Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2021 May 27;16(5):e0252324. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252324

What is good grief support? Exploring the actors and actions in social support after traumatic grief

Joanne Cacciatore 1,*, Kara Thieleman 2, Ruth Fretts 3, Lori Barnes Jackson 4
Editor: Manuel Fernández-Alcántara5
PMCID: PMC8158955  PMID: 34043716

Abstract

Social support seems to enhance wellbeing and health in many populations. Conversely, poor social support and loneliness are a social determinant of poor health outcomes and can adversely affect physical, emotional, and mental well-being. Social support is especially important in traumatic grief. However, the ways in which grieving individuals interpret and define social support is not well understood, and little is known about what specific behaviours are perceived as helpful. Using qualitative description and content analysis, this study assessed bereaved individuals’ satisfaction of social support in traumatic grief, using four categories of social support as a framework. Findings suggest inadequate satisfaction from professional, familial, and community support. Pets emerged with the most satisfactory ratings. Further, findings suggest that emotional support is the most desired type of support following traumatic loss. Implications for supporting bereaved individuals within and beyond the context of the COVID-19 pandemic are discussed.

Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has resulted in untimely deaths and social isolation for many people throughout the world, with some researchers expressing a concern for increased prevalence in prolonged grief disorder [1]. In addition, the culture-specific rituals around deaths have been disrupted, with many unable to grieve with family and friends [2]. Eisma et al. [1] call for improvements to care because bereavement, particularly when prolonged and unattended, is a serious concern for public health, and adverse mental and physical health outcomes have been well documented in the literature even prior to the pandemic [3, 4].

Social support has been studied as a means to enhance human health and well-being, and can be provided at low cost within medical, mental health, and community systems. Beginning in the 1980s, it was recognized that the lack of meaningful social relationships (or lack of meaningful relatedness) is a social determinant of poor health outcomes, associated with an increased risk of death from a variety of causes [5]. It is now more widely accepted that poor social support and loneliness can adversely affect physical, emotional, and mental well-being [68], is linked to an increased premature mortality risk [9, 10] and correlates to adverse health outcomes such as depression, psychosocial maladjustment, poor coping behavior, low health promotion behavior, compromised well-being, reduced quality of life, and self-actualization [11]. Loneliness, specifically, has been called “the 21st century social determinant” of poor outcomes in health.

There is also an inverse benefit to feeling supported and connected, particularly when faced with stressful life challenges. Social support improves mental, physical, and emotional health outcomes and has been studied extensively within varying disciplines over the last 40 years with similar results [12, 13]. Strong social support safeguards against the negative psychological and physiological responses to stress; it is a buffer of protection that aids coping. For example, social support improves cardiovascular, endocrine, and immune system health as measured by blood pressure, heart rate, epinephrine, norepinephrine, and cortisol levels and immune cells and antibodies [14].

Research on bereavement suggests that loneliness and inadequate social support are common, giving rise to concerns about the risks to emotional, mental, and physical health for grievers [4, 15]. For instance, some scholars have found a relationship between loneliness and post-bereavement depressive symptoms, adding to the global burden of mental illness [16]. This situation has been worsened considerably by the COVID-19 pandemic. Studies have reported a significant increase in loneliness during the pandemic [1719], with links to increases in depression, suicidality, and other mental health concerns [17, 18]. Restrictions on social activities have been associated with higher rates of loneliness [17] and measures such as quarantine, while reducing the spread of the illness, have been associated with a number of adverse psychological effects [20]. Authors of many studies on loneliness and COVID-19 have called for increased attention to addressing loneliness and related mental health concerns (e.g., [17, 18, 21]) that are unfolding along with the health crisis, while others have highlighted the additional stressors faced by grievers during pandemics and the down-stream consequences of burn out and traumatic stress [22].

Research has also explored social support in grief as salutary, as a mediator for proactive coping [23] and as a means to mitigate both the intensity and duration of psychological distress and poor physiological outcomes [3]. Some studies show that both the quantity and quality of social support may influence well-being for grievers [24]. Bereaved people who have more frequent contact with family and friends tend to report better quality of life, whether this support comes through technology (email and the internet) or in-person [25]. However, the number of contacts in a person’s social network does not always predict well-being, pointing to the importance of the quality of relationships [26].

Meanwhile, other scholars note inconsistencies and limitations in the research supporting the notion that social support mitigates poor outcomes, particularly long term, in populations without what is commonly termed “complicated grief” [27]. In the only systematic review of its kind on traumatic grief and social support, the authors noted that current studies demonstrate “inconsistencies in quantitative conceptualisation of the measurement of social support” [28] and call for more qualitative research to provide “valuable insights to the bereavement experience in social settings” [28].

What is social support?

There are four categories of social support defined in the literature that have been widely used over the past four decades. Structures and processes of social support, broadly speaking, include: 1) informational, 2) instrumental, 3) appraisal, and 4) emotional, all of which contribute to a sense of perceived and actual connection to a caring social system [29]. Intuitively, there are needs related to loss that traverse all four categories of social support [30], such as funeral planning, financial aid, trauma and crisis care, and emotional experiences related to the loss. Different groups may benefit more from some types of support than from others, depending on their needs and circumstances [31]. Informational support may include logistical help on available services after death as well as advice, data, and information offered during a difficult or stressful time. Instrumental support is actionable aid that helps with specific tasks or provides necessary physical support such as food, shelter, transportation, and financial aid. Appraisal support is a more passive means of self-evaluation often enacted, for example, in peer-to-peer contact. It provides a means to assess one’s self in a particular circumstance through like-others utilizing affirmation, feedback, and social equality [32, 33]. Finally, emotional support occurs through expressions of caring, compassion, trust building, and mutuality [32]. However, the conceptualization of these categories often lacks specificity in published studies [33, 34], perhaps especially in the scholarship of social support after trauma and grief.

Social support and loneliness in grief

Pitman et al. [35] identified thwarted belongingness, withdrawal from others, lack of connection, and loneliness as significant risk factors for suicidality in grievers. The increased risk of suicide attempts for those experiencing grief was maintained even when controlling for cause of death and social stigma. Strong social support for those dealing with intense grief may not accelerate recovery [36], but it may improve the capacity to cope [37]. Typically, much of the responsibility for grief support falls on family and friends [38]. Yet, bereaved people often report insufficient support from these sources [39]. While each person has unique needs, those who have lost a spouse at an early age or a child often need more support than family or friends are typically able to give and may benefit the most from grief counseling or support groups [40]. Additionally, while the death of a loved one sometimes strengthens family relationships, often these relationships are strained, as family members may also be grieving, adding to the psychological distress of the bereaved; additionally, much of the support offered in early grief diminishes quickly [41] while the need for support continues.

