Abstract
The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is a critical structure in the flexible control of value-based behaviors. OFC dysfunction is typically only detected when task or environmental contingencies change, against a backdrop of apparently intact initial acquisition and behavior. While intact acquisition following OFC lesions in simple Pavlovian cue-outcome conditioning is often predicted by models of OFC function, this predicted null effect has not been thoroughly investigated. Here, we test the effects of lesions and temporary muscimol inactivation of the rodent lateral OFC on the acquisition of a simple single cue-outcome relationship. Surprisingly, pretraining lesions significantly enhanced acquisition after overtraining, whereas post-training lesions and inactivation significantly impaired acquisition. This impaired acquisition to the cue reflects a disruption of behavioral control and not learning since the cue could also act as an effective blocking stimulus in an associative blocking procedure. These findings suggest that even simple cue-outcome representations acquired in the absence of OFC function are impoverished. Therefore, while OFC function is often associated with flexible behavioral control in complex environments, it is also involved in very simple Pavlovian acquisition where complex cue-outcome relationships are irrelevant to task performance.
Keywords: acquisition, flexible behavior, orbital prefrontal, Pavlovian, value
Introduction
The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is critical to behavioral flexibility when learning and behavior need to be updated to reflect a change in the environment (Kringelbach 2005; Klein-Flugge et al. 2013; Rudebeck and Murray 2014; Murray and Rudebeck 2018; Gardner et al. 2019). In particular, the OFC is necessary for appropriately updating behavior when the contingencies between predictive cues and outcomes change, or when outcomes change in value (Pickens et al. 2005; Walton et al. 2011; Panayi and Killcross 2018). The information encoded in OFC about the expected value and identity of predicted outcomes is necessary for flexibly updating behavior when these outcome features change. Population and single-unit neuronal firing in the OFC encodes many features of reward outcomes (e.g., size, preference, identity, time, location, probability, certainty, salience; Delamater 2007; Padoa-Schioppa 2009; Ogawa et al. 2013; Takahashi et al. 2013; Stalnaker et al. 2014; Sadacca et al. 2018; Zhou et al. 2019); furthermore, the coding of these features develops over the course of learning to predictive cues in anticipation of the expected outcome (Schoenbaum et al. 2009). There is also substantial evidence to suggest that this outcome expectancy information in the OFC is incorporated into midbrain dopaminergic reward prediction errors (Takahashi et al. 2011), which are critical for learning (Schultz 1998; Steinberg et al. 2013).
However, despite these close ties to the learning process, the OFC is typically not necessary for initial learning (Delamater 2007; Murray et al. 2007; Rudebeck and Murray 2014; Stalnaker et al. 2015; Izquierdo 2017), except in the most complex of circumstances (e.g., Walton et al. 2011). Lesions and functional inactivation of the OFC do not appear to disturb initial learning about Pavlovian cue-outcome relationships in a range of tasks, and instead only reveal their effects when the cue-outcome relationships change, or when the value of expected outcomes change, such as in reversal learning and outcome devaluation procedures (Butter 1969; Iversen and Mishkin 1970; Dias et al. 1996; Gallagher et al. 1999; Schoenbaum et al. 2003; West et al. 2011). To account for these effects, one class of OFC theories suggests that the OFC is necessary for representing information about the sensory-specific properties or identity of expected outcomes (Delamater 2007; Burke et al. 2008; Schoenbaum et al. 2009, 2011). A second, but complementary class of theories using a reinforcement learning framework suggests that the OFC is necessary for the representation of latent state information (Wilson et al. 2014). In reinforcement learning models, tasks such as Pavlovian conditioning can be divided into discrete physically observable states, such as “cue on,” “cue off,” and “reward,” and underlying latent states signaled by partially observable information recalled into working memory such as reinforcement history.
Both theories, while couched in different computational and theoretical frameworks, suggest similar roles for the OFC. Latent states encompass specific outcome expectancies and include a broader category of potential stimuli (e.g., internal context; Niv 2019). Implicit in these theories is that initial acquisition should be affected by OFC dysfunction if performance depends on specific outcome expectancy or latent states (e.g., the differential outcomes effect; McDannald et al. 2005; Boulougouris et al. 2007; Boulougouris and Robbins 2009; complex multiple-choice probabilistic learning tasks; Walton et al. 2011), but not in putatively “simple” single CS–US (cue-outcome) learning tasks (Gallagher et al. 1999), where the outcome identity and value of the US stays constant and is reliably predicted by the CS. While this null effect is often reported in procedures involving learning about multiple CSs and/or USs (Burke et al. 2008; Schoenbaum et al. 2009; Panayi and Killcross 2018), there is little evidence from tasks involving only a single CS–US relationship where a null result is clearly predicted. For example, Gallagher et al. (1999) found no effect of complete OFC lesions on single CS–US acquisition but stopped training before behavior reached asymptote (Schoenbaum et al. 2003).
Both latent state and sensory-specific outcome expectancy theories of OFC function predict a null effect of OFC lesions on initial acquisition learning, particularly in situations involving only a single CS–US relationship. Indeed, this null effect is often reported as an important feature of OFC dysfunction as it demonstrates that behavior can appear normal when the impoverished aspects of the underlying task representation are not directly relevant to task performance (Murray et al. 2007; Schoenbaum et al. 2009; Wilson et al. 2014; Stalnaker et al. 2015). Here, we directly tested this prediction in rats trained on a single CS–US Pavlovian task following lesions targeting the lateral OFC. Surprisingly, pretraining OFC lesions significantly increased Pavlovian acquisition behavior after extended training. In contrast, post-training lesions and intra-OFC infusions of muscimol impaired Pavlovian acquisition behavior. Using an associative blocking design, we confirmed that even though behavior was impaired, the underlying learning about the CS–US contingency remained intact.
Methods and Materials
General
Animals
Subjects were male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services, Gippsland, Victoria, Australia) approximately 4 months old. Rats were housed 4 per cage in ventilated Plexiglass cages in a temperature regulated (22 ± 1°C) and light regulated (12 h light/dark cycle, lights on at 7:00 AM) colony room. At least 1 week prior to behavioral testing, feeding was restricted to ensure that weight was approximately 95% of ad libitum feeding weight, and never dropped below 85%. All animal research was carried out in accordance with the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratories Animals (NIH publications No. 80-23, revised 1996) and approved by the University of New South Wales Animal Care and Ethics Committee.
Apparatus
Behavioral testing was conducted in 8 identical operant chambers (30.5 x 32.5 x 29.5 cm; Med Associates) individually housed within ventilated sound attenuating cabinets. Each chamber was fitted with a 3-W house light that was centrally located at the top of the left-hand wall. Food pellets could be delivered into a recessed magazine, centrally located at the bottom of the right-hand wall. Delivery of up to two separate liquid rewards via rubber tubing into the magazine was achieved using peristaltic pumps located above the testing chamber. The top of the magazine contained a white LED light that could serve as a visual stimulus. Access to the magazine was measured by infrared detectors at the mouth of the recess. Two retractable levers were located on either side of the magazine on the right-hand wall. A speaker located to the right of the house light could provide auditory stimuli to the chamber. In addition, a 5-Hz train of clicks produced by a heavy-duty relay placed outside the chamber at the back-right corner of the cabinet was used as an auditory stimulus. The chambers were wiped down with ethanol (80% v/v) between each session. A computer equipped with Med-PC software (Med Associates Inc.) was used to control the experimental procedures and record data.
Consumption Chambers
To provide individual access to reinforcers during the satiety and devaluation procedures, rats were placed into an individual cage (33 x 18 x 14 cm clear Perspex cage with a wireframe top). Pellet reinforcers were presented in small glass ramekins inside the box and liquid reinforcers were presented in water bottles with a sipper tube. One day prior to the target procedure, all rats were exposed to the individual cages and given 30 min of free access to home cage food and water to reduce novelty to the context and consuming from the ramekin and water bottles.
Locomotor Activity
Locomotor activity was assessed in 8 identical boxes measuring 50 x 36 x 18 cm (length x width x height), housed in a sound attenuated room. Each box consisted of 4 opaque white polyurethane walls and floor and a removable roof. In the center of the roof was an 18 x 40 cm grid of 3 x 3 mm ventilation holes. Two custom pairs of infrared beam detectors spanned the width of the box to detect locomotor activity and were located 15 cm from each end of the box. Beam breaks, corresponding to activity within the box, were recorded on a computer equipped with Med-PC software (Med Associates Inc.).
