Abstract
Identification of neurons undergoing plasticity in response to external stimuli is one of the pertinent problems in neuroscience. Immediate early genes (IEGs) are widely used as a marker for neuronal plasticity. Here, we model the dynamics of IEG expression as a consecutive, irreversible first-order reaction with a limiting substrate. First, we develop an analytical framework to show that such a model, together with two-photon in vivo imaging of IEG expression, can be used to identify distinct neuronal subsets representing multiple memories. Using the above combination, we show that the expression kinetics, rather than intensity threshold, can be used to identify neuronal ensembles responding to the presentation of two events in vivo. The analytical expression allowed us to segregate the neurons based on their temporal response to one specific behavioural event, thereby improving the ability to detect plasticity related neurons. We image the retrosplenial cortex (RSc) of cfos-GFP transgenic mice to follow the dynamics of cellular changes resulting from contextual fear conditioning behaviour, enabling us to establish a representation of context in RSc at the cellular scale following memory acquisition. Thus, we obtain a general method that distinguishes neurons that took part in multiple temporally separated events by measuring fluorescence of individual neurons in live mice.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s13041-021-00798-3.
Keywords: cfos expression, Neuronal ensembles, Dual context exposure, Retrosplenial cortex, IEGs, Two-photon, In vivo imaging
Introduction
Identifying the neuronal ensemble that possibly encodes memory and understanding the changes occurring in the memory circuits over time are pertinent problems in neuroscience research [8, 19, 20, 31]. Immediate early genes are rapidly and transiently expressed in response to neuronal activity and hence used as a marker for plasticity. Generally, to identify the temporal coupling of IEG expression to different behaviours or events, it requires as many distinct molecular labels as the number of events that are being followed.
Cellular compartment analysis of temporal activity by fluorescent in situ hybridization (catFISH) [16–18] initially exploited the unique transport kinetics of Arc mRNA from the nucleus to the cytoplasm, and later has been improved to use the intronic regions of Arc that have faster kinetics along with other IEGs such as Homer [39] to dissect the temporal engagement of neurons. Despite such improvements the method is limited to in vitro identification. Another technique uses a combination of IEG promoter-based expression and a modified Tet-OFF system to achieve the labelling of two distinct populations of neurons [29]. In both cases, the visualisation of the signal is done post hoc in vitro, limiting the investigation of the neuronal population to a snapshot at any given time. Such methods cannot follow an ensemble of neurons and observe their evolution longitudinally.
Neuronal activity indicators such as genetically encoded calcium or voltage indicators (GECIs or GEVIs) are reporters of neuronal firing in sub-millisecond time scales [21, 23]. However, they do not necessarily report the plastic events that occur in response to these firings. Further, they require high-speed imaging with temporal resolution matching the indicators' response time, requiring the imaging be done on awake behaving mice. The slow kinetics of IEG expression enables following cellular plastic events in anesthetised mice after behavioural training [2, 9, 10, 37].
Here, we propose to follow the kinetics of IEG expression in vivo to identify distinct neuronal subsets, each corresponding to distinct events. First, we analytically describe the IEG protein expression dynamics as a function of time. In case of a fluorophore expressed under an IEG promoter, the kinetics can be followed by measuring the fluorescence as a function of time. Among the various IEGs, cfos expression is a widely used marker for cellular activity [13]. We validate our model using data obtained from cfos-eGFP [6] and cfos-shGFP [29] transgenic mice under two conditions: bicuculline-induced seizure and behavioural induction of IEG following contextual exposure. Following this, the kinetics of IEG-fluorescence expression is used to determine when the neuron was activated, enabling us to distinguish between neuronal populations that took part in different events that are separated in time. Thus, we hypothesize that the memory engram of multiple events can be identified using the expression dynamics of an immediate early gene and experimentally show that we can do so for two events.
Recent and emerging evidences suggest RSc plays a vital role in encoding context-related information [3–5, 24, 25]. Clustered addition of spines is observed in RSc when contextual training is carried out across multiple sessions [14]. Similarly, the inactivation of RSc prevents contextual retrieval in mice and water maze post-training [12, 28]. Preferential activation of RSc during spatial navigation has also been reported [11]. Interestingly, it is also shown that in schema-dependent encoding of related events, RSc is engaged only during the encoding of new learning related to prior information but not during encoding of completely novel information [35]. More importantly, RSc lesion in rats abolishes their ability to resolve context-based conflicts [27]. Thus, all these studies suggest RSc plays an active role during the context-based behaviour, although the nature of this role is unclear. Given its function, we reasoned that RSc might maintain contextual information in the form of independent representations and its interrelations. If such contextual interrelations have cellular representations in RSc, we would be able to locate them using our method. This is possible as the method described here, can identify, and longitudinally follow the activated cellular ensembles in vivo. Thus, in this study we simultaneously probe representation for a context, how it changes across time, and when a new context is introduced close in time.
Results
Consecutive first-order kinetics describes IEG protein expression following a behavioural event
We investigate whether the derived analytical expression (Fig. 1, see “Appendix: Theory”) describes IEG protein expression in vivo in cfos-eGFP transgenic mice. In these mice, eGFP is expressed under the cfos promoter where the level of fluorescence indicates the cfos protein concentration in the nucleus of the neuron. We use these mice to study the expression kinetics of cfos in response to seizures (via bicuculline administration [26]) and context exposure. Immediately after the seizure or context exposure, mice are subjected to in vivo imaging of the RSc as, a function of time (Additional file 3: Fig. S3a, Fig. 2a). Our imaging time points range from 20 to 280 min, typically these images are ~ 10 min apart during the initial phase and later adjusted to capture the slower decline in fluorescence with minimal number of image acquisitions. This results in a four-dimensional image stack consisting of three spatial and one temporal dimension. We note that long periods of anaesthesia can alter the IEG expression. However, for this study we limit the anaesthesia administration to two imaging sessions in all the mice (except 1) and duration of each administration to 180 min (except the single administration that lasted for 280 min). Further we also note that IEG expression profile induced by constant presence of anaesthesia will have different time profile compared to a profile triggered by distinct behavioural event. All throughout this series, we ensure that the imaging setup parameters, namely the incident power (30 mW), pulse width (~ 100 fs), excitation wavelength (900 nm), and gain of the detection system are kept constant. The resulting images show cfos-eGFP fluorescence as circular concentrated regions of higher intensities spread across the field of view. Figures 2b and 3a shows a representative snapshot of cfos-eGFP and cfos-shGFP fluorescence image of RSc respectively.