Studies of the effects of grief support groups have reported mixed results [42], perhaps because few studies have examined specific behaviors in the group setting that are perceived as supportive. The same mixed results can be found in research around grief counseling [43], perhaps because what constitutes effective grief counseling is not well understood or defined in pedagogical models for providers [44]. Medical professionals, too, often have little training in dealing with the grief of their patients [45]. Overall, many providers who are present at the time of death are ill prepared to provide sufficient crisis or long-term support to bereaved individuals [46].

In sum, social support seems to help some bereaved people, particularly those with traumatic grief, that is, violent or sudden death of a close loved one or the death of a child, cope with psychological distress, while its absence may exacerbate poor physical and psychological outcomes [3]. Yet, a breakdown in social relationships after a loss is not uncommon [41], and loneliness- particularly salient during the COVID-19 pandemic- may exaggerate that effect for grievers [1], increasing the risk for poor outcomes. Despite findings that perceived social support mitigates risk in bereaved individuals [47], a dearth of qualitative research exists in understanding the specifics of this supportive care [28]. This study seeks to more specifically define good grief support and related actions and actors in this process.

Method

Written consent was obtained and all data were collected anonymously following Institutional Review Board approval at Arizona State University. This cross sectional, qualitative descriptive (QD) study explored ways in which grieving individuals interpret and define social support and its actions and actors after the death of a child, parent, partner, or sibling in adults over the age of 18.

QD is of particular utility in illuminating “poorly understood phenomenon” and commonly used in qualitative methods [48]. Qualitative description helps researchers construct particular meaning through respondents’ descriptions of an event or experience [49, 50]. According to Tracy [51], excellent qualitative research should culminate in nuanced and rich data while meeting the following eight criteria: a worthy topic that is both relevant and timely, rich rigor, sincerity, credibility, resonance, significant contribution, ethics, and meaningful coherence. LaDonna, Taylor, and Lingard [52] note that “context, personal meaning, emotional and social nuances, and layers of detail” are what enrich our understanding of qualitative data (p. 348).

To that end, four open-ended, qualitative questions were developed in order to allow us to explore the actors and actions in social support after traumatic grief both within the context of acute crisis and in long term grief support: 1) What does social support mean to you? 2) What actions or responses felt supportive to you? 3) What actions or responses felt unsupportive to you? 4) How might others in your social support system do better to help you? These questions were carefully crafted to be simple, “focused and appropriate” in order to support “analytical procedures that offer robust insights into the social phenomena being explored” [52].

Practically, text boxes provided unlimited space for participants to expound on their subjective experiences, and these responses were a ‘central focus’ of meaning in the analyses [52]. Quantitative data, using a Likert scale, were also gathered to measure subjective satisfaction with both acute and long-term overall social support in the aftermath of traumatic grief. The survey utilized skip-logic, whereby participants were asked some questions only if they met criteria set by a specific previous question. The survey asked demographic and loss-related questions, Likert scale questions about how satisfied or dissatisfied they were with the quality of social support they received, and four open-ended questions specific social support following bereavement.

The survey was initially piloted with a small group to establish a level of validity and ensure that the questions were written in an appealing, engaging, and sensitive manner that provoked deep and rich responses. Cresswell and Miller’s [53] criteria for determining validity in qualitative inquiry were used to inform the strategy, focusing particularly on incorporating opportunities for member-checking, investigator triangulation, and thick, rich description.

Utilizing snowball sampling through social media and several bereavement support organizations, a total of 372 grieving adults over the age of 18 years participated in a cross-sectional, anonymous, online survey asking them to define various aspects of social support during their bereavement experiences. Over the course of one week during January of 2020, data were collected online using Qualtrics following approval by the principal investigator’s University Institutional Review Board.

Because of its methodological flexibility, content analysis (CA) was used to generate findings from the qualitative responses based on meaningful data that, through the process of recurrence and intentionality, creates cohesion and coherence [54]. The objective of CA is to present the ‘big picture’ that merges with the context, including the target population, their experience, and theory [55]. Qualitative responses were printed, read separately and in their entirety by two members of the research team, then unitized and contextualized [56] into a codebook. Through multiple, iterative-inductive rounds of data coding, open discussion, rated as effective as kappa values (as a means of managing and rationalizing any disagreements on which codes to apply) [57], intercoder consensus, and recoding, the findings were compared and validated in consultation with the literature [58].

All unitized data were also analyzed by the team to discern the degree of congruence with a priori codes in the existing theoretical literature on social support and categorized as: 1) emotional, 2) instrumental, 3) informational, and 4) appraisal. Of note is that emotional support as a construct is more difficult to quantify. For the purposes of these analyses, the working definition of emotional support used includes caring for another through the active expression of compassion and empathy, tenderness, validation of feelings, respect, listening, allowing emotions, and through the expression of loving kindness [59]. The research team paid careful attention to “any new questions or themes that emerge during the coding” [55]. For example, emotional support was distinguished from emotional acts of concern, which emerged as a separate theme in the analyses through investigator triangulation [60, 61], by identifying a tangible and direct action that held emotional meaning for participants.

Results

Descriptive

The survey sample (n = 372) was predominantly female (91.4%), white (91.1%), partnered or married (69%), with college or graduate degrees (58.1%). The majority of respondents had experienced the death of a child (75.1%), followed by a spouse/partner (11.7%), a parent (7.9%), and a sibling (5.2%). The time since the loss was more than five years (43.3%), followed by one to three years (25.8%), three to five years (17.5%), and within the past year (13.4%). The most frequent cause of death was illness or disease (25.8%) followed by accident (19.2%), perinatal/infant death (12.6%), suicide (9.3%), unknown/undeterminable (7.9%), homicide (6%), and overdose (4.9%), with 14.3% choosing “other” as the cause of death.

Social support

Respondents were asked to rate their perception of social support since their loved one’s death on a scale ranging from excellent to very poor; 35.7% reported their overall support as excellent or good, 26.5% as adequate, and 37.9% as poor or very poor. Participants were asked to assess their degree of satisfaction with the initial social support they received from providers during the acute loss-related crisis, when applicable. Not all participants interacted with every category of provider. Response options ranged from extremely satisfied to extremely dissatisfied, shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Satisfaction with crisis support in grief.