Surgery
Excitotoxic lesions targeting the lateral OFC were performed in Experiment 1 and Supplementary Experiment 1. Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane, their heads shaved, and placed in a stereotaxic frame (World Precision Instruments, Inc.). The scalp was incised, and the skull exposed and adjusted to flat skull position. Two small holes were drilled into the skull and the dura mater was severed to reveal the underlying cortical parenchyma. A 1-μL Hamilton needle (Hamilton Company) was lowered through the two holes targeting the lateral OFC (co-ordinates specified below). Stereotaxic co-ordinates were AP: +3.5 mm; ML: ±2.2 mm; D-V: −5.0 mm from bregma. At each site, the needle was first left to rest for 1 min. Then, an infusion of N-methyl-D-aspartic acid (NMDA; Sigma-Aldrich, Switzerland), dissolved in phosphate buffered saline (pH 7.4) to achieve a concentration of 10 μg/μL, was infused for 3 min at a rate of 0.1 μ/min. Finally, the needle was left in situ for a further 4 min to allow the solution to diffuse into the tissue. Following the diffusion period, the needle was retracted, and the scalp cleaned and sutured. Sham lesions proceeded identically to excitotoxic lesions except that during the infusion period no infusion occurred. After a minimum of 1 week of postoperative recovery, rats were returned to food restriction for 2 days prior to further training.
In Experiments 2, 3, and Supplementary Experiment 2, bilateral guide cannulae were surgically implanted targeting the lateral OFC. Rats were anesthetized with isoflurane, their heads shaved, and placed in a stereotaxic frame (World Precision Instruments, Inc.). The scalp was incised, and the skull exposed and adjusted to flat skull position. Two small holes were drilled for the cannulae using a high-speed drill, and four holes were hand drilled on different bone plates to hold fixing screws. Bilateral stainless-steel guide cannulae (26-gauge, length 5 mm below pedestal; Plastics One, Roanoke, VA) were lowered into the lateral OFC (AP: +3.5 mm; ML: ±2.2 mm; D-V: −4.0 mm from bregma). Cannulae were held in place by dental cement and anchored to the skull with 4 fixing screws. Removable dummy cannulae were inserted into the guide cannulae to prevent them from blocking. After 1 week of postoperative recovery, rats were returned to food restriction for 2 days prior to further testing.
Drugs and Infusions
The GABAA agonist muscimol (Sigma-Aldrich, Switzerland) was dissolved in 0.9% (w/v) nonpyrogenic saline to obtain a final concentration of 0.5 μg/0.5 μL. Nonpyrogenic saline 0.9% (w/v) was used as the saline control. During infusions, muscimol or saline was infused bilaterally into the lateral OFC by inserting a 33-gauge internal cannula into the guide cannula that extended 1 mm ventral to the guide tip. The internal cannula was connected to a 25 μL glass syringe (Hamilton Company) attached to a microinfusion pump (World Precision Instruments, Inc.). A total volume of 0.5 μL was delivered to each side at a rate of 0.25 μL/min. The internal cannula remained in place for an additional 1 min after the infusion and then removed. During the infusion, procedure animals could move freely in a bucket to minimize stress. Dummy cannulae were removed prior to, and replaced immediately after, infusions. For the two training sessions prior to infusions, all animals received dummy infusions which were identical to the infusion procedure, except that no liquids were infused. These dummy infusions were performed to familiarize the rats with the microinfusion procedure and thereby minimize stress. Dummy infusions were also conducted on test sessions after the infusions to minimize differences in handling between experimental stages.
Reinforcers
The reinforcers used were a single grain pellet (45 mg dustless precision grain-based pellets; Bio-serv), a single sucrose pellet (45 mg dustless precision sucrose pellets; Bio-serv), and 20% w/v maltodextrin solution (Myopure, Petersham) flavored with 0.4% v/v concentrated lemon juice (Berri, Melbourne) to provide unique sensory properties to the reinforcer. Liquids were delivered over a period of 0.33 s via a peristaltic pump which corresponded to a volume of 0.2 mL. The volume and concentration of liquid reinforcers was chosen to match the calorific value of the corresponding grain and sucrose pellet reward and have been found to elicit similar rates of Pavlovian and instrumental responding as a pellet reward in other experiments conducted in this lab. In all experiments involving liquids, the magazine was scrubbed with warm water and thoroughly dried between sessions to remove residual traces of the liquid reinforcer. To reduce neophobia to the reinforcers, 1 day prior to magazine training sessions all animals were pre-exposed to the reinforcers (10 g of pellets per animal and 25 mL of liquid reinforcer per animal) in their home cage.
Magazine Training
All animals received one session of magazine training for each experimental reinforcer with the following parameters: reward delivery was on an RT60 s schedule for 16 rewards. When necessary, sessions were separated by at least 2 h and the order of reinforcer identity was counterbalanced between groups.
Behavior
CS responding was operationalized as the number of magazine entries during the CS period. PreCS responding was operationalized as the frequency of responding during the immediately preceding the CS period and was used as a measure of baseline responding to the testing context. PreCS responding was analyzed separately, and any group differences identified and reported. Data were presented as CS – PreCS difference scores, which reflect discriminative responding to the CS. All data were analyzed with mixed ANOVAs using R statistical software (Lenth et al. 2020; R Core Team 2020; Singmann et al. 2020), and significant interactions of interest were followed up with ANOVAs on the relevant subset of data, and simple effects with a Tukey family-wise error rate correction. Where relevant, planned linear and quadratic orthogonal trend contrasts and their interactions between groups were analyzed to assess differences in rates of responding.
Experiment 1: Pretraining OFC Lesions
Subjects
Subjects were forty-eight (N = 48) rats, tested in two cohorts. Cohort 1, n = 16 rats weighing between 280 and 361 g (M = 312.2 g) and cohort 2, n = 32 rats weighing between 271 and 328 g (M = 296.3 g).
Training
Pavlovian Acquisition
Following magazine training, all rats received 21 sessions of Pavlovian acquisition training. Each session consisted of 16 presentations of a single auditory CS (a 15 s train of clicks) presented on a VT90s schedule (ranging from 60 to 120 s). A single pellet (US) was delivered at the termination of each CS. The session duration was 28 min and animals were left in the chamber for an additional 2 min before being removed. Animals received either 1 session per day, or 2 sessions per day separated by at least 2 h.
Subgroup 1: General Satiety Prefeeding
At the end of acquisition training on day 21, a subgroup of animals (sham n = 8, lesion n = 8) were taken off food restriction and given 24 h free access to their home cage food before further acquisition training on day 22. This session was rewarded as per acquisition training. At the end of day 22, animals were put back on food restriction and continued acquisition training.
Subgroup 2: Devaluation
Following initial Pavlovian acquisition of a single CS–US association, a second subgroup of animals (sham n = 8, lesion n = 8) were retrained with two novel unique CS–US associations intended to test devaluation in a taste aversion procedure.
Novel Acquisition
Novel acquisition of two unique CS–US associations was conducted with identical parameters to initial acquisition training, 2 sessions per day for 14 days. There were 16 trials per session separated by a vITI90s, with each trial consisting of a 15 s CS coterminating with reward. Unlike initial acquisition, the two CSs were an 80 dB white noise and a 2800 Hz, 80 dB tone followed by either a sucrose pellet or a lemon flavored maltodextrin liquid reinforcers (CS–US identities counterbalanced between animals).
Taste Aversion
Taste aversion took place in the devaluation chambers and involved 30 min exposure to one US every day, alternating each day for 4 days. Following fee access to a US, animals were immediately injected i.p. with either 0.15 M LiCl or 0.9% saline (15 mL/kg). The outcome paired with nausea induced by injection of LiCl was designated the devalued outcome and the outcome paired with neutral saline injections was designated the nondevalued outcome (counterbalanced between animals). Following the final day of injections, all animals were given a day of rest in their home cage to allow hunger levels to return to normal after taste aversion training.
Devaluation Test
Animals were tested with a single session of CS training except that no rewards were delivered, that is, in extinction. Data from the first trial were analyzed at test.
Locomotor Activity
At the end of the experimental procedures, all animals were assessed for locomotor activity over a 1-h period.
Experiment 2: Post-Training Muscimol Inactivation
Subjects
Subjects were 32 (total N = 32) male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services) approximately 4 months old, weighing between 285 and 350 g (M = 319.7 g).
Pavlovian Acquisition
Animals were given 9 sessions, 1 session per day, of Pavlovian acquisition training with session parameters identical to those described in Experiment 1. This number of sessions was chosen because the effect of pretraining lesions appeared after around 9 session in Experiment 1. Briefly, each session consisted of a VT90s ITI with 16 trials consisting of a 15 s click CS coterminating with a single pellet US. Following the final day of training, all animals were taken off food restriction and received surgical implantation of guide cannulae.
