Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Eat Behav. 2021 Mar 22;41:101500. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2021.101500

Emotion Suppression, Coping Strategies, Dietary Patterns, and BMI

Olga M Herren a, Tanya Agurs-Collins a, Laura A Dwyer a, Frank M Perna a, Rebecca Ferrer a
PMCID: PMC8131265  NIHMSID: NIHMS1685840  PMID: 33812125

Abstract

Objective:

Emotion suppression (ES) is associated with unhealthy coping strategies, such as emotional eating. Physical activity (PA) is a healthy coping strategy that may attenuate the association between emotion suppression and emotional eating (EE). This study evaluated whether: 1) ES is associated with body mass index (BMI) through EE and/or dietary patterns, 2) PA moderates these relationships, and 3) these patterns differ by race/ethnicity and gender.

Methods:

Adult participants (N=1674) of the Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health, and Eating study completed modified versions of the Emotion Regulation, Eating in the Absence of Hunger, and International Physical Activity Questionnaires; a validated dietary assessment; and items on demographics, height, and weight.

Results:

Analyses revealed a serial mediation pathway in the full sample where greater ES was associated with higher BMI through greater EE and lower fruit and vegetable (F&V) intake (B=.0017, CI 95% [.0001, .0042]) after controlling for age, gender, and education. Hedonic snack food (HSF) intake was not a significant mediator of the ES-BMI association. Greater PA attenuated associations of ES and EE with dietary intake and BMI. The serial pathway remained significant for non-Hispanic White women only in subgroup analyses. EE was a significant mediator among women, and PA effects were largely found among Hispanics and men.

Conclusions:

ES was associated with higher BMI through greater EE and lower F&V, but not HSF intake. PA attenuated these associations. Differences in patterns of coping strategies may help to explain disparities in obesity-related health behavior.

Keywords: physical activity, disparities, ethnicity, emotion regulation, coping strategies, emotional eating

1. Introduction

Obesity prevalence is increasing among U.S. adults, and evidence linking obesity to adverse health outcomes is substantial (Hales et al., 2017; Jensen et al., 2014; Lauby-Secretan et al., 2016). Racial/ethnic and gender disparities exist, with Hispanic/Latinx1 (47%) and non-Hispanic Black (NHB) adults (46.8%) having the highest rates of obesity, followed by non-Hispanic White (NHW) adults (37.9%). Also, women are more likely than men to be obese in NHB and Hispanic populations (Hales et al., 2017).

The rise in obesity is partly driven by nationwide trends in dietary patterns (i.e., eating behavior and food choices) and sedentary behavior (Livingstone & Pourshahidi, 2014; Millen et al., 2016). Despite the importance of energy balance behavior such as diet and exercise, current behavioral interventions have produced inconsistent weight loss results across different contexts and populations (Tate et al., 2016) and mechanistic studies rarely consider multiple health behaviors concurrently with psychological processes to evaluate joint influence on dietary patterns and body mass index (BMI) in diverse samples. Even fewer studies have examined these associations across both race/ethnicity and gender to understand processes that contribute to disparities in obesity and obesity-related conditions.

1.1. Emotion suppression and coping strategies

Emotion suppression (ES) is an emotion regulation strategy in which behavioral expression of an emotion is concealed to reduce subjective emotion experience and potential negative social reactions to emotion expression (Gross & John, 2003). Consequences of ES are: accumulation of unresolved negative emotion, suppression of positive emotion, and continuous cognitive effort to manage emotion responses (Gross & Levenson, 1997; Stepper & Strack, 1993; Strack et al., 1988). ES also contributes to stress-related symptoms, such as subjective negative affect, depression, anxiety, and decreased well-being (Haga et al., 2009; Moore et al., 2008). Accordingly, individuals who suppress emotions often engage in secondary emotion regulation or coping strategies (Folkman & Lazarus, 1984), including both healthy (e.g., physical activity (PA)) and unhealthy (e.g., eating unhealthy foods) behavior. Greater ES has been reported in individuals with obesity compared to those with normal weight (Fernandes et al., 2018). Greater understanding of behavioral coping strategies in response to ES could facilitate a better understanding of the association of ES with obesity.

1.2. Emotional eating and dietary patterns

When ES is not effective in reducing negative emotions, individuals may engage in eating as a means of coping, even if they are not hungry (Haedt-Matt et al., 2014; Macht & Simons, 2000; Macht, 2008). Indeed, ES was associated with emotional eating (EE) among the sample also used in the present study (Ferrer et al., 2017). EE often translates to intake of hedonic foods (high-calorie and energy dense foods with low nutritional value) that produce pleasurable sensations (Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991; Lehman & Rodin, 1989; Macht & Mueller, 2007; Spoor et al., 2007). Findings are mixed as to whether overconsumption associated with EE also includes more or less nutritious, healthy food intake (Ferrer et al., 2017; Larson et al., 2008). EE may be linked to obesity through its influence on hedonic food intake (Laitinen et al., 2002). However, other coping-related health behaviors (i.e., PA) may disrupt the associations of ES and EE with dietary intake and body mass.

1.3. PA and dietary patterns

PA benefits extend beyond reduced morbidity and mortality to include stress reduction, improvements in quality of life, and positive emotion (Dunn et al., 2005; Sarafino, 2006). Regular PA throughout the lifespan is associated with lower incidence and prevalence of obesity (Booth et al., 2000; Myers et al., 2002). Individuals report using PA to cope with stress (American Psychological Association [APA], 2012). In addition to stress and emotion benefits, PA increases positive self-referent thought, reducing reliance on EE (Childs & de Wit, 2014; Emerson & Williams, 2015; Wood et al., 2018). PA may attenuate otherwise positive associations of EE to dietary behavior and BMI (Dohle et al., 2014; Koenders & van Strien, 2011). However, no research has examined whether PA moderates associations between ES and EE to (healthy and unhealthy) dietary behavior and BMI.

