Skip to main content
. 2021 Jun 25;10:e62578. doi: 10.7554/eLife.62578

Figure 6. Making point-to-point reaches improves tracking performance, especially under mirror reversal.

(A) Participants learned to counter either a visuomotor rotation (n=10) or mirror-reversal (n=10). The experimental design was similar to the main experiment except point-to-point reaching practice was almost entirely eliminated; between the early- and late-learning tracking blocks, participants only performed 15 point-to-point reaches. The purpose of these reaches was not for training but simply to assess learning in the point-to-point task. (B–D) Gain matrix analysis, identical to that in Figure 5, performed on data from the follow-up experiment. (B) Visualization of the gain matrix from one trial of each listed block, averaged across participants. (C) Off-diagonal elements of the gain matrices, averaged across participants. (D) Computed rotation angle for the rotation group’s gain matrices (upper) and gain orthogonal to mirroring axis for the mirror-reversal group (lower), averaged across participants. All error bars in this figure are SEM across participants.

Figure 6—source data 1. This file contains the results of all statistical analyses performed on the data in Figure 6C.

Figure 6.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1. Gain matrix analysis performed on single-subject data for the follow-up experiment.

Figure 6—figure supplement 1.

Gain matrix analysis, identical to the one performed in Figure 6 except performed on a single subject from each group. (A) Visualizations of gain matrices from a single trial in each listed block. (B) Average of the off-diagonal values of the gain matrix. (C) Compensation angle for the rotation group and gain orthogonal to the mirror axis for the mirror-reversal group. Note that compensation angles could not be computed for every trial using our singular value decomposition approach, so several data points are missing in the figure (see ‘Trajectory-alignment analysis’ for details on this approach).