Post-Training
Preinfusion
Following postoperative recovery animals were returned to food restriction for a day before receiving a further 2 days of acquisition training as per pretraining. However, immediately prior to entering the chamber all animals received a dummy infusion.
Infusion
Animals were pseudo-randomly assigned to one of two infusion groups such that performance was matched and there were no differences between groups on the final day of preinfusion acquisition. For the next 4 days, all animals received an infusion of saline or Muscimol immediately prior to entering the testing chamber for a Pavlovian acquisition session.
Postinfusion
On the final 2 days of training, all animals received a further 2 days of acquisition training immediately preceded by a dummy infusion.
Supplementary Experiment 1: Post-Training OFC Lesions
Methods
Subjects
Subjects were 24 (total N = 24) male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services) approximately 4 months old, weighing between 317 and 369 g (M = 338.9 g).
Prelesion Training
Pavlovian acquisition. All animals received 9 days of Pavlovian acquisition training, 1 session per day. On the final day of training, all animals were removed from food restriction for at least 24 h before receiving sham or excitotoxic lesions of the OFC. Lesion conditions were pseudorandomly assigned to animals such that group performance was matched on the final day of acquisition and an equal number of animals were assigned to each lesion condition in each homecage.
Postlesion Training
Pavlovian acquisition. Following postoperative recovery, all animals were returned to food restriction for 24 h before receiving an additional 9 days of acquisition training.
Supplementary Experiment 2: OFC Inactivation Early in Acquisition
Subjects
Subjects were 32 (total N = 16) male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services) approximately 4 months old, weighing between 321 and 399 g (M = 357.4 g).
Surgery
Surgical implantation of cannulae occurred prior to any behavioral training.
Pavlovian Acquisition
Animals were given 10 sessions, 1 session per day. Briefly, each session consisted of a VI 200 s ITI with 16 trials consisting of a 10s light CS (illumination of the house light at the back of the chamber) coterminating with a single pellet US. Subjects received mock infusions on days 3 and 4, and either Saline or Muscimol was infused prior to entering the chamber on days 5–9. On day 10, all animals received a mock infusion.
Experiment 3: OFC Inactivation Prior to Associative Blocking
Subjects
Subjects were 32 (total N = 32) male Long Evans rats (Monash Animal Services) approximately 4 months old, weighing between 299 and 395 g (M = 331.5 g).
Surgery
Surgical implantation of cannulae occurred prior to any behavioral training.
Training
The design of the experiment was such that 4 CSs were designated as cues A, B, C, and D. Cues A and C were always visual cues, either darkness caused by extinguishing the houselight or flashing panel lights (5 Hz; Fig. 3A). Cues B and D were always auditory cues, either an 80 dB white noise or a 5 Hz train of clicks. Throughout all training sessions, the house light was always illuminated unless it was extinguished to act as a visual cue. All cues lasted 10 s and coterminated with the delivery of the US, 2 pellets delivered consecutively 0.25 s apart. The identity of the cues was counterbalanced between subjects except that A and C were always visual cues and B and D were always auditory cues. Simultaneous audio-visual compounds were designated as AB and CD. Pavlovian training sessions were always 56 mins long such that there were 16 trials with a vITI 200 s (range 100–300 s); animals were left in the chambers for an additional 2 min before being removed.
Food Restriction and Magazine Training
Magazine training sessions consisted of an RT120s reward delivery schedule for 16 rewards. Each reward consisted of 2 pellets delivered to the magazine 0.25 s apart.
Stage 1
Stage 1 acquisition involved 10 days of acquisition to cue A, 16 trials per session. On days 1–4 of training, all animals received dummy infusions to familiarize them to the infusion procedure. Animals were then split into two groups with matched performance on day 4. On days 5–10, all animals received an infusion of saline or muscimol immediately prior to entering the test chambers.
Pre-Exposure
On day 11, all rats received pre-exposure to auditory cues B and D, 4 nonrewarded presentations of each cue vITI 200 s. This was done to minimize novelty to the auditory cues during compound training in stage 2. All animals received dummy infusions prior to the session.
Stage 2
On days 12–14, all animals received stage 2 audio-visual compound training. Sessions involved 8 presentations of compound AB and 8 presentations of CD (pseudorandomly presented such that a compound was never repeated more than 2 times in a row). The compounds were rewarded with 2 pellets, the same US that was used in stage 1. All animals received dummy infusions prior to each session.
Test
On days 15 and 16, all animals were tested in extinction for responding to the target auditory cue B and the overshadowing control cue D (8 presentations of each cue, pseudorandom trial order, vITI 200 s). All animals received dummy infusions prior to each session.
Reacquisition
On days 17–19, all animals received reacquisition training to cue B (16 trials per session) to test for differences in rates of reacquisition to the blocked cue. On days 20–21, animals were tested for reacquisition to cue A (16 trials per session) to test for differences in the rate of reacquisition to the blocking cue.
Data Availability
Raw data available at: Panayi MC. 1 February 2021. Data for: The role of the rodent lateral OFC in simple Pavlovian cue-outcome learning depends on training experience. doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/TNBH7.
Results
Experiment 1: Pretraining OFC Lesions
Histology and Group Allocation
Lesion damage is depicted in Supplementary Figure S1 (supplement 1). Lesion extent was judged by a trained observer blind to group allocation. A lesion was retained if there was evidence of significant bilateral damage constrained to LO or DLO. Animals were excluded if there was only unilateral LO/DLO damage, evidence of damage to the dorsal part of the anterior olfactory nucleus ventral to LO/DLO or if there was extensive damage to the white matter of the forceps minor of the corpus callosum. One lesioned animal did not recover from surgery, four lesion animals had only unilateral OFC damage, and one lesioned animal had extensive white matter damage. Forty-two animals were retained (N = 42, sham n = 24, lesion n = 18), of which subgroup 1 contained 15 (N = 15; sham n = 8, lesion n = 7) and subgroup 2 contained 13 (N = 13; sham n = 8, lesion n = 5).
PreCS Analysis
Analysis of the PreCS period using a Group (sham, lesion) x Block (1–7) mixed ANOVA revealed that responding was significantly higher in the lesion group than the sham group (main effect of Group F(1, 40) = 7.24, P = 0.01). Furthermore, while responding increased over blocks (main effect of Block F(6, 240) = 20.37, P < 0.001; positive linear trend F(1, 40) = 33.18, P < 0.001), this increase was greater in the lesion than the sham group (Block x Group interaction F(6, 240) = 2.52, P = 0.02; linear trend interaction F(1, 40) = 5.34, P = 0.03). During the first block, PreCS responding was similar between groups (Sham M = 2.07, SD = 0.60; Lesion M = 2.13, SD = 0.90), by the final block PreCS responding was higher in the Lesion group (M = 4.30, SD = 1.95) than the Sham group (M = 2.76, SD = 2.30).
Acquisition
Pretraining OFC lesions significantly increased responding to the Pavlovian cue relative to sham control animals (Fig. 1A; lesions depicted in Supplementary Figure S1 [supplement 1]). Analysis of conditioned responding was conducted as a CS–PreCS difference score such that levels of responding reflected discriminative performance to the cue (CS) above baseline (PreCS). Acquisition of responding to the CS was significantly greater in the lesion group than the sham group (main effect of Group , , Block , , and Group x Block interaction , ). Follow up comparisons on each block revealed that responding in the lesion group was significantly higher than the sham group during the last 4 blocks (Block 1 , , Block 2 , , Block 3 , , Block 4 , , Block 5 , , Block 6 , , Block 7 , ). Given the ubiquity of nonsignificant effects of OFC lesions on acquisition learning in the literature, two independent replications of this novel effect were conducted (combined here; same pattern of statistical significance in both independent replications) which confirmed the effect was robust.
Locomotor Activity
The enhanced responding observed during acquisition in the OFC lesion group could simply reflect an enhancement of general locomotor activity. However, locomotor activity (Fig. 1C) did not differ between groups (main effect of TimeBin , , but no significant effect of Group , , or Group x TimeBin interaction , ). Therefore, the enhanced responding during acquisition was not simply due to lateral OFC lesions inducing hyperactivity, consistent with previous findings (e.g., Lasseter et al. 2009; Panayi and Killcross 2018).