Extremely Satisfied Satisfied Adequate Dissatisfied Extremely Dissatisfied Mean N
1 2 3 4 5
Mortuary staff 47% (n = 141) 18% (n = 55) 22% (n = 67) 6% (n = 19) 7% (n = 20) 2.08 302
Hospice staff 48% (n = 25) 15% (n = 8) 13% (n = 7) 10% (n = 5) 13% (n = 7) 2.25 52
Nurses 38% (n = 85) 17% (n = 37) 14% (n = 32) 11% (n = 25) 20% (n = 44) 2.58 223
Faith leaders 30% (n = 71) 17% (n = 41) 19% (n = 45) 17% (n = 39) 17% (n = 40) 2.73 236
Crisis response team 32% (n = 25) 10% (n = 8) 17% (n = 13) 17% (n = 13) 24% (n = 19) 2.91 78
First responders 32% (n = 45) 9% (n = 12) 23% (n = 33) 16% (n = 22) 21% (n = 29) 2.84 141
Law enforcement 19% (n = 31) 18% (n = 30) 17% (n = 28) 14% (n = 23) 31% (n = 51) 3.20 163
Physicians 21% (n = 51) 14% (n = 34) 22% (n = 54) 14% (n = 34) 30% (n = 72) 3.17 245
Hospital social workers 22% (n = 32) 13% (n = 19) 17% (n = 25) 14% (n = 20) 34% (n = 49) 3.17 145

Mortuary staff received the largest percentage of ratings of effectiveness, with 65% reporting good or excellent satisfaction, followed by 63% for hospice staff, 55% for nurses, 47% for faith leaders, 42% and 41%, respectively, for crisis response and first responders, 37% for law enforcement, and 35% for physicians and hospital social workers.

Asked about satisfaction with social support generally, notably, of the 248 participants who had a relationship with their pets or other animals, 89% reported being extremely or mostly satisfied with the perception of support. Animals received the highest percentage of satisfaction among all categories of social support including support groups, counselors or therapists, friends, family, faith leaders, colleagues, and community members as shown in Table 2.

Table 2. Satisfaction with overall grief support.

Extremely Satisfied Satisfied Adequate Dissatisfied Extremely Dissatisfied Mean N
1 2 3 4 5
Pets/Animals 78% (n = 194) 11% (n = 27) 10% (n = 25) 1% (n = 2) 0% (n = 0) 1.55 248
Online grief groups 39% (n = 94) 28% (n = 69) 25% (n = 60) 5% (n = 13) 3% (n = 7) 2.67 243
Counselors/Therapists 38% (n = 101) 18% (n = 48) 19% (n = 49) 17% (n = 44) 8% (n = 21) 2.99 263
In person grief groups 34% (n = 63) 24% (n = 46) 19% (n = 36) 15% (n = 29) 7% (n = 14) 3.65 188
Friends 25% (n = 86) 27% (n = 92) 22% (n = 74) 16% (n = 54) 9% (n = 32) 3.31 338
Family 20% (n = 69) 20% (n = 69) 18% (n = 61) 25% (n = 84) 16% (n = 55) 3.76 338
Faith leaders 17% (n = 34) 22% (n = 44) 19% (n = 38) 21% (n = 41) 20% (n = 39) 3.86 196
Colleagues 12% (n = 30) 19% (n = 46) 32% (n = 78) 20% (n = 49) 16% (n = 38) 3.95 241
Neighbors/communities 14% (n = 35) 17% (n = 42) 26% (n = 65) 26% (n = 65) 16% (n = 40) 3.99 247

Extrapolating data specific to dissatisfaction with the degree of perceived social support, faith leaders, family, and communities/neighbors all either elicited extremely or mostly dissatisfied by 41–43% of the sample. This was followed by dissatisfaction with colleagues at 36%, counselors and friends each at about 25%, in-person support groups at 23%, and online support groups at 8.2%. The lowest rating of dissatisfaction was given to animals, at under 1%.

Qualitative findings

Survey results

Respondents were invited to answer a number of open-ended questions in their own words.

What does social support mean to you? This question was answered by 91% (n = 338) of the sample. Informational support was mentioned only once. Instrumental support, in the form of practical aid, was mentioned by 5% (n = 16) of participants 17 times. Seven percent (n = 22) of participants specified appraisal support one time. Importantly, acts of emotional caring emerged in these data as a category of specific emotion-focused actions that traverse emotional and instrumental support, such as sending a card or reaching out through a phone call or text message, and were referenced 121 times by 23% (n = 78) of respondents. And, finally, emotional support yielded the highest percentage and frequency in responses with 64% (n = 215) of the sample mentioning it 292 times:

“It means having a community of people who are safe to share your journey of grief with; who don’t try to fix you or hurry you…people who let me say her name and tell stories about her.”

“It means people checking on me, inviting me to places, listening and remembering.”

“Having a community of people around us that are willing to listen and be there for each other.”

What actions or responses felt supportive to you? This question elicited answers from 93% (n = 346) of the sample. Informational support was not mentioned, and appraisal support was mentioned by 6% (n = 20) of the respondents 20 times. This took the form of connecting with people who had experienced similar grief in online or in-person support groups or with people in their community. The support received from like others included advice, understanding, and connection. While advice was generally not desired from sources of social support, advice and connection with "like others" was experienced as helpful:

"Support groups have been most helpful, knowing there were others who truly understand."

"Advice from those who also lost a child has been helpful."

Instrumental support logged 53 mentions by 13% (n = 44) of the sample and included assistance with childcare, food delivery, financial support, yard work, and housecleaning:

“Helping take care of my child. Helping with household chores.”

“Written notes, gifts, money for the expenses…”

Emotional support was mentioned by 59% (n = 204) of respondents 316 times. Responses and actions related to “being present” and “holding space” were predominant and included descriptions like allowing the expression of grief, deep listening and “quiet understanding,” availability and commitment of time with the griever, acceptance of emotional states, refraining from a “need to fix” or giving unsolicited advice, being actively open to grief, and a commitment to long term support by “not fizzling out:”

“Consistent communication, acceptance of my feelings, allowing me space as needed, listening without trying to fix or belittle my grief.”

“Just being present. Not trying to fix anything. Listening. Letting me talk about Thomas. Remembering Thomas. Honoring him.”

“Telling me that my grief is valid, that my feelings are real. Basically just allowing me to be.”

Sixty two percent of respondents (n = 214) mentioned acts of emotional caring 428 times:

“Being available week after week to walk and chat, gifts from neighbors of homemade cakes.”

“People checking in with me. People willing to simply listen and hold space for me and my pain. People who sent cards months and months even after. Bringing food. Buying gift cards for our family.”

“Checking in on me. Coming over to see how I am. Bringing dinner.”

