Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Dev Psychopathol. 2021 Jan 13;34(3):1064–1078. doi: 10.1017/S0954579420001935

Gonadal and Adrenal Hormones Interact with Pubertal Maturation to Predict Depressive Symptoms in a Group of High School Females

Julia E Chafkin a,*, David S Yeager a, Joseph M O’Brien a, Hae Yeon Lee b, Ciara A McAfee a, Robert A Josephs a
PMCID: PMC8275662  NIHMSID: NIHMS1640435  PMID: 33436142

Abstract

Adolescent females are at elevated risk for the development of depression. In this study, we asked: Are pubertal hormones associated with adolescent mental health? Might this association depend on pubertal development? We tested the hypothesis that estradiol, which has been associated with adolescent social sensitivity, might interact with pubertal stage to predict depression risk at three time points in 9th and 10th grade. Hormones and pubertal development were measured in 9th grade females. Linear regression analyses were used to predict fall 9th (n=79), spring 9th (n=76) and spring 10th (n=67) grade Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) scores. The hypothesized model was not statistically significant, but exploratory analyses revealed that 2-and-3-way interactions incorporating estradiol, puberty (stage and perceived onset), and cortisol predicted current and future CDI scores. Our exploratory model did not predict changes in CDI but did account for future (spring of 9th grade) CDI scores. Specifically, estradiol was positively correlated with fall and spring 9th grade depressive symptoms in participants with high cortisol who also reported earlier stages and later perceived onset of pubertal development. These findings suggest that hormones associated with sensitivity to the social environment deserve consideration in models of adolescent depression risk.

Keywords: Developmental Endocrinology, Adolescence, Pubertal Development, Depression

Introduction

Adolescence is a time of sweeping changes across hormonal, physical, and social domains, and of increased risk for the development of psychopathology, especially in females. An examination of the National Surveys on Drug Use and Health concluded that the 12-month prevalence of Major Depressive Episodes amongst mid adolescent females (ages 12–17) is 17.3%, or nearly one in every five teenage females (Mojtabai, Olfson, & Han, 2016). In addition, subclinical adolescent depressive symptoms are more stable over time in adolescent females (Mason, Chmelka, Trudeau, & Spoth, 2017), and have been shown to strongly predict the appearance of later clinical depression in adulthood (Pine, Cohen, Cohen, & Brook, 1999). These findings suggest a need for models of risk for adolescent depressive symptoms. Such models may help to lay bare the mechanisms underlying the sudden surge in risk for depressive symptoms in adolescent females, and further, may allow us to identify individuals at risk who can benefit from behavioral and psychological interventions.

The Endocrine System in Adolescence

One of the biological hallmarks of the transition to adolescence is the peri-adolescent hormone surge, which consists of dramatic increases in hormones of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) and Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Gonadal (HPG) axes. Hormones produced by both axes have been identified as drivers of the sweeping physical, emotional, and cognitive changes associated with the adolescent period. In adolescent females, it has been suggested that cortisol and estradiol are related to emerging internalizing psychopathology during this developmental stage (Angold, Costello, & Worthman, 1998; Angold, Costello, Erkanli, & Worthman 1999). Strong evidence points to an association between cortisol and psychopathology. Specifically, the presence of high basal cortisol levels, and both hyper-and hypo-cortisol reactivity have been linked to the emergence of depressive symptoms (Angold, 2003; Colich, Kircanski, Foland-Ross, & Gotlib, 2015; Goodyer, Park, & Herbert, 2001; Hankin, Adanes, Abena, & Watamura 2010; Herane-Vives et al., 2018). On the other hand, evidence for an association between estradiol and psychopathology has been mixed (e.g. Angold et al., 1999; Balzer, Duke, Hawke, & Steinbeck, 2015; Slap, Khalid, Paikoff, Brooks-Gunn, & Warren, 1994; Susman, Dorn, & Chrousos, 1991), indicating the possible presence of one or more moderators.