The fluorescence from these individual nuclei identified as circular ROIs can be seen to increase initially, reaches a peak, and then decays to baseline values. This temporal profile is consistent across both the seizure and behaviourally induced cfos expression (Additional file 3: Fig. S3b, Fig. 2c). The fluorescence response from few representative cells as a time series is presented in Figs. 2c and 3b. The fluorescence values from these cells are extracted as explained in Methods (Additional file 1: Fig. S1). For each imaging time point, the identified ROIs are loaded and re-centred based on the centre of mass to correct any misalignment among images of different time points. Thus, the coordinates of the measurement ROIs are shifted rather than aligning the entire image (See “Materials and methods”). This enables us to measure the fluorescence intensity using the directly observed raw intensity values without having to align the images using image registration methods. These fluorescence values are then used to obtain the cellular responses/activity as a time series. These values are then fit to the Eq. 1.
We employ the above procedure in two different sets of mice: cfos-eGFP and cfos-shGFP. In both these cases, we extract and use the fluorescence to obtain the cellular activity profiles and fit to Eq. 1. Figure 2d shows the cellular activity profile (open circles) and their fits (solid line) of four representative neurons of cfos-eGFP transgenic mice in response to context exposure. Similarly, Fig. 3c shows the fit of fluorescence obtained from neurons of cfos-shGFP mice. Additional file 3: Fig. S3c shows the cellular activity profile of four representative cells and their fit to Eq. 1 in response to seizures. The fit parameters are provided in Tables 1, 2, Additional file 3: Table S3, respectively. We see a good agreement of our model with the observed data. Since adjusted R-square (Adj. RSq) estimates the quality of a fit, we use this in our analysis to estimate the fraction of cells that fit with an Adj. R Sq. > 0.5. We see that of the 2527 number of ROIs identified as cells from five mice, ~ 75% of the cells shows a fit with an Adj. R Sq. greater than 0.5. A good agreement of the experimental data with our model in both these mice lines following seizure as well as the behavioural activation suggests that our method can identify the neuronal ensemble that represents activation, thus enabling us to use this as a criterion for identifying a cell that got activated. Further, the fluorescence signal as a function of time is well described by the analytical expression irrespective of the transgenic mice used. Thus, it supports the hypothesis that the analytical equation can be generalised to the protein expression kinetics of other IEGs (e.g., arc, zif).
Table 1.
Fig No | Cell No | Amplitude (D.U.) |
Error in Amplitude | kf (min−1) |
Error in kf | Kd (min−1) |
Error in kd | Adj-R-sq | AIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Figure 2(d) | Cell #28 | 541 | 26.39433 | 0.03675 | 0.00495 | 0.00514 | 5.31E−04 | 0.95524 | 97.85696 |
Figure 2(d) | Cell #11 | 1050 | 148.97179 | 0.02585 | 0.00614 | 0.00895 | 0.00182 | 0.94341 | 123.23644 |
Figure 2(d) | Cell #03 | 596 | 50.12666 | 0.02569 | 0.00406 | 0.00592 | 8.67E−04 | 0.93926 | 101.22503 |
Figure 2(d) | Cell #38 | 461 | 27.40859 | 0.03473 | 0.0054 | 0.00487 | 6.20E−04 | 0.93042 | 98.06544 |
Table 2.
Fig No | Cell No | Amplitude (D.U.) |
Error in Amplitude | kf (min−1) |
Error in kf | Kd (min−1) |
Error in kd | Adj-R-sq | AIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Figure 3c | Cell #501 | 1183 | 148.68142 | 0.0593 | 0.02149 | 0.01404 | 0.00272 | 0.91592 | 150.73661 |
Figure 3c | Cell #486 | 1089 | 104.53632 | 0.10002 | 0.07986 | 0.014 | 0.00265 | 0.86964 | 157.76146 |
Figure 3c | Cell #475* | 3663 | 416.218 | 0.02647 | - | 0.02647 | 0.0036 | 0.87058 | 184.62851 |
Figure 3c | Cell #430 | 1471 | 157.13412 | 0.07198 | 0.03514 | 0.0079 | 0.00176 | 0.79118 | 172.29792 |
*The kf and kd values for these cells were shared
Distribution of fit parameters demonstrate that the rise kinetics of cfos-eGFP and cfos-shGFP expression are similar
The ROIs of successful fits and their fit parameters (describing the rate of formation (kf), decay (kd) and extent of activation (A0)) are used for data analysis. We obtain the histogram of these rate constants and fit to normal distribution to estimate the mean values of these rate constants as described in Fig. 4. In case of decay constant kd we see a bimodal distribution (verified through Akaike's Information Criteria (AIC), [1]) and we report both the decay constants in Table 3.
Table 3.
Transgenic mouse | Tmax (min) | kf (min−1) | kd (min−1) | |
---|---|---|---|---|
cfos-shGFP | 41 ± 1 | 0.0369 ± 4 E−4 | 0.0062 ± 1E−4 | 0.016 ± 0.002 |
cfos-EGFP | 74.5 ± 0.75 | 0.0369 ± 2E−4 | 0.002 ± 1E−4 | 0.005 ± 7E−4 |
From these values, we estimate the rise time and decay of the cfos-eGFP fluorescence following seizure or behaviour induced activation to be 1/kf = 27 ± 3 min and 1/kd = 200 ± 28 min (taking the faster component from Table 3), respectively. We use these rise and decay times to arrive at the sampling interval for further experiments. Similarly, cfos-shGFP expression following context exposure yields a rise and decay time of 1/kf = 27 min and 1/kd = 16 min (taking the faster component from Table 3), respectively. The faster component of decay constant is an indicator of how quickly the generated protein degrades. As expected, we see that the rise kinetics are identical for both these constructs considering the fluorophore is expressed under a cfos promoter.