The most frequently mentioned forms of emotional support included remembering the person who died, speaking their name, sharing memories of them, or acknowledging important dates related to them:

“ANYONE doing ANYTHING that lets me know they are thinking of him.”

“Just letting me mention his name without awkward silence or changing the subject.”

What kinds of actions or responses felt unsupportive to you? Eighty nine percent (n = 330) of respondents answered this question. No participant mentioned actions related to appraisal, informational, or instrumental support. Only two categories, emotional support (34%, n = 112) and emotional acts of caring (63%, n = 208), were reported in these data for unsupportive responses or actions, in other words as examples of the failure to provide meaningful support in these areas. Specifically, respondents talked about the use of platitudes; judging or rushing grief; failure to approach or acknowledge loss; feeling abandoned by family, friends, and community members; avoidance of grief and griever; not listening; the perception that others were pretending the person who died had never existed; others’ propensity to center their own needs and feelings above the primary griever; and offering unsolicited advice, especially about how to heal grief:

“Judging my grief, telling me they are tired of hearing it, facial expressions indicating irritation, annoyance… failing to acknowledge death days, birthdays, him at other holidays, indicating by words, silence, or actions that they are tired of my grief and tired of hearing about him or grief.”

“Never mentioning my daughter as if she never existed… friends stopped talking to us.”

“No one checked in on me, feeling alone and isolated.”

“Walking away from us when they see us. Changing the subject. Insisting I need to get out of the house because they miss the old me. Wanting me to participate in social events or visiting in the same way I did before Gabe was murdered. Not understanding I am forever changed.”

“I was surprised that the pastor who oversaw my son’s funeral never checked on us.”

How might others in your social support system do better to help you? This was answered by 86% (n = 319) of the sample. Informational support was not mentioned. Appraisal support was mentioned by three participants one time (1%), mostly around more access to support groups, with one sibling noting “there is no group” available for this type of loss. Instrumental support in the form of providing food or help with chores was mentioned by 3% (n = 10) of the respondents 23 times:

“Do more practical help. Things like mow the lawn, watch the kids, do the dishes, bring meals.”

One-half of respondents indicated the desire for more emotional support 256 times (49%, n = 156). Participants talked about how important it is for others to listen, to learn about grief, to show up and not avoid them, and to accept their emotional state, to “let me fall apart.” Overwhelmingly they wanted others to be present and to remember their loved one who died without the need to ‘fix’ their grief and absent a timeline or agenda:

“If they would just allow me to express and honor what I feel.”

“Talk to me about him. SAY HIS NAME. He existed.”

“Just keep showing up for me and listen. Don’t try to fix it.”

“… not trying to fix what can’t be fixed. By accepting the new sad, grieving me as someone who is ok to be with and not expecting me to feel like I need to get over this or move on and become the happy person I was. Not making me feel like I have to wear a mask and pretend I am someone I am not.”

“Remembering Michael with me and saying his name—telling me memories of him. I won’t break if you talk about him, I want to honor and remember him forever.”

Acts of emotional caring were referenced by 48% of the sample (n = 154) 290 times, most often through active remembering (32%, n = 102) and reaching out to the grieving person (29%, n = 93):

“Reach out to me more often. Ask me more about my grief experience.”

“If they wait for me to reach out they are only pushing me further away.”

Five percent (n = 15) spontaneously wrote at least one time about ways animals support them emotionally:

“My dog was the person who was with me all the time. I think just having that soul there who can’t say anything so it’s like you know they’re not saying the right or wrong thing, they’re just there."

"And sometimes when we walk, I talk to him about my son and he has no choice but to listen because he’s a dog… a loving comforting animal."

“They brought my dog to the hospital to see me. I think that’s the thing that probably was the divider between going into depression and not.”

Discussion

Findings highlight that many grievers experience dissatisfaction with social support from a variety of actors despite the increased need for support in bereavement and the high risk for poor emotional, mental, and physical outcomes. Just over one-third of the sample rated the level of social support they received post-loss as excellent or good, with 38% rating it as poor or very poor. Specific actors who provided good or excellent social support in more than 50% of the sample included animals, support groups, counselors or therapists, and friends, while the percentage was below 50% for family, faith leaders, colleagues, and community members.

These data also illuminate the primacy of emotional support and acts of emotional concern as shown in Fig 1. Specifically, emotional support in grief can be defined as being present and holding space for the griever, more specifically distilled to listening and allowing grief without judgment, platitudes, or an agenda. This means being available for the griever, spending time with the griever, centering the griever’s needs, and not imposing a time limit on grief. Acts of emotional concern can be expressed by remembering (asking about, sharing memories of, and speaking the name of) the person who died, acknowledging important dates, and engaging in emotionally sensitive communication. It is not simply the amount of contact with family and friends that equates to good social support, but also the quality of the connection, with the presence of emotional support and expression of caring being highly valued.

Fig 1. Supportive actions in grief.

Fig 1

One participant distilled it down to the feeling of being loved:

"I feel supported by my dogs. Simply because the love is unconditional."

These findings echo previous reports underscoring the importance of emotional support for grieving individuals [59, 6264]. Concrete examples of valued emotional support and acts of concern in the current study included reaching out to contact the bereaved person, actively allowing the expression of grief and acceptance of emotional states, sensitive and compassionate communication, and actively remembering the person who died. Also noted was the importance of support not just in the immediate aftermath of the loss but over a longer period of time.

Less salient in these data was the interpretation of instrumental, appraisal, and informational support as good social support. Instrumental support was effective when expressed through helping with meals, childcare, housekeeping, and written notes and gifts. One important aspect of instrumental support deserving of attention may be the classic mistake of saying, “…call if you need anything, without any follow-up. Participants appreciated others actively reaching out to them to offer practical aid. Appraisal support meant connecting with like others through grief support groups, in person and online, and on social media. Time spent with others, both online and in person, who share a common tragedy of loss was reported as supportive in these data. Informational support was largely absent, echoing previous findings suggesting informational support is not among the most helpful forms of social support in bereavement [63]. And, while some respondents noted that they appreciated informational support and guidance through their support groups, others perceived unsolicited information and advice to be unhelpful. Also, the salutary effect of informational support may be more specific to certain subgroups of the bereaved. For instance, in one study with widowed fathers of dependent children, participants expressed a desire for more information and guidance than was offered [65].

Perhaps most interesting is the finding that the overwhelming majority of respondents rated animals as highly satisfactory. Animals are not usually included in conceptualizations of possible sources of social support in bereavement. However, research in other fields has provided evidence of the beneficial impact of interactions with animals on human health and well-being [66, 67]. The recognition of animals as sources of meaningful social support in bereavement may open up avenues for more effective support to grievers, especially in socially isolated populations and under certain conditions such as quarantine or physical distancing.