1.4. Race/Ethnicity-based differences

Coping strategies may differ among various ethnic groups in the U.S. NHB and Hispanic adults disproportionately experience chronic stressors, such as discrimination and neighborhood factors (e.g., greater access to unhealthy foods, limited access to healthy foods, and safety and availability of PA spaces) (Clark et al., 1999; Grigsby-Toussaint et al., 2010; Mwendwa et al., 2011), which may increase distress and need for emotion regulation strategies. Indeed, racial/ethnic minorities report more frequent use of ES and more frequent attempts to control emotions (Gross & John, 2003; Keltner et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 2008). These differences in ES across race/ethnicity and whether or not they are captured may contribute to inconsistent findings of emotional and binge eating behaviors among NHB and Hispanic women (Kelly, Cotter, et al., 2012; Kelly, Mitchell, et al., 2012; Lydecker & Grilo, 2016; Marques et al., 2011; Striegel-Moore et al., 2000). PA may also be less likely to disrupt the consequences of ES and EE among racial/ethnic minorities, given that these individuals are less likely to meet recommended PA guidelines (Center for Disease Control & Prevention [CDC], 2011; Hawkins et al., 2009). Thus, it is important to identify whether patterns of emotion regulation, behavioral coping and their impact on BMI differ by race/ethnicity.

1.5. Gender-based differences

Associations among ES, EE, PA and BMI may differ by gender. While men may engage in more ES, women are more likely than men to report negative affective outcomes from ES (Flynn et al., 2010), and more likely to engage in EE (Chacko et al., 2015; Tanofsky et al., 1997). Additionally, greater engagement in PA among men (Piercy et al., 2018) may help to explain why ES/EE have not been as strongly associated with BMI among men as a means to cope with negative affect (APA, 2012). Women from racial/ethnic minority groups are most likely to have obesity, report more frequent use of ES, and engage in inadequate PA (Crespo et al., 2001). However, NHW women are more likely to report EE than NHB women (Chacko et al., 2015). These findings underscore the importance of contextualizing gender differences in the context of race/ethnicity instead of examining each variable in isolation.

The current study evaluated whether: 1) ES is associated with BMI through EE and/or dietary patterns, 2) PA moderates these relationships, and 3) these relationships differ by race/ethnicity and/or gender. We hypothesized that greater ES would be associated with higher BMI through greater EE, lower fruit and vegetable (F&V) intake, and greater hedonic snack food (HSF) intake (see Figure 1). We further hypothesized that PA would attenuate these associations and we explored potential differences based on race/ethnicity and/or gender.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

A mediation and moderation model of theorized associations between emotion suppression, emotional eating, dietary patterns, BMI and physical activity

2. Methods 2.1.Participants and procedure

Data were collected as part of the National Cancer Institute’s (NCI) Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health and Eating (FLASHE) study. FLASHE is a cross-sectional internet-based survey of psychosocial, generational, and environmental correlates of diet, PA, and other health behaviors among adolescent-parent dyads, and was approved by the NCI Special Studies IRB. Informed consent was obtained before participants received the surveys. Our analyses focus on the adult sample. Of 1,699 adults who completed both the diet and PA surveys, 1,674 had data on all relevant measures and were included in this exploratory study. Full study methodology and item development are reported elsewhere (Ferrer et al., 2017; Nebeling et al., 2017; Oh et al., 2017) and available at https://cancercontrol.cancer.gov/brp/hbrb/flashe.html.

2.2. Measurements

Adults reported demographic information (gender, educational attainment, race/ethnicity, income, etc.), height, and weight (used to calculate BMI).

2.2.1. ES was assessed with a four-item version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (Gross & John, 2003). These items were: “I keep my emotions to myself,” “When I am feeling POSITIVE emotions, I am careful not to express them,” “ I control my emotions by NOT EXPRESSING THEM,” and “When I am feeling NEGATIVE emotions, I make sure not to express them.” Scores were summed such that higher scores reflected a greater degree of ES, and reliability was acceptable (α=.81) (Ferrer et al., 2017).

2.2.2.EE was assessed using two items from the EE subscale of the Eating in the Absence of Hunger Questionnaire, based on factor loadings in a sample of adolescents (Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2008). These items were designed to assess eating in response to anxiety and sadness (i.e., negative affect), consistent with the concept of EE (Faith et al., 1997). The items asked “How often do YOU start or continue to eat when YOU’RE not hungry because… (1) You feel sad or depressed? and (2) You feel anxious or nervous?,” rated Never (1) to Always (5)”. These items were summed, and higher scores were indicative of greater EE. Scale reliability was calculated for this sample and was acceptable (α=.83).

2.2.3.Dietary patterns were ascertained through a screener that used items adapted from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey, 2009–2010 Dietary Screener (http://epi.grants.cancer.gov/nhanes/dietscreen/) (Smith et al., 2017). The screener asked participants: “DURING THE PAST 7 DAYS, how many times did you eat [food/beverage]?” Daily F&V intake was operationalized as the sum of 5 items reflecting consumption of 100% fruit juice, fruits, green salad, other non-fried vegetables, and cooked beans. Daily HSF intake was the sum of 7 items reporting intake of candy/ chocolate, cookies/cake, potato chips, fried potatoes, frozen desserts, sugary cereal, and processed meat.

2.2.4. PA was assessed using 6 items from the short version of the International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ) (Craig et al., 2003). Participants provided information on time spent walking and in moderate- and vigorous-intensity activity and were given definitions of these intensities in the item. Variables were calculated from reported daily minutes to yield weekly minutes of moderate, vigorous, and walking, which were combined to derive total weekly PA. Vigorous-level PA was doubled before being summed (Craig et al., 2003), and total PA was converted into hours per week for easier interpretation.

2.3. Data analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated for all study variables. To correct for extreme values and potential bias in self-reporting, we Winsorized weekly hours of PA (at the 95th percentile) (Reifman & Keyton, 2010) and BMI scores (1st and 99th percentiles). Covariates (e.g., age, gender, highest educational level attained) were selected based on relevant literature and confirmed with bivariate correlational analyses.