Estradiol and Adolescence

Estradiol acts on neural circuitry in the limbic system and prefrontal cortices of the adolescent brain, driving many of the physical, cognitive, and emotional changes typical of the teenage years (Åslund, Leppert, Starrin, & Nilsson, 2009; Blakemore, 2008; Brooks-Gunn & Warren, 1988; Casey, Jones, & Hare, 2008; Nelson, Leibenluft, McClure, & Pine, 2005; Peper & Dahl, 2013; Somerville, 2013).

Evidence implicating estradiol in developmental changes during adolescence (Klapwijk et al., 2013; Rose, Kreuz, Holaday, Sulak, & Johnson, 1972; Sehested et al., 2000; Shirtcliff, Dahl, & Pollak, 2009; Varlinskaya, Vetter-O’Hagen, & Spear, 2013) is consistent with claims regarding estradiol’s role in activation of the so called ‘Affective node’ of the Social Information Processing Network, or SIPN: a theoretical model that seeks to explain from a neural perspective the process by which adolescents respond to social stimuli in their environment (Nelson et al., 2005). The Affective node, which, appropriately, is related to emotional responding to social stimuli, comprises several brain regions which undergo sweeping organizational and activational changes during puberty (for more detail see Nelson et al., 2005), all of which contain large numbers of gonadal hormone receptors (McEwen, 2001; Nelson et al., 2005; Romeo, 2003). These changes are thought to increase adolescent motivation for social reward and sensitivity to social rejection, and to drive the development of strong emotionality in response to social stimuli, a response pattern that is unique to adolescents (Nelson et al., 2005).

From driving physical development, to contributing to activation of specific areas of the brain, to increasing social sensitivity and awareness, it is clear that estradiol plays a number of different roles in the development of the adolescent female. These myriad changes are especially intriguing to consider in the context of increasing risk for mood disorders during this developmental period.

Estradiol and Mood Disorders

Estradiol has long been predicted to play a role in the development of female mood disorders during adolescence. However, as mentioned earlier, examinations of the association between estradiol and adolescent psychopathology, especially depressive symptoms (e.g. Angold et al., 1999; Balzer et al., 2015), have produced mixed results, ranging from significant positive associations between estradiol and mood to partial associations to no association between estradiol and mood (for a thorough review and meta-analysis of the findings in this literature, see Balzer et al., 2015). This mix of findings suggests the possible presence of one or more moderators that may be influencing the relationship between estradiol and mood in adolescence. Evidence in support of moderating influences on the estradiol-psychopathology relationship is provided by a small handful of studies of adolescent girls. In one such study, estradiol was associated with externalizing psychopathology, but only when cortisol levels were low and personality traits of disagreeableness and emotional instability were high (e.g. Tackett et al., 2015), suggesting possible dual-hormone regulation of the estradiol-psychopathology association (Mehta & Josephs, 2010; Tackett et al., 2015). These dual hormone associations stem from the theoretical framework proposed by the dual hormone hypothesis (Mehta & Josephs, 2010), which demonstrated that testosterone promotes status-seeking, but only in the presence of low cortisol levels. When cortisol levels are high, the dual hormone hypothesis predicts no promotion of status-seeking by testosterone. Findings with the dual hormone hypothesis have been mixed, and recent work has proposed that dual hormone associations may be more stable if researchers look only at status-relevant situations (Dekkers et al., 2019). In addition to work with the dual hormone hypothesis, a review of models examining relationships between adolescent hormones and negative affect suggests that the association between adolescent hormones and negative affect may be moderated by secondary sex characteristics that develop in response to pubertal maturation (Brooks-Gunn, Graber, & Paikoff, 1994). Importantly, pubertal maturation can differ on many dimensions and have complex effects: the pace of development of secondary sex characteristics, the social environment surrounding these developmental changes, and the interaction between the two could shape the female adolescent’s internal conceptualizations of her own development and overall self-worth (Brooks-Gunn et al., 1994).