Also instructive are findings about the lowest rated groups, amongst which hospital social workers, followed by physicians and law enforcement, ranked highest in dissatisfaction during acute care, and faith leaders ranked third to lowest in overall satisfaction with grief support. Specifically, these provider groups often encounter others in crisis and, with the exception of law enforcement, are in the caring professions. Emotional support is essential for vulnerable populations [62], which are often served by social workers, hospital staff, and faith leaders. That there is such low satisfaction with the provision of support at a time of such great need is worthy of further study. There may be several reasons for this. First, the “prevailing views of emotions as the antithesis of reason” in pedagogical models [62] requires a serious reappraisal [68, 69]. Scholars have noted the lack of attention to skillfully working with emotions and developing emotional competence in medical education and social work education classrooms [69, 70]. For example, content on emotions in social work pedagogy tends to focus on the compartmentalization of emotions rather than learning to express, process, and integrate emotions [71]. Even in field education, supervisors may refer to students’ emotional responses as “signs of immaturity” stemming from “unresolved personal problems” [72], absent evidence of such assertions. In particular, social work historically serves the most vulnerable populations [68], many of whom are experiencing trauma and grief. They also serve many frequent users of healthcare systems who are at highest risk for poor social determinants of health [68, 73]. Additionally, hospital social work roles have largely shifted from supportive counseling to discharge planning [74], particularly in bereavement, and are relegated to the margins, perhaps because they are comparatively low-cost service providers in the medical setting.

The essentiality of emotion-focused support in bereavement also warrants further attention. While emotional support is perhaps most valued in bereavement and can decrease psychological distress [3], social networks are not always capable of providing adequate emotional support. In addition, the current findings echo previous findings (e.g., [65]) reporting a mismatch between the kinds of social support offered and what grievers desire. These data highlight the importance of educating those in bereaved individuals’ social networks to be more responsive to the emotional needs of grievers and for pedagogical and practice models to promote and teach emotionally supportive behaviors.

While this study adds to the understanding of social support in bereavement, it has a number of limitations. Self-reports are subject to recall bias and other shortfalls occurring during this type of cross-sectional data acquisition [75]. The sample was demographically homogenous, predominantly female, and most respondents experienced the death of a child, or other traumatic death such as the suicide of a parent, and findings may not be representative of relatively uncomplicated grief in the general population. These data, thus, seem to support Stroebe and colleagues [27] finding that social support may be more essential when grief is traumatic and complicated. There may also be differences in the necessary social support structures and processes based on gender, something that should be more specifically investigated in future studies, relationship to the deceased, manner of death, or other factors, or differences in actors and actions depending on the same variables. For example, men and women may desire and elicit different types of social support while the death of a parent to suicide may elicit more social avoidance than the death of a parent to cancer, leaving the bereaved with more unmet social support needs. Additionally, future research on effective bereavement social support could specifically focus on age groups to discern any differences or similarities across the lifespan. Since many of the studies on social support in bereavement focus on uncomplicated bereavement, future studies could explore emotional and physical health outcomes as they relate to satisfaction with traumatic grief support longitudinally. Despite the aforementioned limitations, this study adds to the body of scientific data about social support in bereavement, offering insights into participants’ lived experiences.

Conclusion

The burden of loneliness in grief, especially when traumatic, is costly and presents a potential public health crisis, particularly in the aftermath of the COVID-19 pandemic [1, 17, 18, 21]. Findings from this study can be used to help educate others about the needs of grievers and how to adequately support them to reduce some of the cost to families and society. Improvements in social support that focus on emotions, in turn, might decrease the common loneliness experienced by grievers and thus the associated health risks, both during and beyond the scope of the COVID-19 pandemic. However, pedagogical models, often “woefully deficient in educating its learners about the role of emotions in healthcare,” emphasize, instead, emotional distancing and detachment [69]. Not only is this the case in medicine but also in social work practice [62, 69]. Faculty could restructure educational objectives to emphasize the necessity of tending to the emotions of patients and clients in addition to the importance of personal growth toward emotional intelligence. Future research could more precisely explore the effects of emotional support, specifically, in mitigating poor psychological and health outcomes in the bereaved. Further, community-based education programs might focus on emotional health, in addition to mental health, with a turn toward coping with painful emotions and helping support others, in faith and school systems, in communities, and at work.

A particularly interesting finding is the high level of satisfaction reported with animals as sources of social support. Animals may be an especially important source of emotional support during conditions involving social isolation, such as the COVID-19 pandemic when contact with other people is limited, or during experiential conditions such as the loneliness so common in bereavement. Further research could investigate the ways in which animals are perceived as beneficial in grief more thoroughly, but the adoption of pets could be one avenue by which to promote well-being and reduce loneliness during the pandemic, especially for those who are not able to access strong social support networks.

When it comes to good grief support, perhaps we may have much to learn from our fellow non-human animals.

Supporting information

S1 Questionnaire

(DOCX)

S1 Data

(XLSX)

