Abstract
Early life stress increases one’s risk for health problems later in life, and many studies find that these effects are sex-differentiated. Here, we examined relationships between multiple sources of early life stress and adult immune function in humans across several functional assays. Adult participants provided retrospective information about their childhood (a) socioeconomic status, (b) household unpredictability, and (c) exposure to adverse experiences. Participants’ peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) were then isolated for use in functional assays of immune performance: (a) tumor cell lysis by natural killer cells, (b) phagocytosis of Escherichia coli bioparticles, and (c) mitogen-induced leukocyte proliferation and cytokine release. In men, lower childhood socioeconomic status predicted decrements in immunological performance across functional assays, along with greater spontaneous cytokine release from PBMCs. These changes co-occurred with elevations in plasma testosterone levels. Similar effects were not observed for other sources of stress, nor were they found in women (with the exception of spontaneous cytokine release). These findings provide evidence that low childhood socioeconomic status has a lasting negative impact on multiple aspects of immune function, particularly in men.
Subject terms: Evolutionary developmental biology, Risk factors, Human behaviour
Introduction
Over the last three decades, a considerable body of research has demonstrated that individuals exposed to stressors during childhood exhibit an increased risk for a number of chronic health problems later in life, including metabolic disorders1, cardiovascular disease2, mental health problems3, and susceptibility to infectious illnesses4. This propensity is found to operate independently of an individual’s adult economic circumstances5–7, suggesting that the impact of early life environments on health is long-lasting and not easily reversed. For example, one study found that exposure to childhood socioeconomic disadvantage predicted an increased risk of cardiovascular disease in a cohort of male physicians, despite their achieved adult socioeconomic status far exceeding that of the average American6.
Despite the well-established link between childhood conditions and adult health1–3,7, the myriad environmental and biological pathways through which one’s early life experiences impact adult health outcomes are only beginning to be understood. Initial clues about contributors to this relationship have surfaced in research demonstrating a link between early life adversity and the tendency to exhibit an exaggerated inflammatory response to immunological stimulation8. Individuals exposed to abuse and parental absence during childhood also tend to have higher peripheral levels of inflammatory biomarkers in adulthood, such as C-reactive protein, interleukin-6 (IL-6), and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α)9. Moreover, many have proposed that chronic inflammation plays a mediating role in the relationship between early life trauma and adult disease states (e.g., cardiometabolic diseases, psychiatric disorders2,3,10,11). Additional clues are found in research demonstrating that adults who report lower childhood socioeconomic status (SES) exhibit increased susceptibility to an experimentally-induced viral infection relative to those reporting higher childhood SES4. However, little is known about the broader immunological context in which such tendencies emerge (i.e., which specific facets of immunity are impacted by early life disadvantage) or the extent to which individual-level factors buffer or exacerbate the impact of adverse childhood environments.
One individual-level factor that is likely to moderate the relationship between early life environments and immunological function is biological sex. Indeed, although much research finds that developmental experiences can have a lasting impact on both men’s and women’s health1–3,7, many studies find these effects are sex-differentiated12–16. For example, early life exposure to infection and nutritional stress are often found to have an asymmetrically negative impact on men’s physical development and health compared to women’s14,17,18. Additionally, intervention programs aimed at improving health in the context of poverty are often disproportionately beneficial for boys compared to girls19–21. On the other hand, research also finds that certain early life stressors, such as maltreatment during childhood, more negatively impact the physical and mental health of women than men (e.g.15,16). Together, such results suggest that sex differences exist in health-related developmental plasticity, for better and for worse.
Existing explanations for sex differences in susceptibility to the health consequences of early life stress include factors such as sex differences in hormone levels (e.g., androgens12) and stronger canalization of bodily growth in women than men14. Others have proposed that sex differences in behavior play a role in this context, such as the greater tendency for men to engage in health-harming behaviors in stressful environments compared to women22. Further, research in the evolutionary sciences suggests that such differences reflect sex-differentiated energetic investments in mating effort vs. somatic development and repair23–34.
With these insights in mind, the relationship between stressful early life environments and multiple aspects of immune function were investigated in men and women. The current research was designed to extend previous work linking early life environments to adult immune function to address three key questions: (a) What are the critical features of stressful early life environments that drive the association between childhood stress and adult immune function?, (b) How do the effects of these environmental stressors manifest themselves across qualitatively distinct aspects of immunological function?, and (c) Are these patterns sex-differentiated?
To address question a, the relative impact of three related, but distinct, sources of childhood stress on immune function were examined, including: (1) resource availability (i.e., SES35), (2) unpredictability36, and (3) adverse childhood experiences (ACEs37). Isolating the unique impact that each of these environmental correlates of stress has on adult immune function will yield needed insights into the types of environmental interventions that are likely to have a lasting positive impact on adult health outcomes.
Regarding question b, much of the previous work linking childhood environments to adult immune function has focused primarily on inflammatory processes8–10. In the current research, data were collected on a variety of immunological endpoints, many of which were measured at multiple time-points or at multiple effector-to-target (E:T) cell ratios. These measures included: (1) proliferation of peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) in response to, and in the absence of, stimulation by lipopolysaccharide (LPS), phytohemagglutinin (PHA), and polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (poly (I:C)) measured at 24, 48, and 72 h post-plating; (2) natural killer (NK) cell lysis of target tumor cells at 100:1, 50:1, 25:1, and 12.5:1 E:T ratios; (3) phagocytosis of opsonized Escherichia coli (E. coli) bioparticles; and (4) PBMC release of the pro-inflammatory cytokines interleukin-1β (IL-1β), IL-6, and TNF-α in response to LPS stimulation, as well as in the absence of stimulation (i.e., spontaneous release), at 2, 24, 48, and 72 h post-plating. Additionally, plasma levels of total testosterone were also assayed to test whether diminished immune function co-occurs with increased investment in mating effort.