During this process, we observe that some of the cells have overlapping kf and kd values with high interdependency. We believe this is due to a difference in start time (when the cell got activated), resulting in a lower density of data points for a reliable estimate of the kf, rather than the lack of fit. We include some of these cell responses in Figs. 3c, 5c and d (Please see footnote of Tables 2, 4, 5).
Table 4.
Fig No | Cell No | Amplitude (D.U.) |
Error in Amplitude | kf (min−1) |
Error in kf | Kd (min−1) |
Error in kd | Adj-R-sq | AIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Figure 5c | Cell #233 | 4894 | 420.98677 | 0.09777 | 0.05748 | 0.02089 | 0.00332 | 0.9343 | 202.68672 |
Figure 5c | Cell #228 | 4856 | 282.65906 | 0.11699 | 0.07415 | 0.01537 | 0.00164 | 0.94395 | 187.96317 |
Figure 5c | Cell #66 | 1164 | 107.45433 | 0.07322 | 0.02521 | 0.017 | 0.00255 | 0.9444 | 146.82517 |
Figure 5c | Cell #239* | 3807 | 261.832 | 0.03036 | - | 0.03037 | 0.00229 | 0.94465 | 174.71616 |
Figure 5c | Cell #232 | 1802 | 492.2815 | 0.04495 | 0.0202 | 0.02197 | 0.00738 | 0.9378 | 159.5796 |
*The kf and kd values for these cells were shared
Table 5.
Fig No | Cell No | Amplitude (D.U.) | Error in Amplitude | kf (min−1) |
Error in kf | kd (min−1) |
Error in kd | td | Error in td | Adj-R-sq | AIC |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Figure 5d | Cell #344* | 1910 | 391 | 0.0306 | - | 0.0306 | 0.0066 | 82.12186 | 2.90802 | 0.88918 | 116.25422 |
Figure 5d | Cell #220 | 1777 | 131 | 0.45904 | 0.71656 | 0.0186 | 0.0016 | 86.19771 | 3.8883 | 0.97181 | 141.89924 |
Figure 5d | Cell #348* | 1501 | 130 | 0.02756 | - | 0.01692 | 0.00191 | 60.3284 | 6.61903 | 0.97706 | 124.81833 |
Figure 5d | Cell #420 | 564 | 28 | 0.27809 | 0.06721 | 0.01378 | 9.17E-04 | 88.38919 | 0.2538 | 0.97612 | 106.37056 |
Figure 5d | Cell # 328 | 1689 | 194 | 0.05502 | 0.01922 | 0.01523 | 0.00208 | 76.87335 | 3.09424 | 0.98325 | 126.67097 |
*The kf and kd values for these cells were shared
Thus, we find the time to maximal activation (setting first derivative to zero) estimated from Eq. 1 using the mean kf and kd values to be different for cfos-eGFP (74.5 ± 0.7 min) and -shGFP mice (41 ± 1 min); in accordance with the genetic makeup of the mice and the properties of the transgene. In cfos-eGFP, the transgene is the fusion of eGFP and cfos protein, while in the cfos-shGFP just the GFP protein is expressed under cfos promoter. We note that these estimates of time to peak are different than what has been estimated from conventional studies. However, these estimates are not comparable to our estimates as further elaborated in the discussion section. Given the good fit of the cfos activation data, next we ask if we could use such a model to predict expression of cfos that occurs during memory formation, thereby enabling identification of neuronal ensemble that took part in representation of memory.
Identification and segregation of neuronal ensemble following dual context exposure
In our efforts to identify and segregate the neurons that get activated in response to multiple events, we image the neuronal ensembles that respond to contextual fear conditioning. We use the following behavioural schematic (Fig. 5a) to train and image the contextual representation in cfos-shGFP transgenic mice. Briefly, the mice are trained to associate a foot shock in context A followed by safety training in context B the next day. Imaging sessions are carried out following retrieval tests that are carried out 24 h after safety training in context B.
In order to image the same region of brain and hence the cellular ensembles across the imaging sessions spanning days, we capture and utilise the vasculature of the brain as shown in Fig. 5b. This allows us to image the same region within ~ 0.5 mm (our field of view). Next, we utilise the system of co-ordinates (and co-ordinate transform as required) to further refine our positioning within the field of view. We find that these measures are sufficient to provide a localisation accuracy to locate not just the cell body but the same spines (Fig. 5b). We note that the positioning accuracy required for our purpose is at the cellular scale.
Our behavioural scheme consists of three context exposure sessions spread across two days. The mice are trained in CtxtA (shock context) and CtxtB (safe context). When tested for the fear memory they show significant freezing compared to baseline (Additional file 6: Fig. S6). Imaging is performed in two sessions during the retrieval days with first imaging session after exposure to context A. 24 h later, we image the mice following exposures to context A followed by context B. The exposures to contexts are 60 min apart. Thus, there are two imaging sessions: one following exposure to context A (IS1) on the first day of retrieval, and another after exposure to context B on the second day of retrieval (IS2). As described in the schematic, the second day retrieval consists of exposures to two contexts A and B separated by 60 min. In IS1, the imaging session consists of 20–180 min whereas in IS2 the imaging points consists of 90–300 min, at intervals of ~ 10 min. Since there are ~ 2500 ROIs to be processed, analysed, and classified, we develop a software-based workflow as described in “Materials and methods” (Additional file 1: Fig. S1).
In both these imaging sessions, we see two kinds of cellular responses: cells that are activated (I) once (single activation cell, SAC) and (II) twice (double activation cell, DAC) in response to context exposure. Figure 5c and d show representative cfos expression of SAC and DAC fit to Eqs. 1 and 2, respectively. The quality of the fit can be assessed through the residuals displayed at the bottom panel of each response. Tables 4 and 5 summarise the fit parameters. Due to their difference in initial slope and curvature around the peak of DAC compared to SAC, DAC fit the double activation cell profile corresponding to Eq. 2 better as determined by our criteria (AIC and Adj. R Sq ≥ 0.5). Additional file 4: Figure S4 illustrates this behaviour using an example cell response. The cell responses from second imaging sessions are fit twice, once with a delay of 60 min corresponding to time measurement starting from context B exposure (“delayed IS2”) and other with zero delay corresponding to time measurement starting from context A exposure. The investigated ROIs fit either Eq. 1 or 2 on at least one of the imaging sessions for us to consider it as a neuron. We find that 71 ± 6% of the investigated ROIs show a fluorescence response that fit at least in one of the imaging sessions.