Hierarchical linear regression models were estimated to test whether ES was associated with BMI. Covariates were entered in the first step, and ES was entered in the second step. The PROCESS macro (Hayes, 2017) was used to compute moderation and mediation models (see Figure 1). This macro uses bootstrapping methods with 5000 draws to compute 95% confidence intervals to test the null hypothesis. To assess EE (M) as a mediator between ES(X) and BMI (Y), model 4 was estimated. Model 6 was estimated to evaluate whether dietary patterns (F&V and HSF intake) further mediated this relationship in a serial fashion. To examine whether PA moderated the serial pathways between ES, EE, dietary pattern variables, and BMI, model 92 was estimated. Simple interactions using model 1 were estimated to assess whether PA moderates the various relationships between ES, EE, dietary patterns and BMI. The Johnson-Neyman technique was applied to confirm directionality of the interactions. Analyses were conducted on the full sample, then exploratory analyses were conducted separately by race, gender, and subgroups of gender within racial/ethnic groups.

3. Results

3.1. Sample characteristics

Descriptive statistics are presented in Tables 1 and 2. Most participants were 35–59 years of age, and female. This sample was highly educated, with 45.6% of participants reporting completion of a college degree or higher. Approximately equal proportions of participants reported BMIs indicative of normal weight, overweight, and obesity. NHBs reported the highest average BMI, and among NHBs, women had the highest reported average BMI. NHW women reported the lowest average BMI.

Table 1.

Descriptive statistics for participant characteristics and variables of interest

Body Mass Index (kg/m2) Total Physical Activity (hrs/wk) Emotion Suppression Emotional Eating Hedonic Snack Food Consumption (daily intake in servings) Fruit & Vegetable Consumption (daily intake in servings)
M2(SD)3 M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD) M(SD)

Total Sample 28.12(6.78) 15.94(15.71) 10.62(3.70) 4.77(1.98) 4.58(2.19) 3.11(2.17)
Non-Hispanic Whites 27.61(6.60) 15.62(15.24) 10.69(3.67) 4.88(1.95) 4.52(2.10) 3.11(2.06)
Non-Hispanic Blacks 30.67(7.31) 17.51(17.73) 10.36(3.77) 4.40(2.11) 4.77(2.16) 3.17(2.52)
Hispanics 28.07(6.30) 16.63(15.88) 10.78(3.61) 4.77(2.00) 5.00(2.78) 3.15(2.19)
Men 27.91(5.37) 20.04(17.12) 11.46(3.57) 4.31(1.92) 4.94(2.25) 3.11(2.05)
Non-Hispanic Whites 27.84(5.34) 20.39(17.41) 11.60(3.46) 4.33(1.90) 4.97(2.38) 3.22(2.14)
Non-Hispanic Blacks 28.05(5.16) 22.31(17.71) 10.94(3.54) 3.84(1.84) 4.89(1.81) 2.81(2.06)
Hispanics 28.64(5.94) 19.60(17.10) 10.83(4.16) 4.15(1.80) 2.22(1.34) 3.11(1.82)
Women 28.21(7.21) 14.39(14.89) 10.33(3.71) 4.93(1.98) 4.46(2.15) 3.11(2.22)
Non-Hispanic Whites 27.54(7.01) 13.76(13.88) 10.36(3.73) 5.08(1.93) 4.36(1.96) 3.07(2.03)
Non-Hispanic Blacks 31.18(7.57) 16.55(17.62) 10.24(3.82) 4.51(2.14) 4.75(2.23) 3.25(2.61)
Hispanics 27.75(6.56) 14.14(14.67) 10.70(3.25) 5.12(2.02) 4.96(3.11) 3.18(2.39)
2

M=mean

3

SD= standard deviation

Table 2.

Demographic Characteristics by Race/Ethnicity

Race/Ethnicity
Total Non-Hispanic White Non-Hispanic Black Hispanic
N=1674 N=1229 N=315 N=130
n % n % n % n %

Gender
Men 434 26% 330 26.9% 53 16.6% 51 39.5%
Women 1240 74% 899 73.1% 262 83.4% 79 60.5%
Age
18–34 189 11.3% 108 8.8% 62 19.7% 19 14.6%
35–44 729 43.6% 512 41.7% 140 44.6% 77 59.2%
45–59 708 42.3% 570 46.4% 105 33.4% 33 25.4%
60 + 47 2.9% 39 3.2% 7 2.2% 1 .8%
Body Mass Index Categories4
Underweight 22 1.3% 17 1.4% 2 .6% 3 2.3%
Normal Weight 596 36% 489 40.2% 65 20.8% 42 32.6%
Overweight 506 30.5% 357 29.4% 102 32.7% 47 36.4%
Obese 532 32.2% 352 29.0% 143 45.8% 37 28.7%
Physical Activity
>150 min/wk 195 13.2% 136 12.4% 45 17.4% 14 12%
150–300 min/wk 209 14.2% 160 14.6% 32 12.4% 17 14.5%
>300 min/wk 1068 72.5% 801 73% 181 70.2% 86 73.5%
Highest Education Level
≤ High school degree 306 18.4% 232 19% 45 14.4% 29 22.5%
Some college 600 36% 420 34.3% 137 43.8% 43 33.3%
≥ 4-year college degree 760 45.6% 572 16.7% 131 41.9% 57 44.2%
4

Body Mass Index Categories: underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), normal weight (18.5–24.9 kg/m2), overweight (25–29.9 kg/m2), obese (≥30 kg/m2)

3.2. Association of ES with BMI

Greater ES was significantly associated with higher BMI (R2=.02) (CI 95% [.036, .209]) in the full sample (see Table 3). This association remained significant in subgroup analyses among women (R2=.02) (CI 95% [.034, .247]), NHW (R2=.02) (CI 95% [.018, .221]) and NHB (R2=.07) (CI 95% [.039, .480]) participants. This relationship was also significant among NHW (R2=.03) (CI 95% [.018, .266]) and NHB (R2=.05) (CI 95% [.004, .508]) women, and Hispanic men (R2=.14) (CI 95% [.075, .971]).

Table 3.