Building on these ideas, we propose that the association between estradiol and mental health among females during adolescence is, in part, moderated by the adolescent’s perception of her pubertal timing, such that an adolescent who believes herself to be pubertally “out of step” with her peers will be at elevated risk for mood disorders when estradiol levels, which have been associated with increased social awareness, are high.

Pubertal Development and Mood Disorders

Putting aside the discussion of estradiol levels for a moment, pubertal onset is a well-established risk factor for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) in females (Angold et al., 1998). More specifically, much work exploring risk factors for depression conferred by the pubertal period, especially in females, has examined pubertal timing, a measure of the age of onset and rate of development of secondary sex characteristics (Marceau, Ram, Houts, Grimm, & Susman, 2011). The secondary sex characteristics are sexually dimorphic physical features that develop as a result of exposure to increased levels of gonadal hormones during the pubertal period. Measures of pubertal timing in females generally include subjective and/or objective ratings of skin changes, pubic and underarm hair growth, breast development, and age of first menses. In addition, many measures often include subjective ratings of an individual’s development relative to peers. These measures, and the ages at which they occur, have been related to the development of risk for adolescent psychopathology in both males and females (Hamlat, McCormick, Young, & Hankin, 2020). According to the deviance hypothesis (Petersen & Taylor, 1980), pubertal development that is out of step with one’s peers increases the likelihood of poor mental health outcomes by diminishing peer support and acceptance, and increasing stress and negative peer influences (Mendle, Harden, Brooks-Gunn, & Graber, 2010; Petersen & Taylor, 1980; Thompson, Hammen, & Brennan, 2016). Most of the work examining the link between pubertal timing and mental health symptoms during adolescence has linked early pubertal onset in females (i.e., “early blooming”) to risk for MDD (Graber, Lewinsohn, Seeley, & Brooks-Gunn, 1997; Graber, 2013; Marceau, Ram, & Susman, 2015; Mendle, Turkheimer, & Emery, 2007; Stice, Presnell, & Bearman, 2001), whereas relatively little research has focused on risk for depressive symptoms associated with later onset of puberty, although both early and late pubertal onset relative to peers appear to share the distinction of identifying an individual as being developmentally out-of-step with peers.

The small body of existing studies on late pubertal onset show a modest elevation in depression risk compared to ‘on time’ pubertal onset (Galvao et al., 2014; Ge & Natsuaki, 2009; Hayward et al., 1997; Mendle et al., 2007; Natsuaki et al., 2009). Associations have been reported between late menarcheal timing, which is sometimes used as a proxy for pubertal onset in biological females, and adult depression (Graber, Brooks-Gunn, & Archibald, 2005; Herva et al, 2004). It has been suggested that the weaker association between delayed pubertal onset and depressive symptoms may be due to the existence of one or more unmeasured moderators (Burt, McGue, DeMarte, Krueger, & Iacono, 2006; Hamlat et al., 2015; Lee & Styne, 2013; Oldehinkel, Verhulst, & Ormel, 2011; Reynolds & Juvonen, 2012; Seaton & Carter, 2018; Thompson et al., 2016; Whittle et al., 2012). In support of this possibility, whereas early development conferred risk for anxiety in females at the start of middle school, delayed development also conferred risk for anxiety, but not until the end of middle school (Reynolds & Juvonen, 2012). This points to the possibility that the risk represented by out-of-step pubertal timing may depend, in part, on the average pubertal status of peers, and on the extent to which one’s own status as ‘out-of-step’ is recognized and judged to be important to the individual. We propose that this second criterion is fulfilled after the pubertal estradiol surge occurs. In other words, because the pubertal surge in estradiol is delayed in females for whom pubertal development is delayed, the rise in risk for anxiety symptoms may be undetectable until puberty begins, which, for these individuals, did not occur until the end of middle school (Reynolds & Juvonen, 2012).