Figure 5e shows the snapshot of the cellular activity at the RSc of cfos-shGFP transgenic mice. The green cells are the activated cells on imaging session 1 and the magenta are the activated cells on imaging session 2. The third panel overlays the cells that are activated on IS1 (green) and IS2 (magenta) to show the overlapping cells as white nuclei. Apart from these cells, as can be clearly seen from the images, there are cells that are activated in IS1 or IS2 only. These images represent the fraction of overlapped cells across two context exposures and this fraction is not known so far in RSc. We estimate this number to be ~ 30% (Fig. 5f white bar) and it is similar to what is observed in the other regions of the brain [30, 36].
Retrosplenial cortex ensembles in response to dual context exposure
Next, we address whether there is a differential representation of a context when a new context is introduced. In the behavioural scheme (Fig. 5a) used to train and image the contextual representation in cfos-GFP transgenic mice, the mice are subjected to context exposure three times. We are interested in observing the neurons activated in response to these exposures or experiences. A neuron can get activated in response to anyone or two or all of these experiences. Hence, we use our method to segregate the activated neurons into different categories as described below.
Based on the temporal profile of expression, we interpret the SAC that are identified following an exposure to a given context as representing that context or experience only, whereas the DAC as representing two events e.g., representing two context exposures or context exposure followed by a spontaneous event or a spontaneous event followed by a context exposure. Next, we compare these groups with our behavioural scheme to reason and assign these different classes to the corresponding memory representations, e.g., since IS1 follows retrieval event in context A and SAC IS1 responses are consistent with Eq. 1 representing single events, we assign them to context A cellular ensemble. Similarly, these SAC and DAC are assigned to one of the eight categories as listed in Table 6. The Venn diagram in Fig. 5g represents the various categories of cells representing a context and their expected session-wise fit result(s). We describe below the categories and their rationale.
Table 6.
Venn Diagram section | IS1 (Ctxt A1) | IS2 (Ctxt A2) | IS2 (CtxtB) | Physiological meaning |
---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 1 | 1 | 1 | Cell that is present in all contexts |
2 | 1 | 1 | 0 | Cell that is present in CtxtA only |
3 | 1 | 0 | 0 | Cells present in IS1 CtxtA only |
4 | 0 | 1 | 0 | Cells present in IS2 CtxtA only |
5 | 0 | 0 | 1 | Cells present in B only |
6 | 1 | 0 | 1 | Cells present in IS1 CtxtA and IS2 CtxtB |
7 | 0 | 1 | 1 | Cells present in IS1 CtxtA and IS2 CtxtB |
8 | 0 | 0 | 0 | Cells not activated in all contexts |
One subset (Category (1)) of cells that are activated in response to all three experiences, i.e., these cells show a profile that is consistent with SAC IS1 (CtxtA) and DAC IS2 (CtxtA, CtxtB).
Three categories describe the subsets of cells activated to a single experience only, namely categories (3), (4), (5). These cells have expression profiles corresponding to one of the following: SAC in IS1 (CtxtA), SAC in IS2 (CtxtA), and SAC IS2 with 60 min delay (CtxtB) respectively.
Three other categories (Categories 2, 6 and 7) represent the subset of cells that are reactivated. Category (2): These are cells that consistently respond only to context A i.e., SAC in IS1 (CtxtA) and SAC in IS2 (CtxtA). Category (6) are cells activated in IS1and fit a SAC or DAC profile and then reactivated in IS2 with a profile that can be classified to DAC or a SAC with 60 min delay. We interpret these cells as common between CtxtA IS1 and CtxtB. Similarly, category (7) are cells, activated in response to CtxtA IS2 and CtxtB, i.e. DAC in IS2.
Finally, category (8) are the ROIs/cells that are not activated in response to any of the experiences as both IS1 and IS2 responses did not fit, i.e., the ROIs did not fit as a SAC or DAC.
Following this classification system, we estimate the fraction of neurons activated in response to single context exposure(CtxtA) during the first retrieval event as 44 ± 3% (Fig. 5h, orange bar) and the fraction of cells that are activated in CtxtB, i.e., category 6, as ~ 42 ± 9% (Fig. 5h, green bar). This allowed us to ask if the ensembles representing A and B are unique. We test the uniqueness of these ensembles by estimating the fraction of the cells that are common between these two ensembles. We reason that an above chance overlap would indicate that the same population is getting activated in both the contexts, while an at chance overlap would indicate that the neurons recruited during the corresponding context retrieval are different and independent. We find the overlap fraction to be at chance (Fig. 5h, yellow bar, 19 ± 4%) suggesting that the population representation of these contexts is indeed unique and independent.
Next, we proceed to investigate if it is possible to link two distinct memory representations by retrieving these memories close in time. It is interesting to note that previously it has been shown in hippocampus, that if the memory formation were to happen within a time window, then their likelihood of overlapped neuronal representation is high [7, 33]. We ask if such overlap can be seen in Rsc following temporally close context retrieval as opposed to acquisition. We do this by estimating the chance of a neuron getting activated in CtxtA in IS1 (first exposure of context A) and chance of it getting activated in CtxtB (Fig. 5h, pink bar, 42 ± 8%). We then compare it with the chance of a neuron getting activated in CtxtA in IS2 and CtxtB simultaneously (Fig. 5h, brown bar, 55 ± 18%). Since CtxtA during IS2 and B are occurring closer in time (60-min fraction) than CtxtA in IS1 (24-h fraction), if such temporal linkage were to occur, we would see the 60-min fraction being greater than 24-h fraction. However, we find both of them are comparable despite a trend of 60 min fraction being higher than the 24 h fraction (Fig. 5h). Additionally, we find these ratios comparable to that of reactivation probability estimated as fraction of neurons from IS2 CtxtA that got activated during CtxtA IS1.