Lastly, to address question c, in line with the National Institute of Health’s recent policy on accounting for sex as a biological variable38, a number of steps were taken during study design, data collection, and data analysis to allow for targeted tests of sex differences in the impact of early life stress on immune function. These included recruiting equal numbers of men and women (with women all being naturally cycling and in the early follicular phase of their menstrual cycle), testing interactions between each predictor and sex in all analyses (i.e., rather than merely controlling for sex), and clearly presenting sex-differentiated results (see Table 2).
Table 2.
Dependent variable | Men | Women | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Coefficient (SE) | Coefficient (SE) | |||||
Primary model | Covariates model | Partial model | Primary model | Covariates model | Partial model | |
Phagocytosis | 0.31 (0.12)** | 0.30 (0.15)* | 0.29 (0.13)* | − 0.09 (0.12) | − 0.05 (0.14) | − 0.17 (0.14) |
Testosterone | − 0.24 (0.06)*** | − 0.22 (0.07)** | − 0.25 (0.07)*** | − 0.11 (0.13) | − 0.21 (0.14) | − 0.08 (0.15) |
NK cell cytotoxicity | ||||||
Intercept | 0.19 (0.14) | 0.09 (0.18) | 0.20 (0.11)† | − 0.17 (0.12) | − 0.34 (0.13)* | − 0.12 (0.12) |
Linear Slope | 0.39 (0.10)*** | 0.42 (0.18)* | 0.40 (0.10)*** | − 0.18 (0.12) | − 0.18 (0.14) | − 0.37 (0.13)** |
Quadratic Slope | − 0.33 (0.11)** | − 0.52 (0.18)** | − 0.36 (0.12)** | 0.11 (0.13) | 0.11 (0.18) | 0.32 (0.13)* |
No significant interactions with sex below (main effects for sexes combined reported) | ||||||
Proliferation—LPS | ||||||
Intercept | 0.19 (0.09)* | 0.10 (0.10) | 0.22 (0.10)* | |||
Slope | 0.08 (0.13) | − 0.22 (0.18) | 0.12 (0.15) | |||
Proliferation—PHA | ||||||
Intercept | 0.15 (0.06)* | 0.12 (0.08) | 0.17 (0.07)** | |||
Slope | − 0.08 (0.07) | − 0.19 (0.10) | − 0.09 (0.08) | |||
Proliferation—Poly (I:C) | ||||||
Intercept | 0.21 (0.09)* | 0.10 (0.10) | 0.22 (0.10)* | |||
Slope | − 0.01 (0.01) | − 0.41 (0.33) | − 0.02 (0.02) | |||
PBMC cytokine release | ||||||
Unstimulated | − 0.22 (0.07)** | − 0.22 (0.09)* | − 0.33 (0.09)*** | |||
Stimulated | 0.04 (0.05) | 0.03 (0.04) | 0.03 (0.05) |
Standardized beta coefficients and standard errors (SEs) shown for main effects (no significant interaction found; bottom panel) and simple slopes within each sex for interactions found between childhood socioeconomic status and sex (top panel).
NK natural killer, LPS lipopolysaccharide, PHA phytohaemagglutinin, poly (I:C) polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid, PBMC peripheral blood mononuclear cells.
†p < 0.07, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
It was hypothesized that growing up in adverse environments would have an especially negative impact on the immune function of men, with less pronounced effects found in women. Moreover, consistent with research and theory suggesting that androgens play an important role in mediating sex-differentiated patterns of immune function25–28, we predicted that this effect would co-occur with upregulation of testosterone, a key sex hormone particularly important for coordinating male reproduction39,40.
Data analysis plan
See Supplementary Information for additional details about data analysis. All models were estimated using MPlus statistical software41. All significance tests were two-tailed. Models were tested iteratively. First, relationships between childhood environmental factors (i.e., childhood SES, unpredictability, and adverse experiences), sex, and each dependent measure were tested in separate models for each environmental predictor. The results of these models are reported below as the primary analyses (see “Results” section). Next, each model containing significant predictors was tested a second time while controlling for standard covariates, including adult SES (see “Methods” section for full list). Lastly, a model for each dependent measure was tested that examined the effects of all childhood environmental factors (and their interactions with sex) on the outcome simultaneously (i.e., to obtain partial effects). These latter two models were tested to examine whether significant effects of childhood factors on the immunological outcomes were robust to controlling for the other distinct, but related environmental constructs, as well as potential covariates; results of these models can be found in the Supplementary Information. Information about whether the pattern or significance of the results of the primary analyses differed from these follow-up analyses are reported in the main text and Table 2.
For each model, the dependent measure(s) were regressed on the childhood environmental factor, sex, and the interaction between sex and the childhood environment factor. Per convention, if no significant interaction was found, main effects were reported without inclusion of the interaction term. Where significant interactions were found, direct effects of the childhood factor(s) on the dependent immunological measures within each sex (i.e., simple slopes) were examined. Sex differences at high and low levels (i.e., one standard deviation above and below the mean) of childhood factors involved in a significant interaction with sex in the initial models (i.e., regions of significance) were also probed. Given that the primary objective of the current study was to test whether the impact of childhood environmental factors on immune function differed by sex, only the simple slopes within each sex are reported in the main text. Regions of significance analysis can be found in the Supplementary Information and Figs. 1 and 2.