Current Study

In the present study, we investigate the underpinnings of adolescent depression risk by proposing a hypothesis which incorporates estradiol into a pubertal timing framework (Angold et al., 1999; Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Brooks-Gunn & Warren, 1989; Goddings, Burnett Heyes, Bird, Viner, & Blakemore, 2012; Klump, Keel, Sisk, & Burt, 2010; Nottelmann et al., 1987; Peper & Dahl, 2013; Rowe, 2002; Shulman & Scharf, 2018; Udry, 1979; Zahn et al., 2007). If developing out-of-step with peers confers an increased risk of mood and/or anxiety disorders, and estradiol, which surges at the start of puberty, sensitizes the adolescent brain so that females experience a heightened awareness of their goodness-of-fit with those around them, then estradiol and depressive symptoms may be positively correlated, but only in freshman high school females who feel pubertally out-of-step. However, because our study incorporated only ninth grade females, who are, on average, past the earliest stages of pubertal development, we expect that we will only be able to detect this risk in the late blooming, rather than early blooming, females. Additionally, as previous work (Tacket et al., 2015) has indicated that estradiol-externalizing psychopathology relationships may be moderated by cortisol, we included cortisol as an exploratory interaction term to examine whether cortisol might moderate estradiol-internalizing psychopathology relationships under some conditions.

In summary, despite evidence illustrating the prominent role played by gonadal hormones in shaping the adolescent brain (Angold & Rutter, 1992; Balzer et al., 2015; Blakemore, Burnett, & Dahl, 2010; Ducharme et al., 1976; Shirtcliff et al., 2009; Vermeersch, T’Sjoen, Kaufman, & Vincke, 2008; Vogel, Klaiber, & Broverman, 1978; Young & Altemus, 2004), further clarification is needed to describe how the puberty-driven, hormonal milieu of adolescence interacts with physical pubertal development to confer risk for depressive symptoms. Furthermore, in light of suggestions that the link between pubertal maturation and mental health likely depends on one or more unmeasured moderators (Burt et al., 2006; Hamlat et al., 2015; Lee & Styne, 2013; Oldehinkel et al., 2011; Reynolds & Juvonen, 2012; Seaton & Carter, 2018; Thompson et al., 2016; Whittle et al., 2012), an exploratory study of gonadal hormones could represent a major step toward resolving what has, to date, been a puzzle in the literature. Our approach is novel, in that it represents a simultaneous examination of pubertal development and hormone levels as risk factors for adolescent-onset depressive symptoms, using an approach in which pubertal state and endocrine activity are measured in participants during their first semester in high school (Fall semester of 9th grade), and depressive symptomology is assessed in the Fall of 9th grade, at the end (Spring) of 9th grade, and again, at the end (Spring) of 10th grade.

Methods

The present study investigated the possibility that an interaction between pubertal development and hormone levels was associated with changes in depression symptomatology in a longitudinal study of high school females. Analyses were conducted with a subsample (n=79) of the Texas Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Stress Resilience: Saturated Schools Sample (TLSASR:SSS), which will be a new public-use dataset funded by the NICHD.1

Participants

Data were collected during Fall Semester from 79 9th grade females (62% White, 28% Hispanic, 2.5% Asian, 1.3% Black or African American, 6.3% reporting “Two or More Races/Ethnicities”) enrolled at an urban public high school in Austin, Texas.2 Parental consent, child assent, and saliva samples were provided for all individuals in this sample. N=76 students provided longitudinal follow-up data on depressive symptoms in the spring semester of 9th grade; and N=67 provided 10th grade spring semester follow-up assessments. Sample size was constrained by number of students who provided saliva samples for hormone analysis. As our sample size is small (n=79) and primary outcomes described in the results section are interactional in nature, results should be interpreted with caution (for a further description of errors that can occur with small sample sizes, see Button et al., 2013). Degrees of freedom vary across analyses due to differential patterns of missing data at multiple waves. Research protocols were approved by the institutional research review board at the authors’ institution, by the research committee at the participating school district, and by the collaborating school principal.