Data Availability

Anonymized data are included.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Eisma M. C., Boelen P. A., & Lenferink L. (2020). Prolonged grief disorder following the Coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic. Psychiatry Research, 288, 113031. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Wallace C. L., Wladkowski S. P., Gibson A., & White P. (2020). Grief during the COVID-19 pandemic: Considerations for palliative care providers. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 60(1), E70–76. 10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2020.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Juth V., Smyth J., Carey M., & Lepore S. (2015). Social constraints are associated with negative psychological and physical adjustment in bereavement. Applied Psychology-Health and Well Being, 7(2), 129–148. 10.1111/aphw.12041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Stroebe M., Schut H., & Stroebe W. (2007). Health outcomes of bereavement. Lancet, 370(9603),1960–73. 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61816-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.House J. S., Landis K. R., & Umberson D. (1988). Social relationships and health. Science, 241(4865), 540–545. 10.1126/science.3399889 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Wang J., Mann F., Lloyd-Evans B., Ma R., & Johnson S. (2018). Associations between loneliness and perceived social support and outcomes of mental health problems: A systematic review. BMC Psychiatry, 18, 156. 10.1186/s12888-018-1736-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.West D. A., Kellner R., & Moore-West M. (1986). The effects of loneliness: A review of the literature. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 27(4), 351–363. 10.1016/0010-440x(86)90011-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Prohaska T., Burholt V., Burns A., et al. (2020). Consensus statement: Loneliness in older adults, the 21st century social determinant of health? BMJ Open, 10(8). 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-034967 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Cacioppo J. T., Cacioppo S., Capitanio J. P, & Cole S. W. (2015). The neuroendocrinology of social isolation. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 733–767. 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015240 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Cacioppo J., & Cacioppo S. (2018). The growing problem of loneliness. The Lancet, 391(10119), 426–426. 10.1016/S0140-6736(18)30142-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Wang H. H., Wu S. Z., & Liu Y. Y. (2003). Association between social support and health outcomes: A meta-analysis. Kaoshsiung Journal of Medical Sciences, 19(7), 345–350. 10.1016/S1607-551X(09)70436-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Umberson D., & Karas Montez J. (2010). Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 51(1_suppl), S54–S66. 10.1177/0022146510383501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Yang Y., Schorpp K., & Harris K. (2014). Social support, social strain and inflammation: Evidence from a national longitudinal study of U.S. adults. Social Science & Medicine, 107, 124–135. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2014.02.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Uchino B., Cacioppo J., & Kiecolt-Glaser J. (1996). The relationship between social support and physiological processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mechanisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119(3), 488–531. 10.1037/0033-2909.119.3.488 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Van der Houwen K., Stroebe M., Schut H., Stroebe W., & Van den Bout J. (2010). Mediating processes in bereavement: The role of rumination, threatening grief interpretations, and deliberate grief avoidance. Social Science & Medicine, 71(9), 1669–1676. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Fried E. I., Bockting C., Arjadi R., Borsboom D., Amshoff M., Cramer A. O., et al. (2015). From loss to loneliness: The relationship between bereavement and depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(2), 256. 10.1037/abn0000028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Killgore W. D. S., Cloonan S. A., Taylor E. C., Lucas D. A., & Dailey N. S. (2020). Loneliness during the first half-year of COVID-19 lockdowns. Psychiatry Research, 294, 113551. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Lee C. M., Cadigan J. M., & Rhew I. C. (2020). Increases in loneliness among young adults during the COVID-19 pandemic and association with increases in mental health problems. Journal of Adolescent Health, 67, 714–717. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2020.08.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Li L. Z., & Wang S. (2020). Prevalence and predictors of general psychiatric disorders and loneliness during COVID-19 in the United Kingdom. Psychiatry Research, 291, 113267. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113267 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brooks S. K., Webster R. K., Smith L. E., Woodland L., Wessely S., Greenberg N., et al. (2020). The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: Rapid review of the evidence. Lancet, 395, 912–920. 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Palgi Y., Shrira A., Ring L., Bodner E., Avidor S., Bergman Y., et al. (2020). The loneliness pandemic: Loneliness and other concomitants of depression, anxiety and their comorbidity during the COVID-19 outbreak. Journal of Affective Disorders, 275, 109–111. 10.1016/j.jad.2020.06.036 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Sasangohar F., Jones S. L., Masud F. N., Vahidy F. S., & Kash B. A. (2020). Provider burnout and fatigue during the COVID-19 pandemic: Lessons learned from a high-volume intensive care unit. Anesthesia and Analgesia, 131(1), 106–111. 10.1213/ANE.0000000000004866 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Rogalla K. (2020). Anticipatory grief, proactive coping, social support, and growth: Exploring positive experiences of preparing for loss. Omega—Journal of Death and Dying 81(1), 107–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Burke L. A., & Neimeyer R. A. (2013). Prospective risk factors for complicated grief: A review of the empirical literature. In Stroebe M. S., Schut H., & van den Bout J. (Eds.), Complicated Grief: Scientific foundations for healthcare professionals (pp. 145–161). London, UK: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Vanderwerker L., & Prigerson H. (2004). Social support and technological connectedness as protective factors in bereavement. Journal of Loss and Trauma, 9(1), 45–57. 10.1080/15325020490255304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Tso I. F., & Park S. (2020). Alarming levels of psychiatric symptoms and the role of loneliness during the COVID-19 epidemic: A case study of Hong Kong. Psychiatry Research, 293, 113423. 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Stroebe W., Zech E., Stroebe M. S., & Abakoumkin G. (2005). Does social support help in bereavement? Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24(7), 1030–1050. 10.1521/jscp.2005.24.7.1030 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Scott H. R., Pitman A., Kozhuharova P. et al. (2020). A systematic review of studies describing the influence of informal social support on psychological wellbeing in people bereaved by sudden or violent causes of death. BMC Psychiatry 20, 265. 10.1186/s12888-020-02639-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.House J. S., Umberson D., & Landis K. R. (1988). Structures and processes of social support. Annual Review of Sociology, 14, 293 318. 10.1146/annurev.so.14.080188.001453 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.House J. S. (1981). Work Stress and Social Support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley Pub Co. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cohen S., & Syme S. L. (1985). Issues in the study and application of social support. In Cohen S.& Syme S. L.(Eds.) Social Support and Health. Academic Press. 10.1177/0022167818773467 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Laakso H., & Paunonen‐Ilmonen M. (2002). Mothers’ experience of social support following the death of a child. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 11(2), 176–185. 10.1046/j.1365-2702.2002.00611.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.House J. S., Kahn R. L., McLeod J. D., & Williams D. (1985). Measures and concepts of social support. In Cohen S.& Syme S. L.(Eds.), Social Support and Health (p. 83–108). Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McIntosh N. (1991). Identification and investigation of properties of social support. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12(3), 201–217. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Pitman A. L., King M. B., Marston L. & Osborn D. P. J. (2020). The association of loneliness after sudden bereavement with risk of suicide attempt: A nationwide survey of bereaved adults. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology 55, 1081–1092. 