Given that the proliferation (time collected), NK cell cytotoxicity (E:T ratios), and cytokine release (time collected) data all contained nested structures, complex multilevel modeling (cytokine release data) and latent curve models (i.e., using structural equation modeling framework; proliferation and NK cell cytotoxicity data) were used to analyze these data. First, unconditional models for each of the dependent measures (i.e., without predictors included) were tested to assess model fit to linear change across repeated measures (see Table S1 for model fit indices). Model fit to linear change was acceptable for the proliferation data, so predictors were added to this model. For the NK cell cytotoxicity, model fit was poor to linear change, but improved to acceptable fit after the addition of a quadratic change slope. For the cytokine release data, given the large amount of missing data and non-linear change from the first to last time point that did not fit well to linear or quadratic times scores (see Figs. S1–S3 in Supplementary Information for demonstration), spontaneous cytokine release was modeled as a latent factor comprised of release of the three cytokines across time-points in the absence of stimulation (standardized factor loadings—IL-6: 0.97; IL-1β: 0.94; TNF-α: 0.90), and stimulated cytokine release was modeled as a latent factor comprised of release of the three cytokines in response to LPS stimulation across time points (standardized factor loadings—IL-6: 0.94; IL-1β: 0.92; TNF-α: 0.75). The phagocytosis and total testosterone data did not contain nested structures and were thus analyzed together in a single-level, multivariate model.
Results
Results of all models revealed good model fit (see Table S1 in Supplementary Information for model fit statistics). Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 1 (sex-differentiated). Results revealed that neither childhood unpredictability, nor cumulative exposure to adverse childhood experiences were related to any of the immunological measures, testosterone levels, nor did they interact with participant sex to impact any of these outcomes (all ps > 0.10 for primary and covariates models; see Supplementary Information for these results). Note that while neither of these two childhood variables significantly predicted any immunological outcome in the primary models or models controlling for covariates, results of the partial effects model revealed significant interactions between each of these variables and sex in predicting PHA-stimulated proliferation. Further, there was a main effect of childhood unpredictability on spontaneous cytokine release by PBMCs. These results were not included in the main text as they only emerged in the partial effects model; full results can be found in the Supplementary Information. Additionally, no factors or interactions emerged as significant predictors of unstimulated PBMC proliferation or stimulated cytokine release in vitro.
Table 1.
Variable | Men (n = 80) | Women (n = 79) |
---|---|---|
M (SD) | M (SD) | |
Childhood SES | 4.55 (1.46) | 4.55 (1.50) |
Childhood unpredictability | 2.45 (1.16) | 2.29 (1.29) |
Adverse childhood experiences | 4.05 (2.79) | 2.99 (2.90) |
Phagocytosis of E. coli bioparticles (%) | 13.55 (6.40) | 11.64 (5.21) |
NK cell cytotoxicity (%) | 38.55 (13.69) | 36.59 (16.55) |
Unstimulated proliferation | 0.65 (0.14) | 0.71 (0.16) |
Proliferation in response to LPS | 1.02 (0.19) | 1.01 (0.19) |
Proliferation in response to PHA | 1.07 (0.17) | 1.10 (0.18) |
Proliferation in response to Poly (I:C) | 0.79 (0.16) | 0.82 (0.14) |
Unstimulated release of IL-1β | 73.23 (282.61) | 30.63 (60.36) |
Unstimulated release of IL-6 | 472.32 (1410.60) | 135.35 (253.64) |
Unstimulated release of TNF-α | 143.36 (215.26) | 126.31 (184.14) |
LPS-stimulated release of IL-1β | 1468.77 (1384.79) | 1361.51 (1186.40) |
LPS-stimulated release of IL-6 | 5307.71 (2415.50) | 4611.49 (2094.76) |
LPS-stimulated release of TNF-α | 2125.59 (1213.38) | 1722.65 (1018.11) |
Peripheral blood mononuclear cell (PBMC) proliferation values shown here as absorbance reading at 490 nm collapsed across time-points (24 h, 48 h, 72 h) for each plating condition. Values for natural killer cell lysis of K-562 tumor cells (i.e., NK cell cytotoxicity) shown here collapsed across effector:target cell ratios (100:1, 50:1, 25:1, 12.5:1). Levels of cytokines released from PBMCs in vitro shown here as pg/mL. Proliferation values shown as optical density values (490 nm).
SES socioeconomic status, LPS lipopolysaccharide, PHA phytohemagglutinin, poly (I:C) polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid, NK natural killer, E. coli Escherichia coli, IL-1β interleukin-1 beta, IL-6 interleukin-6, TNF-α tumor necrosis factor-alpha.
As predicted based on research indicating greater male susceptibility to adverse health effects of early life stress, results revealed significant interactions between childhood SES and sex in predicting NK cell cytotoxicity (intercept: β = − 0.54, SE = 0.28, t = − 1.97, p = 0.049; linear slope: β = − 0.97, SE = 0.26, t = -3.68, p < 0.001; quadratic slope: β = 0.72, SE = 0.30, t = 2.38, p = 0.018), phagocytosis of E. coli bioparticles, β = − 0.64, SE = 0.27, t = − 2.35, p = 0.02, and testosterone levels, β = 0.38, SE = 0.14, t = 2.73, p = 0.006.