Procedures

Participants were enrolled in a longitudinal program evaluation study at the beginning of their 9th grade school year. Active parental consent and student assent forms were collected. On three school days, salivary hormone (cortisol and estradiol) concentrations were obtained using saliva samples collected at the same time each day in the fall of 9th grade. Samples were collected in the early afternoon (1 p.m. ~ 4:30 p.m.) to reduce variability due to diurnal changes in cortisol levels (Rose et al., 1972). Time of sample collection was automatically recorded in an electronic daily intake questionnaire and controlled for in analyses as a proxy for time since waking. Students were asked to refrain from eating dairy products (i.e., yogurt), drinking caffeinated beverage (i.e., coffee, soda, tea, and energy drinks), taking nonprescribed medications, or engaging in strenuous physical exercise at least 2 hrs prior to sample collection (Adam & Kumari, 2009). Passive drool saliva was collected using 2.5 ml or 4.0 ml Salicap tubes (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany). While sitting at their desks, students were given a Salicap tube, straw, and napkin, and instructed to provide 1.5ml of saliva (for more detail on passive drool procedures, see Yeager, Lee, & Jamieson, 2016). As soon as salivary sample collection was complete, samples were transferred to a Yeti™ cooler (Austin, TX) at < 0°C, before being moved to a −80°C laboratory freezer on the UT Austin campus at the end of the same day. All samples were stored for 3–4 months in the same −90°C freezer on the UT Austin campus (between September 2016 and late December 2016) before being shipped to the biological health psychology laboratory at Brandeis University, Waltham, MA (PIs, N. Rohleder and J. Wolf) for analysis using a chemiluminescence immunoassay (IBL International, Hamburg, Germany). Samples were pipetted by a Hamilton Company liquid handling robot and measured in duplicate. Samples with a coefficient of variation (CV) > 10% underwent repeated analysis. Cortisol assay intra- and inter-assay CVs were 9.07% and 5.59%, respectively. Estradiol assay intra- and inter-assay CVs were 2.75% and 8.92% respectively.

Measures

Depressive Symptoms

Depression symptomatology was measured at three time points: Fall semester of 9th grade, Spring semester of 9th grade (approximately 8-month follow-up), and Spring semester of 10th grade (20-month follow-up), using the 27-item Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; Kovacs & Beck, 1977), from which item 9, which assesses for suicidality, was removed due to concerns for student safety. A 2015 meta-analysis of the reliability of the English version of the CDI with item 9 removed resulted in a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.841 (95% CI = 0.839–0.851) (Sun & Wang, 2015). For comparison, our within-sample Cronbach’s alpha was calculated to be 0.91 (95% CI = 0.88–0.94). Each of the CDI items asks participants to identify which of three levels of a symptom best describes how they feel (e.g. 0= I do most things O.K.; 1= I do many things wrong; 2=I do everything wrong). Scores from each item were summed together and divided by the total number of items answered to compute an average item score (ranging between 0 ~ 2). This method was employed to assess average ratings of depression symptomatology, and to avoid issues with depression sum scores arising from omission of the suicidality item.

Three waves of analyses were conducted with depressive symptoms: symptomatology in the fall of 9th grade, symptomatology in the spring of 9th grade, and symptomatology in the spring of 10th grade.