10.1007/s00127-020-01921-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Wågø S., Byrkjedal I., Sinnes H., Hystad S., & Dyregrov K. (2017). Social support and complicated grief: A longitudinal study on bereaved parents after the Utøya terror attack in Norway. Scandinavian Psychologist, 4. 10.15714/scandpsychol.4.e10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Harrop E., Scott H., Sivell S., Seddon K., Fitzgibbon J., Morgan F., et al. (2020). Coping and wellbeing in bereavement: Two core outcomes for evaluating bereavement support in palliative care. BMC Palliative Care, 19(1), 29. 10.1186/s12904-020-0532-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Gijzen S., L’Hoir M. P., Boere-Boonekamp M. M., & Need A. (2016). How do parents experience support after the death of their child? BMC Pediatrics, 16(1), 204. 10.1186/s12887-016-0749-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hartig J., & Viola J. (2016). Online grief support communities: Therapeutic benefits of membership. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying, 73(1), 29–41. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Aoun S., Breen L., Howting D., Rumbold B., McNamara B., & Hegney D. (2015). Who needs bereavement support? A population based survey of bereavement risk and support need. PloS One, 10(3), e0121101–e0121101. 10.1371/journal.pone.0121101 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Breen L., & O’Connor M. (2011). Family and social networks after bereavement: Experiences of support, change and isolation. Journal of Family Therapy 33, 98–120. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Dyregrov K., Dyregrov A., & Johnsen I. (2014). Positive and negative experiences from grief group participation: A qualitative study. OMEGA—Journal of Death and Dying, 68(1), 45–62. 10.2190/OM.68.1.c [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Simon N. (2015). Increasing support for the treatment of complicated grief in adults of all ages. JAMA, 313(21), 2172–2173. 10.1001/jama.2015.105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Breen L., & O’Connor M. (2009). Acts of resistance: Breaking the silence of grief following traffic crash fatalities. Death Studies, 34(1), 30–53. 10.1080/07481180903372384 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Soklaridis S., Ferguson G., Bonato S., Saikaly R., & Mosher P. (2018). Being there: Protocol for a scoping review of the medical education literature on grief support training for medical professionals. BMJ Open, 8(11), e022778. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-022778 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Sikstrom L., Saikaly R., Ferguson G., Mosher P. J., Bonato S., & Soklaridis S. (2019). Being there: A scoping review of grief support training in medical education. PLoS ONE, 14(11), e0224325. 10.1371/journal.pone.0224325 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Jacobson N., Lord K., & Newman M. (2017). Perceived emotional social support in bereaved spouses mediates the relationship between anxiety and depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 211, 83–91. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.01.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Kim H., Sefcik J. S., & Bradway C. (2017). Characteristics of qualitative descriptive studies: A systematic review. Research in Nursing & Health, 40(1), 23–42. 10.1002/nur.21768 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Sandelowski Margarete. (2000). Whatever happened to qualitative description? Research in Nursing & Health 23(4), 334–340. Web. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hart P., & Mareno N. (2014). Cultural challenges and barriers through the voices of nurses. Journal of Clinical Nursing, 23(15), 2223–2232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Tracy Sarah J. (2010) Qualitative quality: Eight ‘big-tent’ criteria for excellent qualitative research.” Qualitative Inquiry 16(10): 837–851. Web. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.LaDonna K., Taylor T., & Lingard L. (2018). Why open-ended survey questions are unlikely to support rigorous qualitative insights. Academic Medicine, 93(3), 347–349. 10.1097/ACM.0000000000002088 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Creswell J. W., & Miller D. (2000) Determining validity in qualitative inquiry. Theory into Practice, 39(3), 124–130, 10.1207/s15430421tip3903_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Boeije H. (2002). A purposeful approach to the constant comparison method in the analysis of qualitative interviews. Quality & Quantity, 36, 391–409. [Google Scholar]
  • 55.White M. D., & Marsh E. Content analysis: a flexible methodology. Library Trends, 55(1), 22–45. 10.1353/lib.2006.0053 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Krippendorff K. (2004). Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Chinh B., Zade H., Ganji A., & Aragon C. (2019). Ways of qualitative coding: A case study of four strategies for resolving disagreements. In Extended Abstracts of the 2019 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI EA ’19). Association for Computing Machinery, Paper LBW0241, 1–6 10.1145/3290607.3312879 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cascio M. A., Lee E., Vaudrin N., & Freedman D. A. (2019). A team-based approach to open coding: Considerations for creating intercoder consensus. Field Methods, 31(2), 116–130. 10.1177/1525822X19838237 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Burleson B. R. (2003). The experience and effects of emotional support: What the study of cultural and gender differences can tell us about close relationships, emotion, and interpersonal communication. Personal Relationships, 10(1), 1–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Denzin N. K. (1978). Sociological Methods. McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Tonkin-Crine S., Anthierens S., Hood K., Yardley L., Cals J., Francis N., et al. (2016). Discrepancies between qualitative and quantitative evaluation of randomised controlled trial results: Achieving clarity through mixed methods triangulation. Implementation Science,11(1), 66. 10.1186/s13012-016-0436-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Butler G. (2019). Reflecting on emotions in social work. In Mantell A.& Scragg T.(Eds). Reflective Practice in Social Work, pp. 39–62. Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lehman D. R., Ellard J. H., & Wortman C. B. (1986). Social support for the bereaved: Recipients’ and providers’ perspectives on what is helpful. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54(4), 438–446. 10.1037/0022-006X.54.4.438 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Underman K., & Hirshfield L. E. (2016). Detached concern?: Emotional socialization in twenty-first century medical education. Social Science & Medicine, 160, 94–101, 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.05.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Holmgren H. (2019). Life came to a full stop: The experiences of widowed fathers. OMEGA-Journal of Death and Dying. Advance online publication. 10.1177/0030222819880713 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Nimer J., & Lundahl B. (2007). Animal-assisted therapy: A meta-analysis. Anthrozoos, 20(3), 225–238. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Waite T. C., Hamilton L., & O’Brien W. (2018). A meta-analysis of Animal Assisted Interventions targeting pain, anxiety and distress in medical settings. Complementary Therapies in Clinical Practice, 33, 49–55. 10.1016/j.ctcp.2018.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Hoy J. M. (2017). Rescuing emotion from the margins of social work education. Retrieved from Sophia, the St. Catherine University repository; website: https://sophia.stkate.edu/dsw/19 [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Shapiro J. (2013). The feeling physician: Educating the emotions in medical training. European Journal for Person Centered Healthcare, 1(2), 310–316. 10.5750/ejpch.v1i2.664 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ikebuchi J., & Rasmussen B. (2014). The use of emotion in social work education. Journal of Teaching in Social Work, 34 (3), 285–301. 10.1080/08841233.2014.909378 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Gerdes K. E., & Segal E. (2011). Importance of empathy for social work practice: Integrating new science. Social Work, 56(2), 141–148. 10.1093/sw/56.2.141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Barlow C., & Hall B., (2007). What about feelings?: A study of emotion and tension in social work field education. Social Work Education, 26(4), 319–412. 10.1080/02615470601081712 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Lechman C., & Duder S. (2009). Hospital length of stay: Social work services as an important factor. Social Work in Health Care, 48, 495–504. 10.1080/00981380802619360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Heenan D., & Birrell B. (2019). Hospital-based social work: Challenges at the interface between health and social care, The British Journal of Social Work, 49(7), 1741–1758. 10.1093/bjsw/bcy114 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Rust J. & Golombok S. (1999). Modern Psychometrics: The science of psychological assessment, 2nd edition. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Manuel Fernández-Alcántara