Unpacking these interactions (see Figs. 1, 2, Table 2) revealed that, for men, higher childhood SES predicted greater phagocytosis of E. coli bioparticles, β = 0.31, SE = 0.12, t = 2.62, p = 0.009, greater NK cell cytotoxicity, particularly at the two intermediate E:T ratios (intercept: β = 0.14, SE = 0.11, t = 1.31, p = 0.19; linear slope: β = 0.39, SE = 0.10, t = 4.03, p = 0.001; quadratic slope: β = − 0.33, SE = 0.11, t = − 2.90, p = 0.004), and lower levels of testosterone, β = − 0.24, SE = 0.06, t = − 3.79, p < 0.001. For women, childhood SES did not significantly predict phagocytosis, β = − 0.09, SE = 0.12, t = − 0.22, p = 0.82, NK cell cytotoxicity (intercept: β = − 0.17, SE = 0.12, t = − 1.45, p = 0.15; linear slope: β = − 0.18, SE = 0.12, t = − 1.57, p = 0.12; quadratic slope: β = 0.11, SE = 0.13, t = 0.83, p = 0.41), or testosterone levels, β = − 0.11, SE = 0.13, t = − 0.87, p = 0.38. Notably, although only reaching statistical significance at the highest E:T ratio (see Fig. 2), the relationship between childhood SES and NK cell cytotoxicity in women was in the opposite direction as was observed in men. Specifically, in women, higher childhood SES was related to reduced NK cell cytotoxicity (see Supplementary Information for a discussion of this pattern). The pattern and significance of these results remained largely unchanged when controlling for covariates (including adult SES) or by controlling for the effects of other childhood environmental variables (see Table 2 and Supplementary Information).
In addition to these sex-differentiated results, significant main effects of childhood SES (i.e., across sexes) were found predicting the intercept of PBMC proliferation (i.e., at the first time-point of 24 h) in response to stimulation by LPS, β = 0.19, SE = 0.09, t = 2.17, p = 0.03, PHA, β = 0.15, SE = 0.06, t = 2.32, p = 0.02, and poly (I:C), β = 0.21, SE = 0.09, t = 2.44, p = 0.02, but not the slopes of proliferation over time in these conditions (ps > 0.27). These effects were no longer significant after controlling for covariates (ps > 0.12; see Supplementary Information) and should thus be interpreted with caution. Lastly, there was a main effect of childhood SES on the latent factor of spontaneous in vitro proinflammatory cytokine release by PBMCs (i.e., cells plated in media only), β = − 0.22, SE = 0.07, t = − 3.12, p = 0.002, but not stimulated cytokine release, β = 0.04, SE = 0.05, t = 0.86, p = 0.39. Specifically, higher childhood SES predicted a diminished tendency to release proinflammatory cytokines in the absence of overt immunological stimulation. These results remained significant when controlling for both covariates and the effects of the other childhood environmental factors.
To control for the false discovery rate of the tests conducted in the current research, we utilized the Benjamini and Hochberg (1995) procedure. This method involves rank ordering the significant p-values in ascending order and comparing each p-value to the product of the alpha value (here, 0.05) and the quotient of the rank divided by the total number of tests yielding a significant result (i.e., at p < 0.05). If the ranked p-value is less than the above product, the null hypothesis is rejected. This analysis revealed that all significant p-values passed this test (see Table S2 for results of analysis). Thus, the interpretation of the results is unchanged.
Discussion
See Supplementary Information for additional discussion. Considerable research indicates that early life experiences have a lasting impact on adult health1,8–10, particularly for men12–14,19–21. The current research sought to extend this work by addressing three key questions that have been heretofore unexplored in the context of a single investigation: (a) What are the critical features of early life stress that drive the association between childhood stress and adult health outcomes?, (b) How do these factors manifest themselves across qualitatively distinct aspects of immunological function?, and (c) Are these patterns sex-differentiated?
Results revealed that men exposed to lower childhood SES exhibited a diminished immune response across each measure of immunological function, as well as increased spontaneous pro-inflammatory cytokine release by PBMCs, compared to men with a higher childhood SES. Conversely, with the exception of the effects of childhood SES on stimulated PBMC proliferation and spontaneous cytokine release (both effects not sex-differentiated), the measured childhood environmental factors did not significantly predict immunological function in women. Further, with the exception of the relationship between childhood SES and stimulated PBMC proliferation, significant effects were not attenuated when controlling for adult SES (or other covariates; see Supplementary Information for these results), suggesting that these differences are primarily the result of developmental processes rather than current resource availability.
The present results are consistent with a growing body of research demonstrating a negative relationship between early life adversity on adult health1,7–10, as well as research demonstrating sex differences in key immunological outcomes25,26,28. Further, consistent with research and theory suggesting that androgens play an important in mediating sex-differentiated patterns of immunological investment28,42–44, results revealed that, in men, reduced immune performance co-occurs with higher testosterone levels. The results of the current research suggest that men who grow up in conditions of low SES may be more vulnerable to poor health outcomes later in life due to diminished investment in immune function. Moreover, the current results suggest that these differences might emerge in response to differential resource access, per se, as there was little evidence of a negative impact of childhood unpredictability or other adverse experiences (e.g., abuse, neglect) on immune function in either sex. It is important to note that others have found that the relationship between early life disadvantage and markers of poorer health (e.g., chronic inflammation) are driven by alternative features of such environments, such as abuse and parental neglect9–11, and that these factors may even more negatively impact the physical and mental health of girls relative to boys in certain contexts (see e.g.15,16). Accordingly, additional research is needed to understand the conditions in which each type of developmental stress is likely to negatively impact immunological development in each sex. Future studies may find, for example, that the timing of stress exposure influences both the magnitude of decrements in immune function, as well as differences in the extent to which men and women are affected. These questions could not be answered in the current study given its cross-sectional design and reliance on retrospective accounts of stress exposure across a wide period of early life.