Direct and indirect associations between emotion suppression, emotional eating, dietary patterns, and Body Mass Index

Direct Indirect Serial Mediation

Emotion Suppression Emotional Eating Hedonic Snack Food Intake Fruit and Vegetable Intake Emotional Eating & Hedonic Snack Food Intake Emotional Eating & Fruit and Vegetable Intake
B(p-value) B(95% CI) B(95% CI) B(95% CI) B(95% CI) B(95% CI)
Total Sample .123(.005) .0810(.0556, .1104) .0065(−.0037, .0200) .0095(.0003, .0256) .0003(−.0027, .0037) .0017(.0001, .0042)
 Non-Hispanic Whites .119(.02) .1110(.0752, .1514) .0016(−.0126, .0206) .0084(−.0043, .0314) −.0015(−.0057, .0024) .0021(−.0003, .0057)
 Non-Hispanic Blacks .259(.02) .0415(−.0099, 0991) .0101(−.0275, 0483) .0090(−.0119, .0465) .0002(−.0064, .0094) .0006(−.0038, .0060)
 Hispanics .258(.11) .0210(−.0703, .1133) −.0011(−.0182, .0597) .0374(−.0485, .1606) .0002(−.0337, .0294) −.0074(−.0303, .0165)
Men .053(.45) .0375(−.0009, .0823) −.0076(−.0323, .0074) .0073(−.0099, .0333) −.0066(−.0177, .0021) .0000(−.0024, .0032)
 Non-Hispanic Whites .035(.68) .0458(−.0146, .1096) −.0289(−.0787, .0045) .0033(−.1036, .0295) −.0100(−.0264, −.0001) −.0008(−.0061, .0037)
 Non-Hispanic Blacks .245(.25) −.0046(−.1043, .0627) .0137(−.0829, .1092) −.0051(−.0822, .0889) .0021(−.0300, .0403) .0013(−.0202, .0338)
 Hispanics .523(.02) .0232(−.1103, .2392) −.0157(−.1091, .0762) .1361(−.0533, .4003) .0065(−.0462, .0364) −.0055(−.0725, .0550)
Women .140(.01) .0889(.0557, .1262) .0115(−.0003, .0311) .0079(−.0031, .0270) .0014(−.0009, .0050) .0022(.0001, .0055)
 Non-Hispanic Whites .142(.03) .1233(.0772, .1777) .0082(−.0030, .0342) .0080(−.0105, .0413) .0008(−.0023, .0051) .0035(.0001, .0091)
 Non-Hispanic Blacks .256(.047) .0558(−.0050, .1272) .0156(−.0374, .0647) .0110(−.0126, .0593) .0008(−.0064, .0125) .0011(−.0033, .0075)
 Hispanics .050(.84) .0374(−.1126, .1833) .0000(−.0334, .0989) −.0178(−.0809, .1354) −.0038(−.0752, .0515) −.0057(−.0392, .0405)

Outcomes significant at p<.05 are in bold.

3.3. EE and dietary patterns as mediators

EE significantly mediated the relationship between ES and BMI (R2=.06) (see Table 3 for all results). This mediational pathway was significant in subgroup analyses for women (R2=.08) and NHW participants (R2=.08). When considering both race/ethnicity and gender, this mediational pathway was found in NHW women only (R2=.11).

The association between ES and BMI was also mediated by F&V intake in the full sample only (R2=.03). HSF intake was not a significant mediator.

3.4. Serial mediation effects of EE and dietary patterns

There was evidence for serial mediation, such that ES and BMI were linked via ES’s association with EE, the subsequent association of EE with reduced F&V intake, and finally the association of F&V intake with BMI (R2=.08) (see Table 3 and Figure 2). This pathway remained significant in subgroup analyses among women (R2=.09). When considering both race/ethnicity and gender, this pathway remained significant among NHW women (R2=.12). However, among NHW men, an unexpected pathway showed that greater ES was associated with greater EE and HSF intake, which in turn, was associated with lower BMI (R2=.02).

Figure 2. A mediation and moderation model of associations between emotion suppression, emotional eating, fruit and vegetable consumption, BMI and physical activity*.

Figure 2.

*Values represent standardized regression coefficients of the direct relationships between variables with p values depicted in parentheses. The values stemming from physical activity represent standardized regression coefficients and p values are depicted in parentheses for interaction effects. The colors of the arrows represent direction of association (green=positive, red= negative). Control variables include participant age, sex, and educational attainment. BMI= body mass index.

3.5. PA as a moderator

PA moderated some direct associations between EE, dietary patterns, and BMI; however, it did not moderate any mediation pathways (see Table 4 and Figure 2). In the full sample, greater amounts of PA attenuated the positive association between EE and BMI (CI 95% [−.0242, −.0037]) and between ES with HSF intake (CI 95% [.0000, .0034]). No other pathways were moderated by PA for the full sample.

Table 4.

Interaction of physical activity on associations between emotional eating, emotion suppression, Body Mass Index and dietary patterns

Emotion Suppression × Total Physical Activity Emotional Eating × Total Physical Activity
B(p-value) R2 B(p-value) R2
Total Sample
 Body Mass Index −.00(.73) .03 −.01(.01) .08
 Hedonic Snack Foods .00(.04) .04 .00(.40) .04
 Fruits and Vegetables −.00(.09) .05 .00(.22) .06
Non-Hispanic Whites
 Body Mass Index −.00(.32) .03 −.01(.01) .10
 Hedonic Snack Foods .00(.41) .04 .00(.57) .05
 Fruits and Vegetables −.00(.14) .04 .00(.08) .05
Non- Hispanic Blacks
 Body Mass Index −.00(.95) .09 −.02(.17) .13
 Hedonic Snack Foods .00(.24) .03 −.01(.02) .06
 Fruits and Vegetables −.00(.96) .05 −.00(.63) .09
Hispanics
 Body Mass Index −.00(.85) .09 .01(.66) .05
 Hedonic Snack Foods .00(.79) .06 .03(.00) .26
 Fruits and Vegetables −.02(.00) .36 .01(.04) .28

Outcomes significant at p<.05 are in bold.

PA did not moderate any associations among women. However, PA attenuated the association of ES with HSF intake among men (CI 95% [.0001, .0066]).