Next, we proceed to test if our method can identify double activated cells even in this scenario. We estimate the fraction of DAC in ctxtA during IS2 and compare it with DAC identified during IS1 or delayed IS2. Our comparison reveals that the fraction of DAC in CtxtA during IS2 is indeed significantly higher than DAC in IS1 or delayed IS2 (Fig. 5i). We see DAC of context A followed by B (58.3 ± 16.3%) to be significantly different (t test: p < 0.002, one tailed) than DACs of either context A or B (~ 13 ± 2%), thus establishing the fact that following a protein expression kinetics allows one to segregate cells even when the mouse is exposed to two contexts close in time.
Since cfos is an IEG immediately downstream of CREB, we ask, could the levels of cfos be predictive of reactivation probability during repeated retrieval? We estimate the reactivation probability by binning the neurons according to their amplitudes and estimating the fraction of fit neurons in each of these bins. The ratio between the number of fit or activated neurons to the total number of neurons represents the fraction of reactivated neurons in each intensity bin. We define this ratio as the probability of reactivation in that bin. Figure 6A shows this reactivation probability as a function of amplitude of cfos signal. We see a weak, but a significant correlation (R > 0.6) exists between the amplitude and the reactivation probability. In order to rule out the possibility that the difference in amplitude could bias the R.Sq of the cellular response fit and hence the reactivation probability, we also plot the Adj. R.Sq of the fits as a function of amplitude in Fig. 6B. We see that Adj. R Sq is invariant with respect to the amplitude of the cfos signal as indicated by a near zero slope of 3E-6 ± 4E−6.
Discussion
In summary, we can follow the IEG protein expression dynamics to identify and segregate the activation of neurons into different groups as per the behavioural events. Conventionally, three probes are required to mark three retrieval events; however, our method requires one IEG-fluorophore to mark three retrieval events. Since our method utilises the temporal profile of IEG expression, as opposed to intensity threshold, for identification, segregation, and classification of neurons, we are able to increase the specificity with which we identify behaviourally relevant neuronal ensembles.
Interestingly, we find cfos protein's maximum expression takes place around ~ 74 min for fos-eGFP transgenic mice, and ~ 40 min for fos-shGFP transgenic mice. These differences are consistent with shGFP having a shorter half-life.
Previous studies report cfos expression peaks between 90 and 120 min for protein as detected by immunocytochemistry (IHC) analysis [6, 26]. While we measure the cfos fluorescence in these mice and thus report the peak protein expression directly, other methods measure peak time to obtain maximum number of positive cell counts. The time to peak, in such cases represent the time at which maximum number of cells reach above threshold fluorescence. This is not necessarily the time at which cfos expression in a given neuron reaches a maximum. Since the IHC is a single point measurement in time, it lacks the ability to distinguish if the cell is in the rising or falling phase of the expression. Thus, a variation in the onset time, which is hard to control in IHC as the measurements are compared across different mice, can render cells in different phases with same fluorescence. Given the fact that the positive cells are identified using thresholds, their measurement yields an apparent peak for cell counts and the time at which this peak occurs is different from the time to peak expression of the protein itself. On the other hand, our method directly measures the rise and decay of the cfos expression from single cells through fluorescence from individual mice. Being a single cell measurement arising from same mice, limits, if not eliminates, the contribution of onset time variation. Thus, we argue that in vivo fluorescence signal is more sensitive and a direct measure of cfos-eGFP protein concentration level in a neuron in real time, compared to immunocytochemistry, leading to the discrepancy in reported time range of maximal cfos protein expression. Thus, the other methods measure the time to get maximal cell fraction [6, 16, 38] rather than the maximal expression of protein in the cell.
Estimates from our method allowed us to probe a dual context exposure interaction and show that RSc activation following different context exposures can be studied using our method.
We find the level of cellular activation elicited by CtxtA and CtxtB are nearly same and is similar to what has been found in other regions of the brain with dense encoding [36]. We see responses of ~ 20% (Data not shown) of neurons still show cellular response profile that enables them to be classified as active in context A even though the exposure has occurred 90 min prior to first imaging data point of imaging session 2. In order to arrive at an upper bound for fraction of cells that we could have missed we took the IS1 SAC data from 90 min onwards and asked what fraction of those cells would still qualify as SAC (Additional file 5: Fig. 5b). We estimate that even after 90 min we are still able to fit ~ 50% of cells. Interestingly, many of the neurons activated in context B show expression dynamics consistent with dual activation profile (Fig. 5i), we interpret this as a neuronal response to context A as well as context B exposure. Though, the activated cellular fraction at RSc has not been reported before, our estimate is comparable to the ensemble size typically obtained in other regions through conventional studies [18].
Further we assign the SAC from context B exposure and DAC from context A exposure of IS2 to context B cellular representation. We note this fraction ~ 45% for context B is similar to that of context A cellular fraction. Thus, consistent with our hypothesis, these two contextual representations can be identified and assigned to different cell populations using cellular activation profiles.
One of the defining events of IS2 is the arrival of the context B exposure after context A. It is known that contextual exposure could be temporally linked through cellular activation [7, 33]. This being the case, we expect a larger fraction of activated cells following context A exposure in IS2 should be DACs. Thus, we compare the fraction of DAC seen in response to context B following context A exposure (Fig. 5i, orange bar) with that of DAC seen in response to a single context A or B (Fig. 5i, green bar). Such a piece of information is hard to obtain through conventional molecular probing methods since the comparisons are made at a population level instead of a single cell.
One of the confounding factors in conventional method is the difficulty in assigning the cell response to behavioural event. For example, DAC in response to single context exposure, either in context A or context B, would be falsely identified as representing two contexts in conventional methods. More importantly, majority of the ROIs that did not fit either single or double activation, would have been classified as representing one of the contexts when in fact they do not represent IEG activation intended through behavioural exposure. While it is possible, the lack of fit could represent a different model of activation profile, we see a vast fraction of the non-fit ROIs to be invariant with respect to time. Since their fluorescence is measured to be above the baseline, these cells might add noise in the conventional estimates of cell fractions.
Further, we could identify the activated ensembles following different contextual memory retrieval in RSc as early as 24 h. Taking advantage of these, we probed if distinct memories once formed could be linked through closely spaced retrieval events. As expected, we find that mere retrieval of two distinct memories close in time is not sufficient to link the RSc ensembles representing these memories if the memories themselves are acquired 24 h apart.