There are limitations of the current research that should be considered. First, although several components of immune function were measured in the current study, together they capture only a narrow range of the tremendous complexity that comprises immune function. Accordingly, future research might find that, in women, early life experiences play an important role in influencing other aspects of immune function, such as adaptive immunity. Further, the current research did not experimentally examine the proximate biological mechanisms driving the observed relationships between early life stress and adult immune function. One possibility is that glucocorticoids and other adrenal hormones (e.g., dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA)) play an important role in this context, as they have immunomodulatory properties45 and levels of these hormones are impacted by early life stress46.
It is also important to consider that the majority of the participants in the current study were healthy college students. As a consequence, the low amount of variability in the adult SES variable may have limited our ability to detect effects of participants’ current socioeconomic circumstances on the key immunological outcomes. The present sample’s exposure to certain types of early life stress may also have been lower than that of the general population. Moreover, all of measures of early life stress used in this study were retrospective. Thus, a key direction for future research will be to examine, prospectively, the effects of childhood SES and multiple types of early life adversity on adult immune function in a more diverse sample using a longitudinal design. Such research would minimize the interpretability issues that arise from cross-sectional research using retrospective accounts of early life environments, which is a weakness of the current work. Future research may also benefit from utilizing longitudinal designs to track sex differences in the impact of early life conditions on immune function across multiple stages of development.
The current research provides important evidence of sex differences in the impact of early life SES on multiple facets of immune function. Together, these findings suggest that, for men in particular, early life experiences become immunologically embedded in ways that may have a lasting impact on health. These results have important implications for interventions targeted at reducing the negative impact of early life disadvantage on health, and lay the groundwork for future research examining sex differences in relationships between childhood environments and immune function across the lifespan.
Methods
Participants
A total of 159 individuals from Texas Christian University or the surrounding community, participated in exchange for $25 or experimental research credit (80 men, 79 women; Mage = 20.17 years, SD = 2.75). Restriction criteria were: (1) participants must have no history of chronic medical problems, including mental illness, (2) must be of healthy weight (i.e., body mass index (BMI) < 30), (3) must be free from acute illness for at least two weeks prior to participation, (4) women must not be on hormonal contraceptives, (5) must be willing to abstain from steroidal and non-steroidal anti-inflammatory medications, exercise, and alcohol use for at least 2 days before participating, and (6) must fast the morning of the session. Additional characteristics of the sample are published elsewhere47. Women participated 4–7 days after the start of their most recent menstrual cycle (i.e., early follicular phase). In exchange for participation, participants were given the choice of partial course credit or a $50 gift card.
Procedure
All participants provided informed consent prior to participation, and the study was approved as compliant with ethical standards by the Texas Christian University Institutional Review Board (Approval #: 1411-117-1606). The study was conducted in accordance with the protocol approved by the review board.
On the day of their sessions, participants arrived at the laboratory at 7:30 AM, provided informed consent, and answered study compliance questions (e.g., whether or not they fasted, abstained from alcohol use, etc.). Next, for the purpose of a larger study, participants completed a series of behavioral tasks and questionnaires. Finally, participants were led into an adjoining room, in which 85 mL of blood was collected by venipuncture into heparinized (or EDTA-containing) Vacutainer tubes (Becton–Dickinson, Franklin Lakes, NJ).
Peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) were immediately isolated from whole blood using density gradient centrifugation in Ficoll Paque Plus (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO (GE Healthcare Life Sciences)). PBMCs were used in four functional immunoassays: (1) natural killer (NK) cell lysis of target tumor cells, (2) PBMC phagocytosis of fluorescent dye-labelled Escherichia coli (E. coli) bioparticles, (3) PBMC proliferation in response to, and in the absence of, mitogen/toll-like receptor stimulation, and (4) PBMC release of proinflammatory cytokines in response to, and in the absence of, mitogen stimulation.
For each assay, PBMCs were brought to the plating density appropriate for the assay in RPMI-1640 supplemented with 10% heat-inactivated fetal bovine serum (FBS), 2 mM l-glutamine, 1 mM sodium pyruvate, 100 U of penicillin/mL, 100 µg of streptomycin/mL, and 0.25 µg of amphotericin B/mL (Caisson Labs, Logan, UT). In addition, plasma was collected at the time of blood processing and frozen at − 80 °C. Additional information about each assay follows, below.
Materials
Childhood environmental factors
First, participants’ childhood socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed using a previously validated scale35,48. This measure was chosen to capture the extent to which participants felt they did or did not have access to resources during their childhood. Participants indicated their childhood SES by responding to three statements about their life before age 12 (e.g., “My family usually had enough money for things when I was growing up”) using a 7-point Likert scale (1: Strongly disagree, 7: Strongly agree). The items together yielded acceptable reliability (α = 0.81) and were formed into a mean composite, with a higher score indicating greater resource availability/a higher SES during childhood (M = 4.55, SD = 1.48).
Next, childhood unpredictability was measured with a scale used in previous research36, consisting of three statements about chaos and uncertainty within the home (e.g., “Things were often chaotic in my house”). Participants reported agreement with how well each statement described their lives before age 12 using a 7-point scale (1: Strongly disagree, 7: Strongly agree). Together, the items yielded acceptable reliability (α = 0.74) and were formed into a mean composite with a higher score indicating greater experience with unpredictability during childhood (M = 2.37, SD = 1.23).