PA significantly moderated several relationships in racial/ethnic subsamples (see Table 4). Among Hispanic participants, PA attenuated associations of EE with F&V intake (CI 95% [.0004, .0227]) and HSF intake (CI 95% [.0169, .0448]), and between ES and F&V intake (CI 95% [−.0241, −.0070]). This was also reflected in analyses that considered both race/ethnicity and gender, where associations remained significant for both Hispanic men and women (see supplementary material). The association of EE with HSF intake was attenuated by PA (CI 95% [−.0219, −.0045]) among NHB women.

4. Discussion

Analyses supported several of our hypotheses regarding linkages among ES and EE with dietary behavior and BMI, and the role of PA in attenuating these pathways. As hypothesized, greater ES was associated with higher BMI through greater EE and reduced F&V intake. Interestingly, HSF intake was not a significant mediator in this model. PA attenuated some of these positive associations, including those between EE and BMI, and ES and dietary intake. In subgroup analyses, the serial pathway in which greater ES was associated with higher BMI through greater EE and reduced F&V intake in the full sample remained significant only among NHW women. Moreover, PA generally moderated associations between ES, EE, F&V intake, and BMI among men and Hispanic individuals, but not the mediational pathway.

Results in the full sample are consistent with other studies. The indirect association of ES with HSF and F&V intake via EE in this sample has previously been reported in analyses controlling for BMI (Ferrer et al., 2017). Another study supported a mediated pathway between emotion regulation strategies and BMI via EE (Jones et al., 2019). The present analyses extend these findings by demonstrating a serial pathway between ES and BMI through EE and dietary intake. Moreover, these analyses demonstrate that reduced F&V intake may be more consequential for BMI in the context of ES, compared to increased hedonic eating. However, self-reported dietary screeners, like the one used in this study, may be influenced by social desirability, differences in appropriate portion perception and food choices during mealtimes, or passive overconsumption that are not accurately self-reported and contribute to the development of obesity (Beaulieu et al., 2017; Blundell & King, 1996; Blundell & MacDiarmid, 1997). Thus, more accurate dietary intake measures may yield different results.

Our results are consistent with prior research suggesting that PA may attenuate the association of negative affect with BMI and unhealthy eating behavior (Dohle et al., 2014; Koenders & van Strien, 2011). These findings are important in furthering knowledge of the impact of PA on emotion regulation, EE, food choices, and BMI. The lack of a moderated mediation finding may be due to a reporting bias, as participants tend to overestimate PA levels using the IPAQ (Hagstromer et al., 2010). Alternatively, PA may not impact emotionally influenced eating behavior related to BMI until high levels of PA are achieved, or the associations between PA and the variables outlined in this paper may be complex. For example, although those who experience negative affect are more likely to engage in PA (Liao et al., 2017), PA can cause mood declines among beginners and those who do not exercise regularly or intensely. Future studies using actigraphy data and ecological momentary assessment may also help to clarify temporal relationships, and possibly cyclic patterns, among motives for engaging in PA, intensity and duration of PA, its affective consequences, and other health behavior (Dunton et al., 2020).

These analyses also provide a more comprehensive understanding of the interplay of psychological mechanisms, coping strategies, and BMI across race/ethnicity and gender. The findings among the full sample only remained significant among NHW women. Consistent with previous research that has largely included NHW women, women are more likely than men to report greater emotionality from ES (Flynn et al., 2010), and are more likely to emotionally eat (Chacko et al., 2015; Tanofsky et al., 1997). Additionally, among NHW men, greater ES was related to lower BMI through greater EE and increased HSF intake. This finding may represent potentially greater social and physical environmental supports for healthy lifestyle habits, resilience, and recovery among men (Andrade et al., 2010; Ben-Shlomo & Kuh, 2002). Consistent with previous research, men in this study reported greater amounts of PA. Their PA moderated associations between ES and HSF intake, which may reflect the beneficial impact of PA on the associations among emotions, appetitive urges, and food choices that determine BMI (Dohle et al., 2014; Koenders & van Strien, 2011; Piercy et al., 2018). Although ES was related to BMI for NHWs and NHBs, results suggest that EE may not be a coping strategy that is engaged in or available to all groups. Also, there may be a reporting bias where NHWs are more aware and able to report EE, or that individual factors may be more relevant to health outcomes among NHWs.

Inconsistent findings of ES, EE and PA in other subgroups may reflect unique challenges experienced among different minority groups, and limitations of individual factors as influences on health behavior. Challenges to healthy lifestyle habits exist among racial/ethnic minorities beyond individual-level factors, reflecting the need for ecological frameworks in studies addressing determinants of health behavior (Fleury & Lee, 2006; Mohammed et al., 2016; O’Driscoll et al., 2014). For example, factors such as food availability, instability of work schedules, and acculturation may contribute to obesogenic eating patterns beyond the contribution of emotional influences and PA patterns (Kaplan et al., 2004; O’Driscoll et al., 2014; Hargreaves et al., 2002). Thus, cultural and ecological influences associated with race/ethnicity may be more important to consider when investigating obesity disparities.

The current study was limited by notably smaller sample sizes among NHBs and, especially, among Hispanic individuals. However, given the challenges of recruiting from racial/ethnic minority populations, and the paucity of research that considers multiple racial/ethnic groups, the current findings serve as an exploratory analysis of potential trends in psychological determinants of health behavior underlying racial/ethnic and gender differences. The EE scale used in this study was designed and validated for children and adolescents, which may limit generalizations made to adult populations (Tanofsky-Kraff et al., 2008). Additionally, the self-reported data gathered on PA (Hagstromer et al., 2010), dietary intake (Lechner et al., 1997; Neuhouser et al., 1999), and height and weight (Mozumdar & Liguori, 2016) also present sources of error that yield challenges in being able to reliably extend results, compared to objective measures. However, given the associations that were revealed despite the use of these broad instruments, the use of objective and detailed measurements may help to elaborate on differences between subgroups and how best to address psychological determinants of obesity-related health behavior.