Materials and methods
Transgenic mice
cfos-eGFP (B6.Cg-Tg(Fos/EGFP)1-3Brth/J Stock no: 014135) and cfos-shGFP (B6.Cg-Tg(Fos-tTA,Fos-EGFP*)1Mmay/J Stock no: 018306) transgenic mice were obtained from Jackson Laboratory, USA and maintained at the Central Animal Facility, IISc. All protocols were approved by the Institute Animal Ethics Committee.
Craniotomy
Transgenic mice underwent a craniotomy to enable in vivo imaging [34]. A sterile 6 mm coverglass was positioned over the skull between the bregma and lambda, centred at retrosplenial cortex (RSc). The coordinates of the imaging area (RSc: 2 mm from Bregma, 0.5 mm laterally) were arrived at by visualising the blood vasculature. The mice were anesthetised using a solution of fentanyl (0.05 mg/kg), midazolam (5 mg/kg), and medetomidin (0.5 mg/kg) dissolved in saline.
Artificially and behaviourally induced IEG expression
Artificial IEG induction was produced by injecting mouse with 2 mg/kg bicuculline intraperitonially to induce a seizure. Mild seizure symptoms were observed. On completion of the seizure, mouse was anesthetised with FMM to proceed to in vivo imaging. We injected one cfos-eGFP mouse for this experiment.
Behaviour paradigm: Mice were trained to associate a mild foot-shock (0.7 mA, 2 s) in context A (70% ethanol, spaced grill floor) on training day (2 min 30 s, Day 1). The next day, these mice were placed in context B (20% ethyl acetate, smooth floor, triangle chamber feature) without shock for 2 min 30 s (safety context training). After 24 h (Day 3), mice were placed again in context A to test for memory recall. On Day 4, mice were placed in context A followed by context B separated by a time period of 60 min to test their ability to discriminate similar contexts at a recent time (24/48 h). We used four cfos-shEGFP mice for this experiment followed by imaging. Of the four, one of the mice did not learn (no freezing in any of the retrievals).
Imaging setup
In vivo imaging was performed on a custom-built two-photon setup based on a Zeiss upright microscope (AxioExaminer Z1) equipped with a 25 × water immersion objective (NA 1.05, WD 2 mm, Olympus XLPLN25XWMP2). Femtosecond pulses from an ultrafast Ti:Sapphire laser (Newport, Tsunami) whose intensity was modulated using a half-wave plate (Thorlabs, AHWP05M) and a polarizer (Thorlabs, GL10-B) is used as light source. The excitation beam is raster scanned using a galvo scanning mirror (Thorlabs, GVSM002) before entering the microscope body and is focussed on the imaging plane using the objective lens. The fluorescence that is collected by the objective lens in an epi-illumination geometry is then separated using a dichroic before being detected by photomultiplier module (H7422, Hamamatsu Corporation, Japan).
A low noise current preamplifier (Stanford Research Systems, SR570) was used to amplify the photomultiplier tube photocurrent, which was further digitized using a data acquisition board (National Instruments, PCI-6110). ScanImage (r 3.8.1) software was used to interface instrument control and generation of galvometric scan command. Image acquisition was accomplished using a custom Matlab script interfaced with z-drive of the microscope. The digitized signal was analysed using Matlab, Origin and ImageJ for further analysis.
Estimation of fluorescence from cfos expression neurons
The in vivo images were analysed manually using a modified version of Time Series Analyzer plugin (Time Series Analyzer Plugin, Balaji 2014, https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/plugins/time-series.html) in ImageJ to quantify the fluorescence signal. The modified version of the plugin is publicly available as Java Repository inGitHub (GitHub link: https://github.com/TheNeurodynamicsLab/ImageJ_NDLPlugins). Additional file 1: Fig. S1 describes the steps to extract the fluorescence signal from each neuron to obtain the fluorescence value of a neuron at a given time point. Briefly, for each individual neuron, the peak intensity at given time point was quantified by manually selecting the nucleus as the region of interest (ROI). The mean pixel intensity of the ROI through each z-stack was obtained and fit to a Gaussian function to estimate the activity of the ROI at a particular time point (Additional file 1: Fig. S1). We are able to identify 2527 neuronal ROIs from 5 mice (3 cfos-shGFP, 2 cfos-eGFP).
Classification of SAC and DAC through curve fitting
Curve fitting analysis of fluorescence as a function of time for each ROI was done in Origin(v2020b)’s user-defined NLFit function using Levenberg–Marquardt algorithm. The parameters were set as follows for data fitting to Eq. 1: Parameter A was initialised to the maximum fluorescence of the ROI observed in the imaging session, while rate constants kf and kd are initialised to 0.01 and 0.001, respectively, at the start of the Levenberg–Marquardt algorithm for least squares minimisation. For data fitting to Eq. 2, the additional parameter td was initialised with a value of 60 min. Post data fitting to Eqs. 4 and 5, the preferred model (SAC or DAC) was selected based on Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). In brief, AIC measures the information loss incurred in choosing a fit model given the observed data and degrees of freedom. Thus, it considers the difference in number of parameters used in a fit as well as the goodness of the fit. A model with low AIC explains the observed data with minimal loss of information without over fitting, and hence, is preferred. The goodness of fit was determined by an Adj. R Sq value and it was set to be greater than 0.5 to identify the selected ROI as an activated cell of the preferred model.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgements
The authors thank the funding source for their support.
Abbreviations
- SAC
Single activation cell
- DAC
Double activation cell
- RSc
Retrosplenial cortex
- shGFP
Short half-life Green fluorescent protein
- IEG
Immediate early gene
- Adj. R sq
Adjusted R-square
Appendix
Theory
Analytical description of IEG expression dynamics
In order to model IEG expression kinetics in a neuron, we assume that a pool of mRNA is present which is translated to protein in response to behaviourally relevant neuronal activity or signal [15, 32]. In response to a signal, mRNA (A) is converted to protein (B) with a forward rate constant of protein formation (kf). One of the marked features of IEG proteins is their auto degradation resulting in transient expression. As the protein forms, ubiquitination degrades the protein, and we assume such a reaction to be first order in protein concentration with a degradation rate constant of kd. Thus, the IEG expression kinetics can be considered as consecutive first-order reactions (Fig. 1a).
where A is the concentration of the substrate for protein synthesis, B represents the number of protein molecules and C is that of the degraded products.