General Satiety
To test whether the enhanced responding following pretraining OFC lesions was sensitive to levels of hunger or shifts in general motivation, a subgroup of animals (subgroup 1) was tested when sated, that is, following 24 h ad libitum access to home-cage food (Fig. 1B). General satiety, did not affect the rate of responding in the sham group (Sham: Satiety vs. Hungry , ) but significantly suppressed responding in the lesion group (Lesion: Satiety vs. Hungry , ) compared with subsequent testing 24 h later when hungry again (no significant main effect of Group , , but a significant main effect of Hunger , , and Group x Hunger interaction , ). Since the satiety test session was rewarded, it is possible that OFC lesioned animals could learn that the reward was less valuable by direct experience with the reward, similar to incentive learning effects normally observed in instrumental conditioning (Dickinson and Balleine 2002). However, this possibility is unlikely as responding was comparable between groups on the first trial of the satiety test (, , Supplementary Figure S1 [supplement 2]), before the first reward was delivered. This suggests that, consistent with previous reports (e.g., McDannald et al. 2005), animals with lateral OFC lesions are sensitive to shifts in hunger and general motivation.
Devaluation Test
OFC lesions have been shown to cause characteristic deficits in Pavlovian outcome devaluation (Gallagher et al. 1999; Pickens et al. 2003, 2005; Panayi and Killcross 2018). Therefore, to test whether the present lesion manipulation was comparable with other reports, we tested a subgroup of animals (subgroup 2) on Pavlovian outcome devaluation. First, the sham and lesion animals were given novel acquisition training of two novel and unique cue-outcome relationship (see Supplementary Figure S1 [supplement 3A]). A specific taste aversion was then established by pairing consumption of one of the outcomes with illness (i.p. injection of lithium chloride; Devalued), and the value of the other outcome was left intact (i.p. injection of saline; Nondevalued). Both groups learned the novel cue-outcome associations and acquired the specific taste aversion (see Supplementary Figure S1 [supplement 3B]).
Finally, during a devaluation test (Fig. 1D), the two cues were presented in extinction. The sham group showed a significant devaluation effect, that is, responding was lower to the devalued than nondevalued cue (, ). In contrast, the devaluation effect was abolished in the lesion group, and responding remained high to both the devalued and nondevalued cue (, ; Significant Group x Cue interaction , , but no main effect of Group , , or Cue , ). This finding successfully replicates the finding that both complete OFC and focal lateral OFC lesions abolish the outcome devaluation effect in rodents (Gallagher et al. 1999; Pickens et al. 2003, 2005; Panayi and Killcross 2018).
Experiment 2: Post-Training Muscimol Inactivation
Histology and Group Allocation
Cannulae placements are illustrated in Supplementary Figure S2 (supplement 2). One animal did not recover from surgery and was excluded. Three animals were excluded because of the cannulae assembly detaching from the skull. A further 3 animals were excluded because of failing to consume the pellets after recovery from surgery. One animal from the muscimol group was excluded from analysis because of a cannula tip embedded within the white matter of the forceps minor of the corpus callosum. Therefore, a total of 8 animals were excluded leaving N = 24 (saline n = 12, muscimol n = 12).
PreCS Rates
PreCS baseline responding did not differ between infusion groups across training. In particular, during the infusion period a Group x Day (4 days) mixed ANOVA on PreCS responses revealed a significant effect of Day (F(3, 66) = 5.95, P = 0.001) but no significant effect of Group (F(1, 22) = 0.01, P = 0.93) or Group x Day interaction (F(3, 66) = 0.41, P = 0.741). PreCS response rates on these days were, saline M = 0.70, SD = 0.48, muscimol M = 0.72, SD = 0.48.
The enhanced Pavlovian responding observed following OFC lesions (Fig. 1A) may be due to enhanced learning of a general cue-outcome predictive relationship in the OFC lesion group (see Supplementary Figure S2 [supplement 1]). This is consistent with a role for the OFC in representing outcome expectancy information. For example, incremental learning about a cue-outcome relationship is thought to depend upon prediction errors (Rescorla and Wagner 1972; Mackintosh 1975; Pearce and Hall 1980; Sutton and Barto 1998; LePelley 2004; Esber and Haselgrove 2011; Nasser et al. 2017), that is, the difference between the experience outcome value and the expected outcome value. The expected outcome value of a cue is incrementally updated until this prediction error discrepancy is minimized. If the OFC carries some aspect of outcome expectancy information (Baxter et al. 2000; Pears et al. 2003; Schoenbaum et al. 2009; Takahashi et al. 2009, 2011), then OFC lesions might consistently reduce/underestimate the expected value of a cue which in turn would result in abnormally persistent prediction errors and enhanced learning. Therefore, disruption of OFC function should temporarily lower expected value, and enhance prediction errors and learning supported by other brain regions (for modeling of this prediction see Supplementary Figure S2 [supplement 1]). We tested this hypothesis by inactivating the OFC after first successfully acquiring cue-outcome learning, that is, when expected value is high and prediction errors are low. If the OFC carries some aspect of the learned expected value, then inactivation of the OFC should restore prediction errors at the time of reward and responding should increase to reflect new learning. Following this, returning function to the OFC should result in an overexpectation of the value of the outcome, and performance should decrease to reflect the extinction of this overexpectation. Importantly, while this account is couched in terms prediction-error learning mechanisms, the prediction remains true for any account of OFC lesions enhancing learning (see Supplementary Figure S2 [supplement 1]).
We tested this hypothesis by first training a new group of animals on the same simple Pavlovian task for 9 days, before implantation of bilateral cannulae targeting the OFC (Fig. 2, days 1–9; significant main effect of day , , but no main effect of Group , , or Group x Day interaction , ). Following postoperative recovery (histology depicted in Supplementary Figure S2 [supplement 2]), and prior to infusion, response levels were similar in both groups (Fig. 2, post; no significant differences between Groups , ).
Contrary to our prediction, intra-OFC muscimol infusions disrupted rather than enhanced further acquisition of responding relative to the saline group (Fig. 2, infusion–days 12–15; significant Group x Day interaction , , but no main effect of Group , , or Day , ). Simple effects revealed significantly greater responding in the saline group on the last 2 days of infusions (muscimol vs. saline: day 12 , , day 13 , , day 14 , , day 15 , ). Furthermore, the saline group increased responding across infusion days 12–15 (saline: significant positive linear trend , ), whereas the muscimol group did not (muscimol: no significant linear trend , ). Therefore, post-training inactivation of the OFC impaired acquisition.
Postinfusion, with function returned to the OFC, the group differences observed under drug infusion were no longer apparent, and both groups continued to acquire responding at similar levels (Fig. 2, days 16–17; significant main effect of day , , but no main effect of Group , , or Group x Day interaction , ). Therefore, the effect of OFC inactivation did not persist, which suggests that the disruption in acquisition following OFC inactivation might not have impaired learning per se.
Furthermore, we tested post-training lesions to rule out the possibility that the differences between pre- and post-training OFC manipulations were simply due to differences in the method of manipulation, that is, excitotoxic lesions versus inactivation using a GABA-A agonist. Consistent with muscimol inactivation, post-training lesions significantly impaired Pavlovian acquisition (see Supplementary Experiment 1: Supplementary Figure S2 [supplements 3 and 4]). Therefore, it is unlikely that the difference between pre- and post-training OFC manipulations observed in Experiments 1 and 2 are due to the method of manipulation.
Experiment 3: OFC Inactivation Prior to Associative Blocking
OFC inactivation during acquisition suppressed cue responding, but it is unclear if this reduction in behavior is due to suppression of additional learning or behavioral performance (Fig. 2). This ambiguity is predominantly driven by the assumption that an animal’s response levels represent some monotonic function of acquired learning (Rescorla and Wagner 1972; Mackintosh 1975; Pearce and Hall 1980; Wagner 1981; Sutton and Barto 1998). To disambiguate learning from performance effects, we employed an associative blocking design (Fig. 3A). In a blocking experiment, first an animal is trained such that a cue (cue A) predicts an outcome (pellet). Next, A is presented in compound with a novel cue (cue B) which also leads to the same pellet outcome. If the animal has learned that cue A sufficiently predicts the pellet outcome already, then very little is learned about cue B, that is, learning about cue A blocks subsequent learning about cue B (Kamin 1969). However, if learning about cue A is insufficient, then learning about cue B should not be blocked. We predicted that if OFC inactivation is disrupting learning, then OFC inactivation during initial learning about cue A should disrupt the blocking effect.
First, we demonstrated again that OFC inactivation significantly impairs acquisition in a new cohort of animals using similar parameters to those required for the associative blocking design (inactivation from days 5 to 9 of acquisition with a 10 s visual CS; Supplementary Experiment 2: Supplementary Figure S3 [supplements 1 and 2]). Again, OFC inactivation significantly impaired acquisition, confirming that the effects observed in Figure 2 are not dependent on a specific cue modality or duration.
Associative Blocking
Next, in a different cohort of animals, we tested whether impaired CS–US acquisition following OFC inactivation disrupted subsequent Pavlovian blocking.