Finally, participants’ experience with other types of adversity during childhood were also measured using the well-validated Adverse Childhood Experience International Questionnaire (ACE-IQ), developed by research teams from the World Health Organization37. This questionnaire assesses experience with adversity during childhood across five domains: abuse, family dysfunction, and peer, community, and collective violence/war. Given the nature of the present sample, which was primarily composed of college students, a truncated version of the survey was administered, removing the collective violence, sexual abuse, and secondary household abuse questions, and leaving a total of 20 items. Continuous items were recoded as binary, per convention, and all items were then summed into a composite measure of cumulative exposure to adverse experiences during childhood (M = 3.51, SD = 2.89).
NK cell cytotoxicity assay
NK cell cytotoxicity was measured using the classic 51Cr-release assay. K-562 target tumor cells (ATCC CCL-24, Manassas, VA) were grown inside T-25 flasks (ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) in RPMI-1640 growth medium supplemented with 10% FBS, 2 mM l-glutamine, 1 mM sodium pyruvate, 100 U of penicillin/mL, 100 µg of streptomycin/mL, and 0.25 µg of amphotericin B/mL (Caisson Labs, Logan, UT) and incubated at 37 °C, 5% CO2, and 100% humidity.
Target tumor cells were labeled with 1 µCi 51Cr (PerkinElmer, Waltham, MA) for one hour before plating them at a density of 1 × 104 cells/well in a 200 µL final volume into Corning V-bottom plates (Corning, Corning, NY). PBMCs were plated with target tumor cells, in triplicate, at the following effector:target (E:T) cell ratios: 100:1 (1 × 106 PBMCs/well), 50:1 (5 × 105 PBMCs/well), 25:1 (2.5 × 105 PBMCs/well), and 12.5:1 (1.25 × 105 PBMCs/well). Spontaneous/background lysis controls (i.e., target cells plated in media only, with no PBMCs) and maximal lysis controls (i.e., target cells plated in 1% Triton X-100 (Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO)) were plated in sextuplicate. Plates were incubated for four hours at 37 °C, 5% CO2, and 100% humidity.
After brief centrifugation of the V-bottom plate, supernatants were collected into glass scintillation vials and quantified 51Cr release on a CAPRAC-t gamma counter (Capintec, Inc., Ramsey, NJ). Percent maximal lysis of target tumor cells by participant NK cells was calculated at each E:T ratio by dividing 51Cr release by maximal release after subtracting spontaneous release from both values and multiplying by 100.
Phagocytosis assay
The phagocytic capability of participants’ PBMCs was assessed using fluorescent pHrodo Green E. coli BioParticles (ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA) that were opsonized using 1 mg/mL of manufacturer-provided opsonization buffer. PBMCs were plated in triplicate with E. coli bioparticles into BrandTech black, flat-bottom microplates (BrandTech Scientific, Essex, CT) at a density of 5 × 105 cells/well in a 200 µL final volume. Negative controls (bioparticles plated with media only in 200 µL volume) and positive controls (bioparticles plated with pH 4.5 Intracellular Calibration Buffer (ThermoFisher Scientific, Waltham, MA)) were plated in triplicate. Plates were incubated for two hours at 37 °C, 5% CO2, and 100% humidity before being read on a fluorescence plate reader (BMG LabTech FLUOstar Omega, Cary, NC) at FITC dye settings of 490 nm excitation/520 nm emission. Percent maximal fluorescence was computed by dividing experimental fluorescence by maximal fluorescence (i.e., positive control), after subtracting fluorescence in the negative controls from both, and multiplying by 100.
PBMC proliferation assay
PBMCs were isolated as described above, and plated into Falcon 96-well tissue culture plates (Corning, Corning, NY). PBMCs were plated in triplicate at a final density of 2.5 × 105 cells/well in a 200 µL final volume for four conditions: (1) with media only (i.e., unstimulated proliferation), (2) with 1 µg/mL of lipopolysaccharide (LPS) obtained from E. coli (serotype 026:B6, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), (3) with 5 µg/mL of phytohemagglutinin (PHA; Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), and (4) with 50 µg/mL of polyinosinic:polycytidylic acid (poly (I:C); high molecular weight; InvivoGen, San Diego, CA). Plates were incubated at 37 °C, 5% CO2, and 100% humidity.
Approximate cell density was measured at three time-points: 24, 48, and 72 h post-plating using the CellTiter 96 AQueous Non-Radioactive Cell Proliferation Assay (Promega, Madison, WI). Each plate was read on a plate reader (BMG LabTech FLUOstar Omega, Cary, NC) at 490 nm.
Proinflammatory cytokine release assay
PBMC release of proinflammatory cytokines was measured in vitro both in response to LPS stimulation, as well as in the absence of stimulation. After isolation, PBMCs were plated in triplicate at a density of 2.5 × 105 cells/well, in a 200 µL final volume. PBMCs were plated both in media only (unstimulated condition), as well as with 1 µg/mL of LPS, obtained from E. coli (serotype 026:B6, Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO), and were incubated for up to three days at 37 °C, 5% CO2, and 100% humidity. Cell culture supernatants were collected at 2, 24, 48, and 72 h post-plating, and then stored them at − 80 °C until assays were conducted.