18 Mar 2021

PONE-D-21-04230

Good Grief Support:

Exploring the Actors and Actions in Social Support

after Traumatic Grief

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Cacciatore,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Reviewers have found that the manuscript is promising. It is important that you include more and clear information on the method section of the manuscript. Please, follow Reviewer 1 suggestions regarding the missing information about the research design and the data analysis. Before submitting a revised version of the manuscript please adhere to the COREQ guidelines for reporting qualitative research (https://www.equator-network.org/reporting-guidelines/coreq/) that may help you to describe the methodology of the study.

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 02 2021 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Manuel Fernández-Alcántara, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2.  Please ensure that you include a title page within your main document. We do appreciate that you have a title page document uploaded as a separate file, however, as per our author guidelines (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-title-page) we do require this to be part of the manuscript file itself and not uploaded separately.

Could you therefore please include the title page into the beginning of your manuscript file itself, listing all authors and affiliations.

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The article “Good Grief Support: Exploring the Actors and Actions in Social Support after Traumatic Grief”, assuming that social support enhances wellbeing and health especially in traumatic grief, consider how mourners interpret and define social support. In the scenario of the current pandemic, the study assessed mourners satisfaction of social support in traumatic grief, using four categories of social support as a framework. Findings suggest inadequate satisfaction from professional, familial, and community support. Pets emerged with the most satisfactory ratings. Further, findings suggest that emotional support is the most desired type of support following traumatic loss.

The introduction is too long and perhaps too detailed with respect to the concept of social support. This part can be summarized. Less clear, however, is the description of the effects of Self Help Groups, one of the most important forms of social support offered to traumatic grief (lines 113-120). I highlight this work that may be helpful: “Testoni, I., Francescon, E., De Leo, D., Santini, A., & Zamperini, A. (2019). Forgiveness and Blame Among Sui-cide Survivors: A Qualitative Analysis on Reports of 4-Years Self-Help-Group Meetings. Community Mental Health Journal, 55(2), 360-368. doi:10.1007/s10597-018-0291-3”

The methodology part is very messy. It is best to follow this structure: a) clearly define the objectives; b) clearly define the participants and describe all demographic characteristics of the participants; c) clearly describe the research design and justify the methodologies; d) clearly describe the steps of the analyses. The qualitative approach used is unclear. Direct quotes in the methodological section make the authors seem inexperienced in the field.

The relationship between the research and the pandemic is not at all clear: when was the survey done? were clear references to the pandemic made? was it clear to participants that the survey was about or could be influenced by the pandemic experience?

The topic is very interesting but the presentation of results at the moment is still very haphazard.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for asking me to review this interesting paper. Here are my impressions that I hope are helpful and constructive.

In the abstract, the authors summarize the research objective and the main findings. However, a mention of the methodology used is missed.

The introduction section identifies comprehensively the topic and explains how the study relates to this previously published research. The need for this study is justified.

The tables provided as supporting information are clear and legible and support the findings, however they lack titles.

Regarding the methodology, it provides information on how the research was carried out. One minor comment: Line 160 mentions triangulation as a validity strategy. Detailing what type of triangulation would be clarifying.

In the results and discussion, two elements caught my attention: The disparity of etiologies of death and a markedly female sample. Although both are mentioned in the study limitations, and especially the cause of death is justified with evidence, gender is only mentioned in line 418. It would be interesting to address this aspect, and even show if the perception of social support differs whether the death is unexpected or traumatic or it is not.

Finally, the text lacks a section on conclusions.

Reviewer #3: Thank you for the opportunity to review manuscript ID# PONE-D-21-04230, “Good Grief Support: Exploring the Actors and Actions in Social Support after Traumatic Grief.” This paper presents the results of a mixed method study assessing bereaved individuals’ satisfaction with social support in the aftermath of a traumatic loss. Strengths of this paper include the large sample size and discussion of specific behavioral examples of social support identified via qualitative methods. I commend the authors for this important work, which has implications for providers working across a variety of different disciplines, and have only a few suggestions for the authors’ consideration.

1) In the beginning of the Method section, the authors note that the data presented here was part of a larger effort to define perceptions of specific actions and actors of effective post-bereavement social support. To clarify, was this data collected as part of a larger study, and if so, is this sample a subset of the sample from the larger study? Additional clarification would be helpful.

2) When describing the study methodology, please discuss any explicit inclusion/exclusion criteria.

3) When presenting descriptive statistics, I noticed no information on age was provided. Did the authors collect any information on age, and, if not, is there any reason to think that this might be an important area of future inquiry worth mentioning in the Discussion section (e.g., ensuring that the kinds of social support mentioned are similarly relevant across the lifespan)?

4) On page 10, lines 196-197, the authors mention that participants were asked to assess their degree of satisfaction with the initial social support they received from various individuals during the acute loss-related period. I recommend mentioning this in the Method section when discussing survey content, along with a description of the various relationship groups/categories included in this survey. Additionally, how was the acute loss-related period defined on the survey?

5) On page 19 of the Discussion section, the authors suggest several interesting and important reasons why provider groups such as hospital social workers, physicians, law enforcement, and faith leaders are among the lowest rated groups in terms of satisfaction with support. In addition to issues with training models and curricula, could occupational burnout also be relevant here given the intense demands place on these folks with often minimal organizational support? I might mention the literature on burnout here as well just for context.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Reviewer #3: Yes: Joah Williams

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2021 May 27;16(5):e0252324. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0252324.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


13 Apr 2021

Thank you so much for improving our manuscript. We are included a list of changes as recommended by reviewers and responses. We have uploaded an anonymized database. We have included a track changes version and clean version of the manuscript. We have followed the suggestions for methods of R1 and relevant COREQ guidelines. We hope these changes are satisfactory. Thank you!

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers- PONE-D-21-04230R1.docx

Decision Letter 1

Manuel Fernández-Alcántara

14 May 2021

What is Good Grief Support?

Exploring the actors and actions in social support after traumatic grief

PONE-D-21-04230R1

Dear Dr. Cacciatore,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Manuel Fernández-Alcántara, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The articile is quite interesting and wel written.

After the revision it can be published.

Congratulation on this result.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Manuel Fernández-Alcántara

19 May 2021

PONE-D-21-04230R1

What is Good Grief Support? Exploring the actors and actions in social support after traumatic grief

Dear Dr. Cacciatore:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Manuel Fernández-Alcántara

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

RESOURCES