Histology and Group Allocation
Cannulae placements are illustrated in Supplementary Figure S3 (supplement 3). One animal failed to consume pellets throughout the experiment and was excluded from testing. One animal from the muscimol group lost its cannula assembly during the infusion period and was excluded from testing. One animal in the muscimol group was euthanized due to severe illness. A further 2 animals were excluded after histological analysis revealed that the cannulae were only unilaterally targeting DLO and LO. Therefore, a total of 6 animals were excluded leaving N = 26 (saline n = 13, muscimol n = 13).
PreCS Responding
Baseline levels of responding did not differ between groups during training, and on the final day of infusions (day 10 of stage 1) PreCS response rates (per 10 s) were saline M = 0.122, SD = 0.24, muscimol M = 0.67, SD = 0.87. These observations were supported by mixed Group x Day ANOVAs on PreCS responding in stage1 suggesting that there were no group differences on days 1–4 prior to infusion (all F < 1.69, P > 0.21) or on days 5–10 during infusions (significant main effect of Day F(5, 120) = 15.21, P < 0.001, all remaining F < 1.00, P > 0.50).
Stage 1
During stage 1 of blocking (Fig. 3B), all animals were given 10 days of acquisition training to cue A. OFC function was intact during the first 4 days of acquisition, and all animals began to acquire the cue A-outcome relationship (days 1–4: significant main effect of day , , but no effect of Group, or Group x Day interaction , ). All animals then received an additional 6 days of acquisition to cue A (Fig. 3B, days 5–10) following either intra-OFC infusions of muscimol or saline. Infusions of muscimol depressed overall responding relative to saline infusions (significant main effect of Group , , and day , , but no Group x Day interaction , ). Importantly, on the final day (day 10), responding in the muscimol group was significantly lower than the saline group (, ).
Stage 2
Next, animals were trained such that compounds AB and CD also predicted reward (Fig. 3C, Stage 2), importantly OFC function was intact in all animals, that is, no infusions. Responding in both the saline and muscimol groups was initially lower to the novel compound CD than to AB (significant Cue x Day interaction , , and main effect of day , , but no other main effects or interactions with Group were significant, all remaining effects F < 1.91, P > 0.160; Cue AB vs. CD: day 12 , , day 13 , , day 14 , ). However, the pattern of means suggests that responding to compound AB in the muscimol group was similar to the novel compound CD on day 12 (Fig. 3C, right—day 12, Muscimol: AB vs. CD , ), and lower than compound AB in the saline group (Fig. 3C, left—day 12; day 12, saline: AB vs. CD , ). Furthermore, within-session changes over trials on day 12 revealed rapid within-session acquisition to both compounds in both groups, but responding was significantly lower in the muscimol group at the start of the session (see Supplementary Figure S3 [supplement 4]; first 2 trials, significant main effect of Group , , and Cue , , but no Group x Cue interaction , ). The lower responding to cue AB in the muscimol group suggests that acquisition to cue A was impaired following infusions in Stage 1 and this impairment persisted (albeit transiently) when test drug free in stage 2. Indeed, the levels of responding to compound AB in the muscimol group at the start of day 12 (see Supplementary Figure S3 [supplement 4]) are similar to levels of responding to the novel compound CD in the saline group. This would suggest that learning about cue A in the muscimol group was impaired in stage 1, and therefore cue A should not effectively block learning to cue B in stage 2.
Test
At test both groups showed significant blocking of learning to cue B relative to the control cue D (Fig. 3D; Significant main effect of Cue , , but no main effect of Group , , or Group x Cue interaction , ). This suggests that inactivation of the OFC significantly reduced behavioral performance but not learning to cue A in Stage 1, and this impairment transiently affected compound AB on day 12 in the absence of OFC inactivation. Therefore, the impairments observed in our earlier findings (Fig. 2A, post infusion) are unlikely to be due to impairments in learning. In addition to this, we rule out the possibility that the two groups used different attentional solutions to achieve a similar blocking result (see Supplementary Figure S3 [supplement 5]).
Discussion
The present studies tested the hypothesis that the rodent lateral OFC is not necessary for Pavlovian acquisition in simple single CS–US procedure. Here, we show that OFC lesions and inactivation significantly affects Pavlovian acquisition. Furthermore, we found a dissociation between pre- and post-training OFC manipulations on Pavlovian acquisition such that pretraining OFC lesions enhance, whereas post-training lesions and inactivation impairs acquisition behavior. Given the absence of these effects in the extant literature, it is notable that these effects were robust and were replicated multiple times. Next, using an associative blocking design, we tested whether impaired behavior following post-training OFC inactivation reflects a disruption of learning or behavioral control. OFC inactivation did not disrupt the underlying learning about the predictive CS–US relationship as assayed by blocking, and instead disrupted the appropriate control of anticipatory behavior to the CS.
Lateral OFC is Necessary for Simple Pavlovian Acquisition
The significant role of the OFC in Pavlovian acquisition in the present studies is surprising since OFC lesions and inactivation have consistently been reported to have no effect on acquisition in rats (Gallagher et al. 1999; Schoenbaum et al. 2002; Stalnaker et al. 2007; e.g., Burke et al. 2008), unless there are complex cue- or outcome-specific task demands (e.g., Ramirez and Savage 2007). However, in tasks involving simple single Pavlovian CS–US procedures and pretraining OFC lesions, performance often does not reach asymptote (e.g., after 9 days, Gallagher et al. 1999) before proceeding to a new stage of the experiment. In Experiment 1, we did not observe any significant effects of OFC lesions on acquisition until around 15–21 days of acquisition. However, after extended training Schoenbaum et al. (2003) have reported significant effects of OFC lesions on acquisition in a simple cue-outcome go–nogo task when looking at response latencies, but not on trials-to-criterion. Therefore, the effects of pretraining lesions may not have been observed previously due to task specific parameters such as the length of training and the sensitivity of the response measures.
Pretraining OFC lesions have been shown to disrupt Pavlovian acquisition in sign-tracking procedures in which lever insertion is used as the CS (Chudasama and Robbins 2003). Focal lateral OFC lesions also significantly impair sign-tracking behavior (i.e., engaging with the lever cue), and bias behavior towards goal-tacking (i.e., approaching the magazine) (Panayi and Killcross 2018). The present findings that pretraining OFC lesions enhanced behavior focused towards the magazine is consistent with a deficit in sign-tracking and a bias towards goal-tracking.
In contrast to pretraining lesions, post-training OFC inactivation/lesions normally coincide with changes in experimental phase and continued acquisition is not assessed. In tasks in which OFC inactivation coincides with a change in experimental stage, the effects of OFC inactivation are consistent with an impairment in subsequent acquisition (2009). For example, Burke et al. (2009) found that post-training OFC inactivation impaired acquisition to a Pavlovian CS in reversal task. Similarly, Takahashi et al. (2009) found that OFC inactivation during a Pavlovian overexpectation task disrupted new learning. Therefore, the robust effect of impaired acquisition following post-training OFC inactivation that we report is consistent with impaired subsequent acquisition in tasks with more complex manipulations.
Lateral OFC is not Necessary for Learning the Predictive CS–US Relationship
Post-training OFC inactivation significantly impaired acquisition behavior (Experiment 2), and this disruption was more profound when inactivation occurred earlier in training and more likely to persist after OFC function returned (see Supplementary Experiment 2). This seems to suggest that learning about the CS–US relationship was disrupted. The idea that the OFC could be involved in learning is also consistent with a role for the OFC in the representation of expected values (Burke et al. 2008; Schoenbaum et al. 2011; Stalnaker et al. 2018), which influence midbrain dopaminergic prediction errors (Takahashi et al. 2009, 2011), known to be necessary for Pavlovian learning (Steinberg et al. 2013; Sharpe et al. 2017).
Unexpectedly, the impaired acquisition we observed following post-training OFC disruption did not disrupt the ability of the CS to block learning about a novel cue (Experiment 3, Fig. 3), despite significantly impaired performance postinactivation (see Supplementary Figure S3 [supplement 4]; muscimol AB is as low as saline CD which does not show evidence of blocking). This is surprising given that in some Pavlovian learning contexts, levels of behavioral expression can dictate the extent to which learning occurs (Delamater 2004). This finding highlights the importance of using multiple tests of learning (Rescorla 2002a, 2002b) to assess disrupted acquisition effects.