Given the first order nature of the reaction we proceed to write the rate equations for A and B as follows,
In order to solve for [B] the number of protein molecules that is going to be present transiently we need to solve the above coupled differential equations. We proceed by solving for B in Laplace space. Thus, using Laplace transform to transform the equations above we get,
which can be reduced to the below equations,
3 |
4 |
where A(0) is the maximum substrate concentration per signalling event. B(0) is the concentration of B at time t = 0, hence B(0) = B0. Rearranging Eq. (3), we get the value of A(s).
Using this in Eq. (4) and rearranging for ~ B(s), we get,
5 |
At this stage we have several possibilities for B(0) each one of them corresponding to different and unique physical scenarios as discussed in special cases.
Equation (5) represents the number of IEG molecules in Laplace space. Using partial fractions and obtaining the inverse Laplace transform, we have,
A fluorescence signal from a sample (F(t)) is proportional to the number of molecules (B(t)). Using the quantum efficiency (ϕf), the absorption cross-section of the fluorophore (∈A) and the collection efficiency (Cf), we can write,
For a given imaging system, the fluorescence signal (F(t)) is directly proportional to the number of molecules present at any given time (B(t)).
Thus, the equation can be rewritten as,
Eq. 1 in Fig. 1b |
Using a similar approach, we can generalise the equation for multiple activation events in a neuron, as follows:
In case of a double activation event, the equation simplifies to:
Eq. 2 in Fig. 1c |
where td is the time of the second activation event or signal.
Special cases
At this point we consider some special cases where the following physically relevant scenarios present as modifications to our equation above:
- Case (i)
- There is background non-zero steady state expression of cfos fluorescence is represented by B(0) = constant. We note that this requires the frequency of the stochastic activity such that their production and decomposition lead to a quasi-steady state. The corresponding final expression for this case is given by,
where the co-efficient of the exponent representing the decay is modified but overall functional form largely remains unchanged. - Case (ii)
Stochastic activity: First we note that stochastic activity induced expression and decay is not any different from event induced expression when the frequency of activation is sparse. (Case(i) represents where the frequency is high leading to a steady state expression). In these cases, the cell will fit to single activation or dual event activation profile with stochastic event response being one of the component.
- Case (iii)
Decay due to cellular decomposition /photo-bleaching: Such responses do not have a formation component. Our analysis in terms of classifying the cells based on activation profile does not make assumption about the nature of fluorescence decay. This could arise from cellular degradation or photo-bleaching or any other time dependent process. So, in principle our expression would cover any such decay as long as the defining parameters (such as incident intensity) are kept constant during the experiment. Thus, this would reflect as modified value for kd but not affect or alter the functional form of the resulting equation. Further, it has been shown that the time scales (ms) [22] at which the photo-bleaching affects the fluorescence intensity is longer than the pixel dwell time (4 μs) that we have used for imaging. Additional file 5: Fig. S5 shows the response of non-responsive (did not fit) cells and as one can see the responses are more or less constant showing minimal reduction in intensity as compared to the cellular responses shown in the rest of the figures (Figs. 2, 3 and 5).
As it can be seen all these cases do not alter our final expression in terms of the functional form but do modify or alter the meaning of some of the parameters. Thus, a method that does not rely on the value of the kinetic parameters but utilises the functional form to test if the cellular response conforms to it (fit) would be able to classify the cells relatively free of confounds.
Authors’ contributions
MP, SK and BJ designed the experiments. MP and SK collected the data from the behavioural and imaging experiments. MP and BJ developed the theory, analysed the data, and wrote the article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
This work is funded by grants to BJ from SERB (EMR/2017/004155), DBT IISc Partnership, Ramanujan Fellowship, and Pratiksha Trust. CSIR Fellowship to M.P (CSIR-09/079(2697)/2016-EMR-I) and CSIR Fellowship to S.K. (CSIR-09/079(2561)2012-EMR-I).
Availability of data and materials
The datasets used and/or analysed in the current study are available after publication from the corresponding author on written request.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
All protocols were approved by the Institute Animal Ethics Committee.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Automat Contr. 1974;19:716–723. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Attardo A, Lu J, Kawashima T, Okuno H, Fitzgerald JE, Bito H, Schnitzer MJ. Long-term consolidation of ensemble neural plasticity patterns in hippocampal area CA1. Cell Rep. 2018;25:640–650.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.09.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Auger SD, Maguire EA. Retrosplenial cortex indexes stability beyond the spatial domain. J Neurosci. 2018;38:1472–1481. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2602-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Auger SD, Mullally SL, Maguire EA. Retrosplenial cortex codes for permanent landmarks. PLoS ONE. 2012 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Auger SD, Zeidman P, Maguire EA. A central role for the retrosplenial cortex in de novo environmental learning. Elife. 2015 doi: 10.7554/eLife.09031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Barth AL, Gerkin RC, Dean KL. Alteration of neuronal firing properties after in vivo experience in a FosGFP transgenic mouse. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6466–6475. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4737-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cai DJ, Aharoni D, Shuman T, Shobe J, Biane J, Song W, Wei B, Veshkini M, La-Vu M, Lou J, Flores SE, Kim I, Sano Y, Zhou M, Baumgaertel K, Lavi A, Kamata M, Tuszynski M, Mayford M, Golshani P, Silva AJ. A shared neural ensemble links distinct contextual memories encoded close in time. Nature. 2016;534:115–118. doi: 10.1038/nature17955. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cajal SR. Estructura de los centros nerviosos de las aves. Rev Trimest Histol Norm y Patológica. 1888;1:1–10. [Google Scholar]