Pubertal development

The Pubertal Developmental Scale (PDS; Petersen, Crockett, Richards, & Boxer, 1988) was administered in the fall semester of 9th grade to assess adolescents’ pubertal development stage. The PDS scale asks participants to rate progression of puberty-relevant physical changes, including breast development, presence of pimples, growth spurt, body hair, and presence or absence of menstruation. Additionally, the PDS includes a question not calculated in the total score of the PDS which is about perceived pubertal onset relative to peers. This item is scored on a 1–5 scale and asks individuals to evaluate their own development relative to peers on a Likert scale (answers range from 1 = I developed much earlier than my peers, to 5 = I developed much later than my peers). For all other PDS items, a score of 1 means that growth in the given area had not yet begun, scores of 2 indicated that some growth had started, a score of 3 indicated that growth was definitely underway, and a score of 4 indicated that the student perceived growth to be complete in that area. Scores from each item were added together and divided by the number of questionnaire items to create an average composite score of pubertal development (all items except the item related to menarche were scored 1, 2, 3, or 4, whereas menarche was dichotomously scored as 1=I have not yet gotten my period, or 4=I have gotten my period). The average PDS score in our sample was 3.35 out of 4, with a range of 1.8 to 4 on a scale ranging from 1 to 4. We coded low PDS scores (less developed individuals, or individuals at an earlier pubertal stage relative to peers) as PDS scores equal to or less than 1 SD below the mean (PDS = 2.84). High PDS scores (more developed individuals, or individuals at a later pubertal stage relative to peers) were coded as PDS scores equal to or above 1 SD above the mean (PDS = 3.85). Seven participants had a PDS composite score of 4, indicating that they marked all measures of pubertal growth as complete. It should be noted that the PDS scores in our sample were restricted, ranging from 1.8 to 4.0 instead of from 1.0 to 4.0. Though this restricted range was expected given the relatively late age of our participants, it is worth noting that we were unable to examine a sample representing the full range of PDS scores for this analysis.

Data Analysis Plan

Hormone levels were averaged across the three days, z-scored, log transformed to improve non-normal distributions, and winsorized. Depressive symptoms (CDI average scores) remained untransformed. Because results associated with estradiol can be a marker for menstrual cycle phase, menstrual cycle stage was included as a covariate for all presented analyses. Ethnicity was included as an additional covariate in order to control for any group differences in hormone levels (Boileau, Barbeau, Sharma, & Bielajew, 2019). A correlation matrix of all relationships between variables are presented in the results section, as was a table of all regression analyses performed, including main effect and interaction models. Multiple regression models were used to analyze higher-order interactive effects of z-scored, winsorized values of hormones and self-reported pubertal status (PDS). We tested whether our main model, the interaction of estradiol and PDS: 1) was able to predict changes in CDI from Fall 9th grade to Spring semester 9th, and from Fall 9th grade to spring 10th grade; and 2) was correlated with CDI scores at each time point without controlling for baseline CDI score. We conducted exploratory analyses to examine the moderating effect of perceived pubertal onset relative to peers, rather than pubertal stage, on the association between estradiol and CDI scores at all time points. Additionally, in line with research examining interactions between the HPG and HPA endocrine axes (Mehta & Josephs, 2010; Mehta & Prasad, 2015; Tackett et al., 2015), we included cortisol as an interaction term. Because a portion of this exploratory analysis included two hormones and a measure of pubertal development, allowing for tests of 3-way interactions, it was necessary to complement these higher order analyses with an examination of lower order effects. Lower order effects were assessed using procedures described by Aiken and West (Aiken & West, 1991). A simple slopes analysis to test for lower order interactions involving continuous predictors was first described in 1991 (Aiken & West, 1991). In the exploratory model, this procedure allows for a test of the association between estradiol and depression symptomatology at three discrete levels of cortisol (mean cortisol, and ±1 sd from the mean cortisol levels). Non-parametric bootstrapping (with replacement, resampled 10,000 times) was completed due to the modest sample size. Bonferroni corrections were calculated in order to correct for multiple comparisons, setting our p-value criteria at 0.005. All analyses were completed in R and RStudio. The ‘Interactions’ and ‘Jtools’ packages were used to create all figures (Long, 2010; Long, 2020).