5. Conclusions

ES was associated with higher BMI through greater EE and reduced F&V intake, but not HSF intake. PA attenuated many of these associations. Future studies should consider gender and race/ethnicity concurrently to more precisely identify potential pathways and barriers for weight loss and maintenance interventions through adoption of healthy coping strategies, even in the presence of unhealthy behavior. In addition, ES and EE did not fully explain determinants of dietary behavior in all subsamples, supporting a view that adherence to a healthy lifestyle cannot be achieved by self-regulation alone and is highly influenced by factors outside of the individual (Krieger, 2001; Thorpe Jr et al., 2015; Williams & Jackson, 2005). Future research should also consider other potential contributing factors, such as built environment, epigenetics, education, social and cultural norms.

Supplementary Material

1

Highlights:

  • Emotion suppression was related to BMI through emotional eating and F&V intake.

  • HSF intake was not a mediator between emotion suppression and BMI.

  • PA moderated associations among emotion suppression, emotional eating and BMI.

  • Coping behavior differences may underlie BMI trends by race/ethnicity and gender.

Acknowledgments

Funding: The Family Life, Activity, Sun, Health and Eating (FLASHE) Study was funded by the National Cancer Institute (NCI) under contract number HHSN261201200039I.

Abbreviations:

(ES)

Emotion suppression

(EE)

Emotional Eating

(BMI)

Body mass index

(F&V)

fruit and vegetable

(HSF)