- 9.Cao VY, Ye Y, Mastwal S, Ren M, Coon M, Liu Q, Costa RM, Wang KH. Motor learning consolidates arc-expressing neuronal ensembles in secondary motor cortex. Neuron. 2015;86:1385–1392. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Cao VY, Ye Y, Mastwal SS, Lovinger DM, Costa RM, Wang KH. In vivo two-photon imaging of experience-dependent molecular changes in cortical neurons. J Vis Exp. 2013 doi: 10.3791/50148. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Cowansage KK, Shuman T, Dillingham BC, Chang A, Golshani P, Mayford M. Direct reactivation of a coherent neocortical memory of context. Neuron. 2014;84:432–441. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Czajkowski R, Jayaprakash B, Wiltgen B, Rogerson T, Guzman-Karlsson MC, Barth AL, Trachtenberg JT, Silva AJ. Encoding and storage of spatial information in the retrosplenial cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2014;111:8661–8666. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1313222111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Dragunow M, Faull R. The use of c-fos as a metabolic marker in neuronal pathway tracing. J Neurosci Methods. 1989;29:261–265. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(89)90150-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Frank AC, Huang S, Zhou M, Gdalyahu A, Kastellakis G, Silva TK, Lu E, Wen X, Poirazi P, Trachtenberg JT, Silva AJ. Hotspots of dendritic spine turnover facilitate clustered spine addition and learning and memory. Nat Commun. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02751-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Greenberg ME, Hermanowski AL, Ziff EB. Effect of protein synthesis inhibitors on growth factor activation of c-fos, c-myc, and actin gene transcription. Mol Cell Biol. 1986;6:1050–1057. doi: 10.1128/mcb.6.4.1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Guzowski JF. Insights into immediate-early gene function in hippocampal memory consolidation using antisense oligonucleotide and fluorescent imaging approaches. Hippocampus. 2002;12:86–104. doi: 10.1002/hipo.10010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Worley PF. Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:1120–1124. doi: 10.1038/16046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Guzowski JF, Worley PF. Cellular compartment analysis of temporal activity by fluorescence in situ hybridization (catFISH) Curr Protoc Neurosci. 2001;15:181–1816. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0108s15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Josselyn SA, Köhler S, Frankland PW. Finding the engram. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:521–534. doi: 10.1038/nrn4000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Josselyn SA, Tonegawa S. Memory engrams: recalling the past and imagining the future. Science (80-) 2020 doi: 10.1126/science.aaw4325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Knöpfel T. Genetically encoded optical indicators for the analysis of neuronal circuits. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13:687–700. doi: 10.1038/nrn3293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kumar S, Singh A, Singh VR, George JB, Balaji J. Saturation dynamics measures absolute cross section and generates contrast within a neuron. Biophys J. 2016 doi: 10.1016/j.bpj.2016.06.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Lin MZ, Schnitzer MJ. Genetically encoded indicators of neuronal activity. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:1142–1153. doi: 10.1038/nn.4359. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Miller AMP, Vedder LC, Law LM, Smith DM. Cues, context, and long-term memory: the role of the retrosplenial cortex in spatial cognition. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:586. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00586. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mitchell AS, Czajkowski R, Zhang N, Jeffery K, Nelson AJD. Retrosplenial cortex and its role in spatial cognition. Brain Neurosci Adv. 2018;2:239821281875709. doi: 10.1177/2398212818757098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Morgan JI, Cohen DR, Hempstead JL, Curran T. Mapping patterns of c-fos expression in the central nervous system after seizure. Science (80-) 1987;237:192. doi: 10.1126/science.3037702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nelson AJD, Hindley EL, Haddon JE, Nelson AJD, Hindley EL, Haddon JE, Vann SD, Aggleton JP. A novel role for the rat retrosplenial cortex in cognitive control. Learning Memory. 2014;90:97. doi: 10.1101/lm.032136.113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Opalka AN, Wang DV. Hippocampal efferents to retrosplenial cortex and lateral septum are required for memory acquisition. Learn Mem. 2020;27:310–318. doi: 10.1101/LM.051797.120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reijmers LG, Perkins BL, Matsuo N, Mayford M. Localization of a stable neural correlate of associative memory. Science. 2007;317:1230–1233. doi: 10.1126/science.1143839. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Repa JC, Muller J, Apergis J, Desrochers TM, Zhou Y, LeDoux JE. Two different lateral amygdala cell populations contribute to the initiation and storage of memory. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:724–731. doi: 10.1038/89512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Richards BA, Frankland PW. The persistence and transience of memory. Neuron. 2017;94:1071–1084. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.04.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Saha RN, Wissink EM, Bailey ER, Zhao M, Fargo DC, Hwang JY, Daigle KR, Fenn JD, Adelman K, Dudek SM. Rapid activity-induced transcription of Arc and other IEGs relies on poised RNA polymerase II. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14:848–856. doi: 10.1038/nn.2839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Silva AJ, Zhou Y, Rogerson T, Shobe J, Balaji J. Molecular and cellular approaches to memory allocation in neural circuits. Science (80-) 2009 doi: 10.1126/science.1174519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Trachtenberg JT, Chen BE, Knott GW, Feng G, Sanes JR, Welker E, Svoboda K. Long-term in vivo imaging of experience-dependent synaptic plasticity in adult cortex. Nature. 2002;420:788–794. doi: 10.1038/nature01273. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Tse D, Takeuchi T, Kakeyama M, Kajii Y, Okuno H, Tohyama C, Bito H, Morris RGM. Schema-dependent gene activation and memory encoding in neocortex. Science (80-) 2011;333:891–895. doi: 10.1126/science.1205274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Vazdarjanova A, Guzowski JF. Differences in hippocampal neuronal population responses to modifications of an environmental context: evidence for distinct, yet complementary, functions of CA3 and CA1 ensembles. J Neurosci. 2004;24:6489–6496. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0350-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang KH, Majewska A, Schummers J, Farley B, Hu C, Sur M, Tonegawa S. In vivo two-photon imaging reveals a role of Arc in enhancing orientation specificity in visual cortex. Cell. 2006;126:389–402. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2006.06.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wen JA, Deblois MC, Barth AL. Initiation, labile, and stabilization phases of experience-dependent plasticity at neocortical synapses. J Neurosci. 2013;33:8483–8493. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3575-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Wiltgen BJ, Zhou M, Cai Y, Balaji J, Karlsson MG, Parivash SN, Li W, Silva AJ. The hippocampus plays a selective role in the retrieval of detailed contextual memories. Curr Biol. 2010 doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The datasets used and/or analysed in the current study are available after publication from the corresponding author on written request.