Intact blocking despite impaired acquisition behavior suggests that OFC inactivation did not disrupt the underlying learning about the associative strength of the CS–US relationship. Associative blocking is often used to assess the role of prediction-error based learning (e.g., Steinberg et al. 2013; Sharpe et al. 2017), suggesting that the OFC is not necessary for this aspect of Pavlovian learning. This distinction suggests that the learned value of a Pavlovian CS–US association might be independent of the current expected or subjective value of expected reward. Informally, learning whether an outcome will be delivered might reasonably be separate from learning the subjective value or identity of that outcome (Delamater 2007; Delamater and Oakeshott 2007; McDannald et al. 2011; Zhou et al. 2019). Indeed, the present findings are consistent with reports that pretraining OFC lesions do not disrupt the blocking effect (McDannald et al. 2011), and neural activity in the lateral OFC in blocking procedures appears to predominantly track sensory specific features of the US (McDannald et al. 2014; Lopatina et al. 2015).
Pre- vs. Post-Training Effects
The dissociable and opposite effects of pre- and post-training OFC lesions/inactivation on acquisition were surprising and rule out a simple account of OFC dysfunction in terms of prediction-error based learning impairments (see Supplementary Figure S2 [supplement 1]). One possibility is that pretraining lesions result in compensatory function such that learning is supported by other neural systems. In contrast, post-training lesions and inactivation disrupts learning/behavior that has been acquired in an OFC dependent manner. This argument has been proposed when only pretraining OFC lesions (Boulougouris et al. 2007; Boulougouris and Robbins 2009), or only post-training OFC lesions disrupt behavior (Ostlund and Balleine 2007; Balleine et al. 2011). We will also consider two alternative accounts of pre- versus post-training OFC lesion differences based on theoretical accounts of OFC function, sensory-specific outcome expectancy and latent state theories. Note that these theories do not predict an effect of OFC lesions on simple Pavlovian acquisition a priori, and therefore require additional assumptions to account for the present data.
From an associative learning framework, even putatively “simple” single cue-outcome Pavlovian learning can involve a number of different psychological/behavioral processes (Konorski 1967; Mackintosh 1974; Holland 1977; Dickinson 1980; Rescorla 1988; Hall 2002). Take for example a 10 s light cue that reliably predicts the delivery of a pellet reward. A rat can learn that the cue predicts the sensory-specific properties of the outcome (e.g., taste, texture, sweetness, color, size, location etc.), or the general motivational value of that reward, or simply develop a stimulus–response habit to approach the reward location when the cue is presented. Indeed, there is experimental evidence for these multiple aspects of learning occurring during Pavlovian conditioning (for review, see Delamater and Oakeshott 2007). It is possible that pretraining OFC lesions disrupt the balance of these different aspects of Pavlovian learning and behavior (Burke et al. 2007; Delamater 2007).
If the OFC is necessary for the representation of the sensory specific properties of expected outcomes, then OFC lesions might allow a stimulus–response habit system to dominate behavioral control. Following pretraining lesions, this may lead to an unconstrained habit learning system (Dickinson 1985; Coutureau and Killcross 2003; Killcross and Coutureau 2003; Dolan and Dayan 2013) that is not necessarily bounded by the current value of the outcome, and overly sensitive to current general motivational states (e.g., overall hunger levels; Figure 1B) of the organism. This is consistent with evidence that a stimulus–response habit like system develops in Pavlovian conditioning paradigms (Hall 2002; Killcross and Blundell 2002; Parkinson et al. 2005), and is likely to interact and compete with stimulus-outcome learning systems for behavioral control, similar to the interaction found between instrumental habit and goal-directed systems (Coutureau and Killcross 2003; Killcross and Coutureau 2003; Yin et al. 2005, 2006; Balleine and Killcross 2006; Lee et al. 2014; Kim et al. 2019), except that in Pavlovian conditioning the cue-outcome system often dominates in control animals (Holland et al. 2008). However, once initial learning occurs with an intact OFC, the encoding of the identity of the expected outcome is likely to have occurred (e.g., Delamater and Holland 2008). Subsequently, a post-training lesion or inactivation of the OFC is likely to affect the subsequent updating of this information. Therefore, one possible account is that the impaired acquisition behavior we observed following post-training inactivation reflects an inability to update the current motivational value of the specific outcome that is expected.
The latent state representation account of the OFC might also be able to account for the differences observed dissociation between pre- and post-training OFC lesions on acquisition. Computational models (e.g., Wilson et al. 2014) often assume, for simplicity, that in a simple single cue-outcome procedure, the cue state (e.g., “light on”) is stable throughout acquisition. Given that the same cue is presented, and it always leads to the pellet outcome, this stable representation is a reasonable assumption. However, it is also likely that early in acquisition, this state representation is not yet stable in healthy control animals (Niv 2019). How can the animal be certain that the light cue, the testing chamber context, or the reward pellet that they see on each trial is identical to the trials they have already experienced within the session, and from previous days? The subjective experience of these states and their physical features is very likely to be different within- and between-sessions, for example, the ambient noises, odors, temperature of the context, the location and intensity of the light cue based on where the rat happens to be located when it turns on, and the gradual onset of sensory specific satiety to the pellet, etc. Informally, how does the rat know that this light is the same light that they saw at the start of the session, or the day before? The perception and recognition of these states is therefore subject to differences in variables such as generalization, confidence, and certainty.
Paradoxically, in a simple and stable cue-outcome training procedure, pretraining OFC lesions may result in an accurate, but inflexible, representation of these simple task states quite rapidly. In this stable and simple training context, this could lead to enhanced Pavlovian acquisition. However, in a task with multiple or uncertain cue-outcome contingencies pretraining OFC lesions might impair acquisition (Walton et al. 2010; Stolyarova and Izquierdo 2017). However, post-training inactivation of the OFC would disrupt the ability to update already established state representations at whatever stage of certainty/stability that they have currently achieved. In the stable single cue-outcome learning situation employed in the present studies, this would result in disruption of further acquisition. Again, in a task with interference from multiple cue-outcome relationships, post-training lesions might improve performance.
Conclusion
Here, we show that the rodent lateral OFC is involved in Pavlovian acquisition learning process in an experience dependent manner. Once initial learning has taken place, the lateral OFC appears to be necessary for updating the current value of Pavlovian behaviors driven by expected outcome value. These findings raise two important issues. First, they demonstrate the importance of not interpreting a null effect of lesions on acquisition behavior as evidence that the OFC is not involved in acquisition learning. Instead, the underlying deficit in acquisition either is not being expressed or is not relevant to behavioral performance in the task yet. Second, these findings demonstrate that even within a putatively “simple” behavioral task, there are many potential underlying psychological processes that can contribute to performance and change over time. This is consistent with growing suggestions that the competition and interaction between underlying learning systems (Lee et al. 2014; e.g., Kool et al. 2018; Kim et al. 2019) is important and needs further study (Collins and Cockburn 2020).
Recently, we demonstrated functional heterogeneity within the lateral OFC between anterior and posterior subregions (Panayi and Killcross 2018). While the present experiments did not explicitly target and compare anterior and posterior subregions, it is notable that present lesion and cannula placements targeted predominantly anterior lateral OFC. Therefore, one possible account of the surprising role of lateral OFC in simple Pavlovian acquisition is that prior research has often focused on the posterior lateral OFC or the structure as a whole (Gallagher et al. 1999; Ostlund and Balleine 2007; Izquierdo 2017). However, further studies systematically comparing anterior and posterior subregions within lateral OFC are still needed.
While the OFC has often been found not to be necessary for initial acquisition learning, recently, there have been reports that simple Pavlovian acquisition is significantly impaired rather than enhanced following optogenetic inhibition of OFC function in head fixed mice (Namboodiri et al. 2019; Wang et al. 2020), in a manner that does not depend on VTA prediction error signaling. In contrast to our results, these studies target more ventral and medial OFC, which is likely to be an important anatomical distinction given the emerging evidence of functional heterogeneity within the OFC (Sharpe et al. 2015; Barreiros et al. 2020; Bradfield and Hart 2020; Barreiros et al. under review). Indeed, there appears to be dissociable but complementary roles of the medial and lateral OFC such that lateral OFC lesions disrupt Pavlovian whereas medial OFC lesions disrupt instrumental behavioral control (Ostlund and Balleine 2007; McDannald et al. 2011; Bradfield et al. 2015, 2018; Gardner et al. 2017, 2018; Panayi and Killcross 2018). This suggests that the OFC, as a whole, is engaged in the learning and flexible updating of value-based behaviors, but within the orbital subregions this process appears to be remarkably specialized for distinct types of behavior and learning.
Supplementary Material
Notes
We gratefully acknowledge Fred Westbrook, Nathan Holmes, David Bannerman, Mark Walton, Mehdi Khamassi, and Geoffrey Schoenbaum for their invaluable feedback. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing interests.
Funding
Australian Research Council (ARC Discovery Grants DP0989027 and DP120103564 to S.K.).
