changes in receptor density. Low emotional contagion also associated with impaired reactivity to external stressors |
Emotional Contagion – Brain Areas Associated |
Li et al., 2014 |
rats |
Prefrontal cortex (PFC) |
Knapska et al., 2006 |
rats |
PFC and amygdala |
Zaniboni et al., 2018 |
mice |
Insula |
Observational Fear Learning - Behaviors |
Bredy and Barad, 2009 |
mice |
Context-dependent modulation of the social transmission of fear learning to an observer from a conspecific |
Guzman et al., 2009 |
mice |
Bruchy et al., 2010 |
rats |
Knapska et al., 2010 |
rats |
Atsak et al., 2011 |
rats |
Jeon and Shin, 2011 |
mice |
Sanders et al., 2013 |
mice |
Jeon et al., 2010 |
mice |
Vicarious freezing behavior due to affective transfer to observer from conspecific receiving footshocks |
Jeon and Shin, 2011 |
mice |
Hernandez-Lallement et al., 2020 |
rats |
Instrumental harm aversion tested when observers became directly responsible for the footshock of the conspecific. This behavior was shown to be subject to effort |
Guzman et al., 2009 |
mice |
Social buffering, both passive and active, modulated observational fear learning in observer. This phenomenon is dependent on the level of familiarity with the fearful conspecific |
Watanabe 2011; 2015
|
mice |
Kiyokawa et al., 2014 |
rats |
Pisansky et al., 2017 |
mice |
Oxytocin administration, or chemogenetic activation of oxytocin neurons in the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN), causes enhanced sensitivity to distress of the conspecific |
Guzman et al., 2014 |
mice |
Oxytocin infusion into Lateral septum (LS) accelerated social buffering of observational fear |
Observational Fear Learning - Brain Areas Associated |
Pisansky et al., 2017 |
mice |
PVN |
Guzman et al., 2014 |
mice |
LS |
Jeon et al., 2010 |
mice |
Anterior cingulate cortex |
Carrillo et al., 2019 |
rats |
Restraint Stress Model - Behaviors |
Bartal et al., 2011 |
rats |
Animals learn to release a conspecific from a restraint tube |
Ueno et al., 2019a |
mice |
Bartal et al., 2014 |
rats |
Release of a conspecific is modulated by social experience and familiarity |
Ueno et al., 2019b |
mice |
Underlying motivation for release behavior in mice is less clear and may be driven by social interest and curiosity with the apparatus |
Blystad et al., 2019 |
rats |
Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on contents of the container |
Silberberg et al., 2014 |
rats |
Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on social contact |
Carvalheiro et al., 2019 |
rats |
Release behavior may be modulated or dependent on alternative choices afforded to the observer |
Bartal et al., 2011 |
rats |
Bartal et al., 2016 |
rats |
Midazolam administered to observers before the session reduced door openings |
Restraint Stress Model – Brain Areas Associated |
Tomek et al., 2020 |
rats |
Insula |
Soaked Conspecific Model - Behaviors |
Sato et al., 2015 |
rats |
Animals learn to release a “soaked conspecific” into a dry chamber. Release behavior specific to presence of conspecific, and previous experience in the distressing condition potentiates the helping behavior |
Cox and Reichel, 2019 |
rats |
|
|
Mild stress (footshock) reduces latency to release distressed conspecific |
Karakilic et al., 2018 |
rats |
Yuksel et al., 2019 |
mice |
8 weeks of voluntary wheel running improved door opening latency in females |
Yamagishi et al., 2019 |
rats |
Oxytocin injections enhanced early acquisition of helping behavior in single-housed, but not pair-housed rats |
Ueno et al., 2019b |
mice |
Oxytocin had no effect on door opening |
Kandis et al., 2018 |
rats |
Dose-response relationship observed between acetaminophen concentration administered and reduction in door opening latency |
Targeted Helping Independent of Social Interaction-Behaviors |
Cox and Reichel, 2019 |
rats |
Rats will release distressed conspecific from a pool of water independent of social interaction. Release behavior is specific to a distressed conspecific, modulated by previous experience, and is dependent on effort |