Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2021 May 18.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Sci. 2020 May 10;23(6):e12973. doi: 10.1111/desc.12973

TABLE 4.

Multiple regression analyses examining predictors of English-language outcomes at 4½ years, controlling for SES and exposure (n = 95)

Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10 Model 11 Model 12
SESa 4.5 (1.1)*** 4.4 (1.1)*** 4.1 (1.1)*** 4.1 (1.1)*** 4.3 (1.1)*** 4.3 (1.1)***
% Spanish exposureb −5.3 (1.1)*** −5.7 (1.1)*** −6.3 (1.1)*** −6.5 (1.1)*** −6.5 (1.1)*** −6.7 (1.1)***
Spanish vocabulary Sizec 2.8 (1.1)* 2.1 (1.1) 2.1 (1.1)
Spanish accuracyd 3.6 (1.1)** 3.1 (1.1)**
Spanish RTd −4.0 (1.1)*** −3.4 (1.1)**
Total R2 30.4%*** 35.1%*** 37.6%** 40.0%*** 38.7%*** 41.1%***
r2-change 4.7%* 7.2%** 9.6%** 8.3%** 10.7%***

Note: Unstandardized coefficients (SE); r2-change for Models 8–12 are in reference to Model 7.

a

Socioeconomic status (SES) at 4½ years from an updated version of the Hollingshead Index of Social Status (Hollingshead, 1975).

b

Reported relative exposure to Spanish versus English at 4½ years based on a comprehensive language background interview.

c

Number of words reported as ‘comprende y dice’ on the Spanish-language Inventario: Palabras y Enunciados at 2 years (Jackson-Maldonado et al., 2003).

d

Accuracy and speed (RT) of processing familiar Spanish words in the looking-while-listening (LWL) procedure at 2 years (Fernald et al., 2008).

*

p < .05;

**

p < .01;

***

p < .001.