Cell culture supernatants were later thawed and assayed in duplicate for levels of a trio of proinflammatory cytokines: interleukin-1beta (IL-1β), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and tumor necrosis factor-alpha (TNF-α) using a MILLIPLEX MAP Human Cytokine Panel magnetic bead kit (EMD Millipore Corporation, Billerica, MA), and read using a Luminex MAGPIX fluorescent detection system (Luminex, Austin, TX) and xPONENT software (Version 4.2; build: 1324; Luminex, Austin, TX). Intra-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) were 8.20% (IL-6), 6.97% (IL-1β), and 5.98% (TNF-α). Inter-assay coefficients of variation (CVs) were 17.27% (IL-6), 10.53% (IL-1β), and 11.62% (TNF-α).
Due to a freezer failure on November 5, 2016, cell culture supernatant samples were compromised for 32 participants (for additional information see47). No compromised samples were assayed, and thus data from these samples were not included in any analysis. All other biological samples from these participants (including plasma used for the total testosterone assay) were stored elsewhere and were thus unaffected by the freezer failure.
However, given this loss of data, analyses based on these cytokine data were likely underpowered and the results should be interpreted with due caution.
Total testosterone assay
Plasma samples were and assayed in duplicate for levels of total testosterone using commercially-available enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay kits (Abcam, Cambridge, UK). Plates were read on a plate reader (BMG LabTech FLUOstar Omega, Cary, NC) at a wavelength of 450 nm. The intra-assay CV was 2.48% and the inter-assay CV was 13.45%.
Covariates and alternative explanations
Several variables that may influence relationships between early life conditions, sex, and immune function were measured. These included race, age, exercise, sleep, BMI, stress, loneliness, recent illness, season, and adult SES. See Supplementary Information for more details.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
We thank Emily Corrigan, Maggie Kleiser, Adam Burgess, and Mary Eliza Baker for their research assistance with this project. We thank Marian L. Kohut for her advice with the immunological measures.
Author contributions
J.G., R.P.P.L., J.D.W., J.L.P., M.L.P., M.J.E., D.J.C. and G.W.B., assisted with sample collection and processing. G.W.B. and S.E.H. designed the study and oversaw data collection and analysis. J.G. performed data analysis. S.M., J.G., R.P.P.L., K.B., G.W.B. and S.E.H. wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors edited and revised later drafts of the manuscript prior to submission.
Funding
This research was funded by a grant from the National Science Foundation awarded to the last author (BCS #155120).
Data availability
All data reported in the manuscript are publicly available on the Open Science Framework (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/DXPZU).
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41598-021-89413-y.
References
- 1.Delpierre C, et al. The early life nutritional environment and early life stress as potential pathways towards the metabolic syndrome in mid-life? A lifecourse analysis using the 1958 British Birth cohort. BMC Public Health. 2016;16:815. doi: 10.1186/s12889-016-3484-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Murphy MO, Cohn DM, Loria AS. Developmental origins of cardiovascular disease: Impact of early life stress in humans and rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017;74:453–465. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mock SE, Arai SM. Childhood trauma and chronic illness in adulthood: Mental health and socioeconomic status as explanatory factors and buffers. Front. Psychol. 2011;1:246. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cohen S, Doyle WJ, Turner RB, Alper CM, Skoner DP. Childhood socioeconomic status and host resistance to infectious illness in adulthood. Psychosom. Med. 2004;66:553–558. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000126200.05189.d3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Galobardes B, Lynch JW, Davey Smith G. Childhood socioeconomic circumstances and cause-specific mortality in adulthood: Systematic review and interpretation. Epidemiol. Rev. 2004;26:7–21. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxh008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kittleson MM, et al. Association of childhood socioeconomic status with subsequent coronary heart disease in physicians. Arch. Intern. Med. 2006;166:2356–2361. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.21.2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Taylor SE. Mechanisms linking early life stress to adult health outcomes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2010;107:8507–8512. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003890107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Miller GE, et al. Low early-life social class leaves a biological residue manifested by decreased glucocorticoid and increased proinflammatory signaling. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:14716–14721. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902971106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Baumeister D, Akhtar R, Ciufolini S, Pariante CM, Mondelli V. Childhood trauma and adulthood inflammation: A meta-analysis of peripheral C-reactive protein, interleukin-6 and tumour necrosis factor-α. Mol. Psychiatry. 2016;21:642–649. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Danese A, Lewis SJ. Psychoneuroimmunology of early-life stress: The hidden wounds of childhood trauma? Neuropsychopharmacology. 2017;42:99–114. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Brenhouse HC, Danese A, Grassi-Oliveira R. Neuroimmune impacts of early-life stress on development and psychopathology. In: Coolen LM, Grattan DR, editors. Neuroendocrine Regulation of Behavior. Springer; 2018. pp. 423–447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Aiken CE, Ozanne SE. Sex differences in developmental programming models. Reprod. Camb. Engl. 2013;145:R1–R13. doi: 10.1530/REP-11-0489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Del Giudice M, et al. Individual differences in developmental plasticity: A role for early androgens? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2018;90:165–173. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Stinson S. Sex differences in environmental sensitivity during growth and development. Am. J. Phys. Anthrol. 1985;28:123–147. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.1330280507. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gallo EAG, Munhoz TN, de Mola CL, Murray J. Gender differences in the effects of childhood maltreatment on adult depression and anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Child Abuse Negl. 2018;79:107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Laceulle OM, et al. Stressful events and psychological difficulties: Testing alternative candidates for sensitivity. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2014;23:103–113. doi: 10.1007/s00787-013-0436-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kuzawa CW, Adair LS. Lipid profiles in adolescent Filipinos: Relation to birth weight and maternal energy status during pregnancy. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2003;77:960–966. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/77.4.960. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Yap P, et al. Soil-transmitted helminth infections and physical fitness in school-aged Bulang children in southwest China: Results from a cross-sectional survey. Parasit. Vectors. 2012;5:50. doi: 10.1186/1756-3305-5-50. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Campbell F, et al. Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health. Science. 2014;343:1478–1485. doi: 10.1126/science.1248429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Conti G, Heckman J, Pinto R. The effects of two influential early childhood interventions on health and healthy behaviour. Econ. J. Lond. Engl. 2016;126:F28–F65. doi: 10.1111/ecoj.12420. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.García JL, Heckman JJ, Leaf DE, Prados MJ. The Life-Cycle Benefits of an Influential Early Childhood Program. National Bureau of Economic Research; 2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Mustard CA, Etches J. Gender differences in socioeconomic inequality in mortality. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. 2003;57:974–980. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.12.974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Brumbach BH, Figueredo AJ, Ellis BJ. Effects of harsh and unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life history strategies. Hum. Nat. 2009;20:25–51. doi: 10.1007/s12110-009-9059-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Giudice M, Gangestad SW, Kaplan HS. Life history theory and evolutionary psychology. In: Buss D, editor. The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Foundations. 2. Wiley; 2016. pp. 88–114. [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nunn CL, Lindenfors P, Pursall ER, Rolff J. On sexual dimorphism in immune function. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci. 2009;364:61–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rolff J. Bateman’s principle and immunity. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2002;269:867–872. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2002.1959. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stearns SC. The Evolution of Life Histories. OUP; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stoehr AM, Kokko H. Sexual dimorphism in immunocompetence: What does life-history theory predict? Behav. Ecol. 2006;17:751–756. doi: 10.1093/beheco/ark018. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Belsky J, Schlomer GL, Ellis BJ. Beyond cumulative risk: Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of parenting and early life history strategy. Dev. Psychol. 2012;48:662–673. doi: 10.1037/a0024454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ellis BJ, Figueredo AJ, Brumbach BH, Schlomer GL. Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk: The impact of harsh versus unpredictable environments on the evolution and development of life history strategies. Hum. Nat. 2009;20:204–268. doi: 10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Newey S, Thirgood SJ, Hudson PJ. Do parasite burdens in spring influence condition and fecundity of female mountain hares Lepus timidus? Wildl. Biol. 2004;10:171–176. doi: 10.2981/wlb.2004.023. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Pioz M, et al. Diseases and reproductive success in a wild mammal: Example in the alpine chamois. Oecologia. 2008;155:691–704. doi: 10.1007/s00442-007-0942-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wasser SK, Barash DP. Reproductive suppression among female mammals: Implications for biomedicine and sexual selection theory. Q. Rev. Biol. 1983;58:513–538. doi: 10.1086/413545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bateman AJ. Intra-sexual selection in Drosophila. Heredity. 1948;2:349–368. doi: 10.1038/hdy.1948.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Griskevicius V, Delton AW, Robertson TE, Tybur JM. Environmental contingency in life history strategies: The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on reproductive timing. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011;100:241–254. doi: 10.1037/a0021082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mittal C, Griskevicius V, Simpson JA, Sung S, Young ES. Cognitive adaptations to stressful environments: When childhood adversity enhances adult executive function. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2015;109:604–621. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.World Health Organization (WHO) Adverse Childhood Experiences International Questionnaire (ACEs-IQ) WHO; 2009. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Arnegard ME, Whitten LA, Hunter C, Clayton JA. Sex as a biological variable: A 5-year progress report and call to action. J. Womens Health. 2020;29:858–864. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2019.8247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Alvergne A, Faurie C, Raymond M. Variation in testosterone levels and male reproductive effort: Insight from a polygynous human population. Horm. Behav. 2009;56:491–497. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Bancroft J. The endocrinology of sexual arousal. J. Endocrinol. 2005;186:411–427. doi: 10.1677/joe.1.06233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. MPlus User’s Guide. 7. Muthén & Muthén; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 42.French SS, DeNardo DF, Moore MC. Trade-offs between the reproductive and immune systems: Facultative responses to resources or obligate responses to reproduction? Am. Nat. 2007;170:79–89. doi: 10.1086/518569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.McKean KA, Nunney L. Increased sexual activity reduces male immune function in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2001;98:7904–7909. doi: 10.1073/pnas.131216398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Norris K, Evans MR. Ecological immunology: Life history trade-offs and immune defense in birds. Behav. Ecol. 2000;11:19–26. doi: 10.1093/beheco/11.1.19. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Buford TW, Willoughby DS. Impact of DHEA(S) and cortisol on immune function in aging: A brief review. Appl. Physiol. Nutr. Metab. Physiol. Appl. Nutr. Metab. 2008;33:429–433. doi: 10.1139/H08-013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Van Voorhees EE, Dennis MF, Calhoun PS, Beckham JC. Association of DHEA, DHEAS, and cortisol with childhood trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2014;29:56–62. doi: 10.1097/YIC.0b013e328364ecd1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gassen J, et al. Inflammation predicts decision-making characterized by impulsivity, present focus, and an inability to delay gratification. Sci. Rep. 2019;9:4928. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41437-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hill SE, Prokosch ML, DelPriore DJ, Griskevicius V, Kramer A. Low childhood socioeconomic status promotes eating in the absence of energy need. Psychol. Sci. 2016;27:354–364. doi: 10.1177/0956797615621901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data reported in the manuscript are publicly available on the Open Science Framework (https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/DXPZU).