hedonic snack food

Footnotes

Declarations of interest: none

1

Hereafter referred to as Hispanics

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Andrade AM, Coutinho SR, Silva MN, Mata J, Vieira PN, Minderico CS, … Teixeira PJ (2010). The effect of physical activity on weight loss is mediated by eating self-regulation. Patient Educ Couns, 79(3), 320–326. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2010.01.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychological Association [APA]. (2012). Stress in America: Our health at risk. In.
  3. Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell J, & Finlayson G (2017). Impact of physical activity level and dietary fat content on passive overconsumption of energy in non-obese adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act, 14(1), 14. doi: 10.1186/s12966-017-0473-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ben-Shlomo Y, & Kuh D (2002). A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int J Epidemiol, 31(2), 285–293. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blundell JE, & King NA (1996). Overconsumption as a cause of weight gain: behavioural-physiological interactions in the control of food intake (appetite). Ciba Found Symp, 201, 138–154; discussion 154–138, 188–193. doi: 10.1002/9780470514962.ch9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Blundell JE, & MacDiarmid JI (1997). Fat as a risk factor for overconsumption: satiation, satiety, and patterns of eating. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 97(7 Suppl), S63–69. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8223(97)00733-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Booth FW, Gordon SE, Carlson CJ, & Hamilton MT (2000). Waging war on modern chronic diseases: primary prevention through exercise biology. Journal of Applied Physiology, 88(2), 774–787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chacko SA, Chiodi SN, & Wee CC (2015). Recognizing disordered eating in primary care patients with obesity. Preventive Medicine, 72, 89–94. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.12.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Childs E, & de Wit H (2014). Regular exercise is associated with emotional resilience to acute stress in healthy adults. Front Physiol, 5, 161. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2014.00161 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, & Williams DR (1999). Racism as a stressor for African Americans - A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist, 54(10), 805–816. doi:Doi 10.1037/0003-066x.54.10.805 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC]. (2011). Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Health Disparities and Inequalities Report.
  12. Craig CL, Marshall AL, Sjostrom M, Bauman AE, Booth ML, Ainsworth BE, … Oja P (2003). International physical activity questionnaire: 12-country reliability and validity. Med Sci Sports Exerc, 35(8), 1381–1395. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000078924.61453.FB [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Crespo CJ, Smit E, Carter-Pokras O, & Andersen R (2001). Acculturation and leisure-time physical inactivity in Mexican American adults: results from NHANES III, 1988–1994. Am J Public Health, 91(8), 1254–1257. doi: 10.2105/ajph.91.8.1254 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Dohle S, Hartmann C, & Keller C (2014). Physical activity as a moderator of the association between emotional eating and BMI: evidence from the Swiss Food Panel. Psychol Health, 29(9), 1062–1080. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.909042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dunn AL, Trivedi MH, Kampert JB, Clark CG, & Chambliss HO (2005). Exercise treatment for depression: efficacy and dose response. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 28(1), 1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2004.09.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dunton GF, Kaplan JT, Monterosso J, Pang RD, Mason TB, Kirkpatrick MG, … Leventhal AM (2020). Conceptualizing Health Behaviors as Acute Mood-Altering Agents: Implications for Cancer Control. Cancer Prev Res (Phila), 13(4), 343–350. doi: 10.1158/1940-6207.CAPR-19-0345 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Emerson JA, & Williams DM (2015). The Multifaceted Relationship Between Physical Activity and Affect. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(8), 419–433. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12190 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  18. Faith MS, Allison DB, & Geliebter A (1997). Emotional eating and obesity: theoretical considerations and practical recommendations. In Overweight and weight management: The health professional’s guide to understanding and practice (pp. 439–465). [Google Scholar]
  19. Fernandes J, Ferreira-Santos F, Miller K, & Torres S (2018). Emotional processing in obesity: a systematic review and exploratory meta-analysis. Obesity Reviews, 19(1), 111–120. doi: 10.1111/obr.12607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Ferrer RA, Green PA, Oh AY, Hennessy E, & Dwyer LA (2017). Emotion suppression, emotional eating, and eating behavior among parent-adolescent dyads. Emotion, 17(7), 1052–1065. doi: 10.1037/emo0000295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fleury J, & Lee SM (2006). The social ecological model and physical activity in African American women. Am J Community Psychol, 37(1–2), 129–140. doi: 10.1007/s10464-005-9002-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Flynn JJ, Hollenstein T, & Mackey A (2010). The effect of suppressing and not accepting emotions on depressive symptoms: Is suppression different for men and women? J Personality Individual Differences, 49(6), 582–586. [Google Scholar]
  23. Folkman S, & Lazarus R (1984). Coping and adaptation. In The handbook of behavioral medicine (pp. 282–325). New York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
  24. Grigsby-Toussaint DS, Zenk SN, Odoms-Young A, Ruggiero L, & Moise I (2010). Availability of Commonly Consumed and Culturally Specific Fruits and Vegetables in African-American and Latino Neighborhoods. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 110(5), 746–752. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2010.02.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gross JJ, & John OP (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol, 85(2), 348–362. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Gross JJ, & Levenson RW (1997). Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion. J Abnorm Psychol, 106(1), 95–103. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.1.95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK, Racine SE, Burt SA, Hu JY, Boker S, … Klump KL (2014). Do emotional eating urges regulate affect? Concurrent and prospective associations and implications for risk models of binge eating. Int J Eat Disord, 47(8), 874–877. doi: 10.1002/eat.22247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Haga SM, Kraft P, & Corby EK (2009). Emotion Regulation: Antecedents and Well-Being Outcomes of Cognitive Reappraisal and Expressive Suppression in Cross-Cultural Samples. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10(3), 271–291. doi: 10.1007/s10902-007-9080-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Hagstromer M, Ainsworth BE, Oja P, & Sjostrom M (2010). Comparison of a subjective and an objective measure of physical activity in a population sample. J Phys Act Health, 7(4), 541–550. doi: 10.1123/jpah.7.4.541 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Hales CM, Carroll MD, Fryar CD, & Ogden CL (2017). Prevalence of Obesity Among Adults and Youth: United States, 2015–2016. NCHS Data Brief(288), 1–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Hargreaves MK, Schlundt DG, & Buchowski MS (2002). Contextual factors influencing the eating behaviours of African American women: a focus group investigation. Ethn Health, 7(3), 133–147. doi: 10.1080/1355785022000041980 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hawkins MS, Storti KL, Richardson CR, King WC, Strath SJ, Holleman RG, & Kriska AM (2009). Objectively measured physical activity of USA adults by sex, age, and racial/ethnic groups: a cross-sectional study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act, 6, 31. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-6-31 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Hayes AF (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach: Guilford publications. [Google Scholar]
  34. Heatherton TF, & Baumeister RF (1991). Binge eating as escape from self-awareness. Psychol Bull, 110(1), 86–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Jensen MD, Ryan DH, Apovian CM, Ard JD, Comuzzie AG, Donato KA, … Obesity S (2014). 2013 AHA/ACC/TOS guideline for the management of overweight and obesity in adults: a report of the American College of Cardiology/American Heart Association Task Force on Practice Guidelines and The Obesity Society. J Am Coll Cardiol, 63(25 Pt B), 2985–3023. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2013.11.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Jones J, Kauffman BY, Rosenfield D, Smits JAJ, & Zvolensky MJ (2019). Emotion dysregulation and body mass index: The explanatory role of emotional eating among adult smokers. Eating Behaviors, 33, 97–101. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.05.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaplan MS, Huguet N, Newsom JT, & McFarland BH (2004). The association between length of residence and obesity among Hispanic immigrants. J American journal of preventive medicine, 27(4), 323–326. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kelly NR, Cotter EW, & Mazzeo SE (2012). Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q): Norms for Black women. Eating Behaviors, 13(4), 429–432. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2012.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Kelly NR, Mitchell KS, Gow RW, Trace SE, Lydecker JA, Bair CE, & Mazzeo S (2012). An Evaluation of the Reliability and Construct Validity of Eating Disorder Measures in White and Black Women. Psychological Assessment, 24(3), 608–617. doi: 10.1037/a0026457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Keltner D, Gruenfeld DH, & Anderson C (2003). Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110(2), 265–284. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.110.2.265 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Koenders PG, & van Strien T (2011). Emotional eating, rather than lifestyle behavior, drives weight gain in a prospective study in 1562 employees. J Occup Environ Med, 53(11), 1287–1293. doi: 10.1097/JOM.0b013e31823078a2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Krieger N (2001). Theories for social epidemiology in the 21st century: an ecosocial perspective. Int J Epidemiol, 30(4), 668–677. doi: 10.1093/ije/30.4.668 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Laitinen J, Ek E, & Sovio U (2002). Stress-related eating and drinking behavior and body mass index and predictors of this behavior. Preventive Medicine, 34(1), 29–39. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0948 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Larson NI, Neumark-Sztainer DR, Harnack LJ, Wall MM, Story MT, & Eisenberg ME (2008). Fruit and vegetable intake correlates during the transition to young adulthood. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 35(1), 33–37. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2008.03.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Lauby-Secretan B, Scoccianti C, Loomis D, Grosse Y, Bianchini F, Straif K, & International Agency for Research on Cancer Handbook Working, G. (2016). Body Fatness and Cancer--Viewpoint of the IARC Working Group. N Engl J Med, 375(8), 794–798. doi: 10.1056/NEJMsr1606602 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Lechner L, Brug J, & De Vries H (1997). Misconceptions of fruit and vegetable consumption: differences between objective and subjective estimation of intake. Journal of nutrition education, 29(6), 313–320. [Google Scholar]
  47. Lehman AK, & Rodin J (1989). Styles of self-nurturance and disordered eating. J Consult Clin Psychol, 57(1), 117–122. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.57.1.117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Liao Y, Chou CP, Huh J, Leventhal A, & Dunton G (2017). Examining acute bi-directional relationships between affect, physical feeling states, and physical activity in free-living situations using electronic ecological momentary assessment. J Behav Med, 40(3), 445–457. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9808-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Livingstone MB, & Pourshahidi LK (2014). Portion size and obesity. Adv Nutr, 5(6), 829–834. doi: 10.3945/an.114.007104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Lydecker JA, & Grilo CM (2016). Different yet similar: Examining race and ethnicity in treatment-seeking adults with binge eating disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol, 84(1), 88–94. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Macht M (2008). How emotions affect eating: a five-way model. Appetite, 50(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Macht M, & Mueller J (2007). Immediate effects of chocolate on experimentally induced mood states. Appetite, 49(3), 667–674. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Macht M, & Simons G (2000). Emotions and eating in everyday life. Appetite, 35(1), 65–71. doi: 10.1006/appe.2000.0325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Marques L, Alegria M, Becker AE, Chen CN, Fang A, Chosak A, & Diniz JB (2011). Comparative prevalence, correlates of impairment, and service utilization for eating disorders across US ethnic groups: Implications for reducing ethnic disparities in health care access for eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord, 44(5), 412–420. doi: 10.1002/eat.20787 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Millen BE, Abrams S, Adams-Campbell L, Anderson CA, Brenna JT, Campbell WW, … Lichtenstein AH (2016). The 2015 Dietary Guidelines Advisory Committee Scientific Report: Development and Major Conclusions. Adv Nutr, 7(3), 438–444. doi: 10.3945/an.116.012120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Mohammed A, Harrell JP, Makambi KH, Campbell AL Jr., Sloan LR, Carter-Nolan PL, & Taylor TR (2016). Factors Associated with Exercise Motivation among African-American Men. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities, 3(3), 457–465. doi: 10.1007/s40615-015-0158-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Moore SA, Zoellner LA, & Mollenholt N (2008). Are expressive suppression and cognitive reappraisal associated with stress-related symptoms? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(9), 993–1000. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Mozumdar A, & Liguori G (2016). Corrective Equations to Self-Reported Height and Weight for Obesity Estimates Among U.S. Adults: NHANES 1999–2008. Res Q Exerc Sport, 87(1), 47–58. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2015.1124971 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Mwendwa DT, Gholson G, Sims RC, Levy SA, Ali M, Harrell CJ, … Campbell AL (2011). Coping With Perceived Racism: A Significant Factor in the Development of Obesity in African American Women? Journal of the National Medical Association, 103(7), 602–608. doi:Doi 10.1016/S0027-9684(15)30386-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Myers J, Prakash M, Froelicher V, Do D, Partington S, & Atwood JE (2002). Exercise capacity and mortality among men referred for exercise testing. New England Journal of Medicine, 346(11), 793–801. doi:DOI 10.1056/NEJMoa011858 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Nebeling LC, Hennessy E, Oh AY, Dwyer LA, Patrick H, Blanck HM, … Yaroch AL (2017). The FLASHE Study: Survey Development, Dyadic Perspectives, and Participant Characteristics. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 52(6), 839–848. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.01.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Neuhouser ML, Kristal AR, McLerran D, Patterson RE, & Atkinson J (1999). Validity of short food frequency questionnaires used in cancer chemoprevention trials: results from the Prostate Cancer Prevention Trial. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev, 8(8), 721–725. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. O’Driscoll T, Banting LK, Borkoles E, Eime R, & Polman R (2014). A systematic literature review of sport and physical activity participation in culturally and linguistically diverse (CALD) migrant populations. Journal of Immigrant Minority Health, 16(3), 515–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Oh AY, Davis T, Dwyer LA, Hennessy E, Li T, Yaroch AL, & Nebeling LC (2017). Recruitment, Enrollment, and Response of Parent-Adolescent Dyads in the FLASHE Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 52(6), 849–855. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2016.11.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Piercy KL, Troiano RP, Ballard RM, Carlson SA, Fulton JE, Galuska DA, … Olson RD (2018). The Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. JAMA, 320(19), 2020–2028. doi: 10.1001/jama.2018.14854 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Reifman A, & Keyton K (2010). Winsorize. J Encyclopedia of research design, 3, 1636–1638. [Google Scholar]
  67. Roberts NA, Levenson RW, & Gross JJ (2008). Cardiovascular costs of emotion suppression cross ethnic lines. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70(1), 82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sarafino EP (2006). Health psychology, biopsychosocial interactions. New York: John willey & sons. [Google Scholar]
  69. Smith TM, Calloway EE, Pinard CA, Hennessy E, Oh AY, Nebeling LC, & Yaroch AL (2017). Using Secondary 24-Hour Dietary Recall Data to Estimate Daily Dietary Factor Intake From the FLASHE Study Dietary Screener. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 52(6), 856–862. doi: 10.1016/j.amepre.2017.01.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Spoor STP, Bekker MH, Van Strien T, & van Heck GL (2007). Relations between negative affect, coping, and emotional eating. Appetite, 48(3), 368–376. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.10.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Stepper S, & Strack F (1993). Proprioceptive Determinants of Emotional and Nonemotional Feelings. J Pers Soc Psychol, 64(2), 211–220. doi:Doi 10.1037/0022-3514.64.2.211 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Strack F, Martin LL, & Stepper S (1988). Inhibiting and Facilitating Conditions of the Human Smile - a Nonobtrusive Test of the Facial Feedback Hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol, 54(5), 768–777. doi:Doi 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Striegel-Moore RH, Wilfley DE, Pike KM, Dohm FA, & Fairburn CG (2000). Recurrent binge eating in black American women. Arch Fam Med, 9(1), 83–87. doi: 10.1001/archfami.9.1.83 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Tanofsky-Kraff M, Ranzenhofer LM, Yanovski SZ, Schvey NA, Faith M, Gustafson J, & Yanovski JA (2008). Psychometric properties of a new questionnaire to assess eating in the absence of hunger in children and adolescents. Appetite, 51(1), 148–155. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2008.01.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tanofsky MB, Wilfley DE, Spurrell EB, Welch R, & Brownell KD (1997). Comparison of men and women with binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord, 21(1), 49–54. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tate DF, Lytle LA, Sherwood NE, Haire-Joshu D, Matheson D, Moore SM, … Michie S (2016). Deconstructing interventions: approaches to studying behavior change techniques across obesity interventions. Transl Behav Med, 6(2), 236–243. doi: 10.1007/s13142-015-0369-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Thorpe RJ Jr, Kelley E, Bowie JV, Griffith DM, Bruce M, & LaVeist T (2015). Explaining racial disparities in obesity among men: Does place matter? J American journal of men’s health, 9(6), 464–472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Williams DR, & Jackson PB (2005). Social sources of racial disparities in health. Health Aff (Millwood), 24(2), 325–334. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.24.2.325 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Wood CJ, Clow A, Hucklebridge F, Law R, & Smyth N (2018). Physical fitness and prior physical activity are both associated with less cortisol secretion during psychosocial stress. Anxiety Stress Coping, 31(2), 135–145. doi: 10.1080/10615806.2017.1390083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

RESOURCES