References
- Balleine BW, Killcross AS. 2006. Parallel incentive processing: an integrated view of amygdala function. Trends Neurosci. 29(5):272–279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine BW, Leung BK, Ostlund SB. 2011. The orbitofrontal cortex, predicted value, and choice. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1239:43–50. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barreiros IV, Panayi MC, Walton ME. 2021. Organization of afferents along the anterior-posterior and medial-lateral axes of the rat orbitofrontal cortex. Neuroscience. In Press. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.02.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baxter MG, Parker A, Lindner CC, Izquierdo AD, Murray EA. 2000. Control of response selection by reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 20(11):4311–4319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boulougouris V, Dalley JW, Robbins TW. 2007. Effects of orbitofrontal, infralimbic and prelimbic cortical lesions on serial spatial reversal learning in the rat. Behav Brain Res. 179(2):219–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boulougouris V, Robbins TW. 2009. Pre-surgical training ameliorates orbitofrontal-mediated impairments in spatial reversal learning. Behav Brain Res. 197(2):469–475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradfield LA, Dezfouli A, Van Holstein M, Chieng B, Balleine BW. 2015. Medial orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome retrieval in partially observable task situations. Neuron. 88(6):1268–1280. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradfield LA, Hart G. 2020. Rodent medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortices represent unique components of cognitive maps of task space. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 108:287–294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradfield LA, Hart G, Balleine BW. 2018. Inferring action-dependent outcome representations depends on anterior but not posterior medial orbitofrontal cortex. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 155(May):463–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke KA, Franz TM, Miller DN, Schoenbaum G. 2007. Conditioned reinforcement can be mediated by either outcome-specific or general affective representations. Front Integr Neurosci. 1:2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke KA, Franz TM, Miller DN, Schoenbaum G. 2008. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in the pursuit of happiness and more specific rewards. Nature. 454(7202):340–U45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burke KA, Takahashi YK, Correll J, Brown PL, Schoenbaum G. 2009. Orbitofrontal inactivation impairs reversal of Pavlovian learning by interfering with “disinhibition” of responding for previously unrewarded cues. Eur J Neurosci. 30(10):1941–1946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butter CM. 1969. Perseveration in extinction and in discrimination reversal tasks following selective frontal ablations in Macaca mulatta. Physiol Behav. 4:163–171. [Google Scholar]
- Chudasama Y, Robbins TW. 2003. Dissociable contributions of the orbitofrontal and infralimbic cortex to Pavlovian autoshaping and discrimination reversal learning: further evidence for the functional heterogeneity of the rodent frontal cortex. J Neurosci. 23(25):8771–8780. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins AGE, Cockburn J. 2020. Beyond dichotomies in reinforcement learning. Nat Rev Neurosci. 21(10):576–586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coutureau E, Killcross AS. 2003. Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behav Brain Res. 146(1–2):167–174. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR. 2004. Experimental extinction in Pavlovian conditioning: behavioural and neuroscience perspectives. Q J Exp Psychol Sect B-Compar Physiol Psychol. 57(2):97–132. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR. 2007. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in sensory-specific encoding of associations in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. In: Schoenbaum G, Gottfried JA, Murray EA, Ramus SJ, editors. Linking affect to action: critical contributions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Vol. 1121. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p. 152–173. [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR, Holland PC. 2008. The influence of CS-US interval on several different indices of learning in appetitive conditioning. J Exp Psychol Behav Process. 34(2):202–222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delamater AR, Oakeshott S. 2007. Learning about multiple attributes of reward in Pavlovian conditioning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dias R, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. 1996. Dissociation in prefrontal cortex of affective and attentional shifts. Nature. 380(6569):69–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. 1980. In: Gray J, editor. Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A. 1985. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 308(1135):67–78. [Google Scholar]
- Dickinson A, Balleine BW. 2002. The role of learning in the operation of motivational systems. Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology. John Wiley & Sons, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Dolan RJ, Dayan P. 2013. Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron. 80(2):312–325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Esber GR, Haselgrove M. 2011. Reconciling the influence of predictiveness and uncertainty on stimulus salience: a model of attention in associative learning. Proc R Soc B-Biol Sci. 278(1718):2553–2561. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gallagher M, McMahan RW, Schoenbaum G. 1999. Orbitofrontal cortex and representation of incentive value in associative learning. J Neurosci. 19(15):6610–6614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner MPH, Conroy JC, Sanchez DC, Zhou J, Schoenbaum G. 2019. Real-time value integration during economic choice is regulated by orbitofrontal cortex. Curr Biol. 29(24):4315–4322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner MPH, Conroy JC, Styer CV, Huynh T, Whitaker LR, Schoenbaum G. 2018. Medial orbitofrontal inactivation does not affect economic choice. Elife. 7:e38963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner MPH, Conroy JS, Shaham MH, Styer CV, Schoenbaum G. 2017. Lateral orbitofrontal inactivation dissociates devaluation-sensitive behavior and economic choice. Neuron. 96(5):1192–1203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hall G. 2002. Associative structures in Pavlovian and instrumental conditioning. In: Gallistel CR, editor. Steven’s handbook of experimental psychology. Vol. 3. New York: John Wiley & Sons. p. 1–45. [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC. 1977. Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the Pavlovian conditioned response. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 3(1):77–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holland PC, Lasseter H, Agarwal I. 2008. Amount of training and cue-evoked taste-reactivity responding in reinforcer devaluation. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 34(1):119–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iversen SD, Mishkin M. 1970. Perseverative interference in monkeys following selective lesions of the inferior prefrontal convexity. Exp Brain Res. 11(4):376–386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Izquierdo AD. 2017. Functional heterogeneity within rat orbitofrontal cortex in reward learning and decision making. J Neurosci. 37(44):10529–10540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamin LJ. 1969. Predictability, surprise, attention and conditioning. In: Campbell BA, Church RM, editors. Punishment and aversive behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. p. 279–296. [Google Scholar]
- Killcross AS, Blundell P. 2002. Associative representations of emotionally significant outcomes. In: Moore SC, Oaksford M, editors. Emotional cognition: from brain to behaviour. Vol. 44. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. p. 35–74. [Google Scholar]
- Killcross AS, Coutureau E. 2003. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex. 13(4):400–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim D, Park GY, O’Doherty JP, Lee SW. 2019. Task complexity interacts with state-space uncertainty in the arbitration between model-based and model-free learning. Nat Commun. 10:5738. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klein-Flugge MC, Barron HC, Brodersen KH, Dolan RJ, Behrens TE. 2013. Segregated encoding of reward-identity and stimulus-reward associations in human orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 33:3202–3211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konorski J. 1967. Integrative activity of the brain; an interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kool W, Cushman FA, Gershman SJ. 2018. Competition and cooperation between multiple reinforcement learning systems. Elsevier Inc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kringelbach ML. 2005. The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 6(9):691–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lasseter HC, Ramirez DR, Xie X, Fuchs RA. 2009. Involvement of the lateral orbitofrontal cortex in drug context-induced reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior in rats. Eur J Neurosci. 30(7):1370–1381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S, Shimojo S, O’Doherty J. 2014. Neural computations underlying arbitration between model-based and model-free learning. Neuron. 81(3):687–699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lenth R, Singmann H, Love J, Buerkner P, Herve M. 2020. emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means. R package version 1.5.2–1. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans. [Google Scholar]
- LePelley ME. 2004. The role of associative history in models of associative learning: a selective review and a hybrid model. Q J Exp Psychol. 57B:192–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lopatina N, McDannald MA, Steyer CV, Sadacca BF, Cheer JF, Schoenbaum G. 2015. Lateral orbitofrontal neurons acquire responses to upshifted, downshifted or blocked cues during unblocking. Elife. 4:e11299. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ. 1974. The psychology of animal learning. London, UK: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ. 1975. A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 82(4):279–298. [Google Scholar]
- McDannald MA, Esber GR, Wegener MA, Wied HM, Tzu-Lan L, Stalnaker TA, Jones JL, Trageser J, Schoenbaum G. 2014. Orbitofrontal neurons acquire responses to “valueless” Pavlovian cues during unblocking. Elife. 3:e02653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDannald MA, Lucantonio F, Burke KA, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. 2011. Ventral striatum and orbitofrontal cortex are both required for model-based, but not model-free, reinforcement learning. J Neurosci. 31:2700–2705. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDannald MA, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC. 2005. Lesions of orbitofrontal cortex impair rats’ differential outcome expectancy learning but not conditioned stimulus-potentiated feeding. J Neurosci. 25(18):4626–4632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, O’Doherty JP, Schoenbaum G. 2007. What we know and do not know about the functions of the orbitofrontal cortex after 20 years of cross-species studies. J Neurosci. 27(31):8166–8169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray EA, Rudebeck PH. 2018. Specializations for reward-guided decision-making in the primate ventral prefrontal cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci. 19(7):404–417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Namboodiri VMK, Otis JM, Heeswijk K, Voets ES, Alghorazi RA, Rodriguez-Romaguera J, Mihalas S, Stuber GD. 2019. Single-cell activity tracking reveals that orbitofrontal neurons acquire and maintain a long-term memory to guide behavioral adaptation. Nat Neurosci. 22(7):1110–1121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nasser HM, Calu DJ, Schoenbaum G, Sharpe MJ. 2017. The dopamine prediction error: contributions to associative models of reward learning. Front Psychol. 8:244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Niv Y. 2019. Learning task-state representations. Nat Neurosci. 22(10):1544–1553. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogawa M, Meer MAA, Esber GR, Cerri DH, Stalnaker TA, Schoenbaum G. 2013. Risk-responsive orbitofrontal neurons track acquired salience. Neuron. 77(2):251–258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ostlund SB, Balleine BW. 2007. Orbitofrontal cortex mediates outcome encoding in Pavlovian but not instrumental conditioning. J Neurosci. 27(18):4819–4825. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Padoa-Schioppa C. 2009. Range-adapting representation of economic value in the orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 29:14004–14014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Panayi MC, Killcross S. 2018. Functional heterogeneity within the rodent lateral orbitofrontal cortex dissociates outcome devaluation and reversal learning deficits. Elife. 7:e37357. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parkinson JA, Roberts AC, Everitt BJ, Di Ciano P. 2005. Acquisition of instrumental conditioned reinforcement is resistant to the devaluation of the unconditioned stimulus. Q J Exp Psychol Sect B. 58(1b):19–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearce JM, Hall G. 1980. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev. 87(6):532–552. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pears A, Parkinson JA, Hopewell L, Everitt BJ, Roberts AC. 2003. Lesions of the orbitofrontal but not medial prefrontal cortex disrupt conditioned reinforcement in primates. J Neurosci. 23(35):11189–11201. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M, Holland PC. 2005. Orbitofrontal lesions impair use of cue-outcome associations in a devaluation task. Behav Neurosci. 119(1):317–322. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pickens CL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B, Gallagher M, Holland PC, Schoenbaum G. 2003. Different roles for orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala in a reinforcer devaluation task. J Neurosci. 23(35):11078–11084. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team . 2020. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Austria: R A Lang Environ Stat Comput R Found Stat Comput Vienna. [Google Scholar]
- Ramirez DR, Savage LM. 2007. Differential involvement of the basolateral amygdala, orbitofrontal cortex, and nucleus accumbens core in the acquisition and use of reward expectancies. Behav Neurosci. 121(5):896–906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 1988. Pavlovian conditioning - its not what you think it is. Am Psychol. 43(3):151–160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 2002a. Comparison of the rates of associative change during acquisition and extinction. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 28(4):406–415. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 2002b. Savings tests: separating differences in rate of learning from differences in initial levels. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 28(4):369–377. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditiong: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Black AH, Prokesy WF, editors. Classical conditioning II: current research and theory. New York: Appleton Century Crofts. p. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
- Rudebeck PH, Murray EA. 2014. The orbitofrontal oracle: cortical mechanisms for the prediction and evaluation of specific behavioral outcomes. Neuron. 84(6):1143–1156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadacca BF, Wied HM, Lopatina N, Saini GK, Nemirovsky D, Schoenbaum G. 2018. Orbitofrontal neurons signal sensory associations underlying model-based inference in a sensory preconditioning task. Elife. 7:e30373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Nugent SL, Saddoris MP, Setlow B. 2002. Orbitofrontal lesions in rats impair reversal but not acquisition of go, no-go odor discriminations. Neuroreport. 13(6):885–890. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Takahashi YK. 2009. A new perspective on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex in adaptive behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. 10(12):885–892. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Setlow B, Nugent SL, Saddoris MP, Gallagher M. 2003. Lesions of orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala complex disrupt acquisition of odor-guided discriminations and reversals. Learn Mem. 10(2):129–140. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoenbaum G, Takahashi YK, Liu T, McDannald MA. 2011. Does the orbitofrontal cortex signal value? Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1239(1):87–99. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W. 1998. Predictive reward signal of dopamine neurons. J Neurophysiol. 80(1):1–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe MJ, Chang CY, Liu MA, Batchelor HM, Mueller LE, Jones JL, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. 2017. Dopamine transients are sufficient and necessary for acquisition of model-based associations. Nat Neurosci. 20(5):735–742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sharpe MJ, Wikenheiser AM, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. 2015. The state of the orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron. 88(6):1075–1077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singmann H, Bolker B, Westfall J, Aust F, Mattan SB-S. 2020. afex: Analysis of Factorial Experiments. R package version 0.28–0. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=afex.
- Stalnaker TA, Cooch NK, McDannald MA, Liu TL, Wied H, Schoenbaum G, Tzu-Lan L. 2014. Orbitofrontal neurons infer the value and identity of predicted outcomes. Nat Commun. 5:3926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Cooch NK, Schoenbaum G. 2015. What the orbitofrontal cortex does not do. Nat Neurosci. 18(5):620–627. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Franz TM, Singh T, Schoenbaum G. 2007. Basolateral amygdala lesions abolish orbitofrontal-dependent reversal impairments. Neuron. 54(1):51–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker TA, Liu T-L, Takahashi YK, Schoenbaum G. 2018. Orbitofrontal neurons signal reward predictions, not reward prediction errors. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 153:137–143. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg EE, Keiflin R, Boivin JR, Witten IB, Deisseroth K, Janak PH. 2013. A causal link between prediction errors, dopamine neurons and learning. Nat Neurosci. 16(7):966–973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stolyarova A, Izquierdo AD. 2017. Complementary contributions of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex to value learning under uncertainty. Elife. 6:e27483. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, Barto AG. 1998. Reinforcement learning: an introduction. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi YK, Chang CY, Lucantonio F, Haney RZ, Berg BA, Yau H-J, Bonci A, Schoenbaum G. 2013. Neural estimates of imagined outcomes in the orbitofrontal cortex drive behavior and learning. Neuron. 80:507–518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi YK, Roesch MR, Stalnaker TA, Haney RZ, Caiu DJ, Taylor AR, Burke KA, Schoenbaum G, Calu DJ. 2009. The orbitofrontal cortex and ventral tegmental area are necessary for learning from unexpected outcomes. Neuron. 62(2):269–280. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi YK, Roesch MR, Wilson RC, Toreson K, O’Donnell P, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. 2011. Expectancy-related changes in firing of dopamine neurons depend on orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 14(12):1590–1597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagner AR. 1981. SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behavior. In: Spear NE, editor. Information processing in animals: memory mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. p. 5–47. [Google Scholar]
- Walton ME, Behrens TE, Buckley MJ, Rudebeck PH, Rushworth MF. 2010. Separable learning systems in the macaque brain and the role of orbitofrontal cortex in contingent learning. Neuron. 65(6):927–939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walton ME, Behrens TE, Noonan MP, Rushworth MF. 2011. Giving credit where credit is due: orbitofrontal cortex and valuation in an uncertain world. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1239:14–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wang PY, Boboila C, Chin M, Higashi-Howard A, Shamash P, Wu Z, Stein NP, Abbott LF, Axel R. 2020. Transient and persistent representations of odor value in prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 108(1):209–224. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- West EA, DesJardin JT, Gale K, Malkova L. 2011. Transient inactivation of orbitofrontal cortex blocks reinforcer devaluation in macaques. J Neurosci. 31(42):15128–15135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson RC, Takahashi YK, Schoenbaum G, Niv Y. 2014. Orbitofrontal cortex as a cognitive map of task space. Neuron. 81(2):267–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. 2006. Inactivation of dorsolateral striatum enhances sensitivity to changes in the action-outcome contingency in instrumental conditioning. Behav Brain Res. 166(2):189–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yin HH, Ostlund SB, Knowlton BJ, Balleine BW. 2005. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci. 22(2):513–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou J, Gardner MPH, Stalnaker TA, Ramus SJ, Wikenheiser AM, Niv Y, Schoenbaum G. 2019. Rat orbitofrontal ensemble activity contains multiplexed but dissociable representations of value and task structure in an odor sequence task. Curr Biol. 29(6):897–907. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Raw data available at: Panayi MC. 1 February 2021. Data for: The role of the rodent lateral OFC in simple Pavlovian cue-outcome learning depends on training experience. doi: 10.17605/OSF.IO/TNBH7.