Results

Descriptive statistics are presented in table 1, and all zero-order correlations between log transformed hormone values, pubertal status, and CDI scores at all three time points are presented in table 2. In opposition to our primary hypothesis, none of our regression models accounted for change in CDI scores over time. Additionally, our original model (E x PDS stage) did not predict CDI at individual time points. However, exploratory models were successful in predicting CDI scores at individual time points. Analyses presented below therefore include main effects, two-, and-three-way analyses predicting fall 9th grade, spring 9th grade, and spring 10th grade CDI scores, all of which can be found in table 3. Results of change score analyses can be found in the supplemental materials.

Table 1.

Descriptive Statistics of Variables: sample size, mean, standard deviation, and variable distribution

Statistic N Mean St. Dev. Min Pctl(25) Pctl(75) Max
Cortisol 79 4.15 2.08 1.20 2.73 5.34 12.54
Estradiol 79 5.22 2.34 2.12 3.48 6.02 16.62
PDS 79 3.36 0.50 1.8 3.2 3.8 4
Onset 79 3.05 1.12 1 2 4 5
F 9 CDI 79 0.51 0.34 0.04 0.29 0.71 2
Sp 9 CDI 76 0.51 0.34 0.08 0.26 0.65 1.77
Sp 10 CDI 67 0.55 0.33 0.08 0.27 0.83 1.39

Note: Variables include cortisol, estradiol, pubertal stage (PDS), pubertal Onset (Onset), Fall 9th grade CDI (F 9 CDI), Spring 9th grade CDI (Sp 9 CDI), and Spring 10th grade CDI (Sp 10 CDI).

Table 2.

Zero Order Correlation Matrix

Variable log(C) log(E) PDS Onset F 9 CDI Sp 9 CDI
log(C)
log(E) −0.02
PDS 0.25* 0.08
Onset 0.02 −0.13 −0.47****
F 9 CDI 0.10 0.23* 0.24* −0.2
Sp 9 CDI 0.20* 0.14 0.27* −0.22 0.85****
Sp 10 CDI −0.05 0.17 0.06 −0.34** 0.68**** 0.58****

Note. Zero order correlation matrix of variables in models. Variables include cortisol (log(C)), estradiol (log(E)), pubertal stage (PDS), pubertal Onset (Onset), Fall 9th grade CDI (F 9 CDI), Spring 9th grade CDI (Sp 9 CDI), and Spring 10th grade CDI (Sp 10 CDI).

*

p <0.05

**

p<0.01

****

p<0.0001

Table 3.

Regression Results for one, two-, and three-way interactions predicting CDI scores

Variable DV β b SE 95% CI R2 F P
E F 9 CDI 0.305 0.105 0.047 (0.012, 0.197) 0.125 1.783 (df = 4; 50) 0.031
E Sp 9 CDI 0.278 0.091 0.045 (0.003, 0.180) 0.113* 1.591 (df = 4; 50) 0.05
E Sp 10 CDI 0.236 0.081 0.051 (−0.019, 0.181) 0.155 1.978 (df = 4; 43) 0.118
C F 9 CDI 0.043 0.015 0.047 (−0.078, 0.107) 0.040 0.522 (df = 4; 50) 0.759
C Sp 9 CDI 0.225 0.072 0.044 (−0.014, 0.159) 0.09 1.232 (df = 4; 50) 0.107
C Sp 10 CDI −0.048 −0.015 0.044 (−0.102, 0.072) 0.108 1.296 (df = 4; 43) 0.742
PDS F 9 CDI 0.168 0.060 0.060 (−0.057, 0.178) 0.057 0.759 (df = 4; 50) 0.321
PDS Sp 9 CDI 0.295 0.102 0.056 (−0.009, 0.212) 0.099 1.383 (df = 4; 50) 0.077
PDS Sp 10 CDI 0.362 0.121 0.06 (0.006, 0.236) 0.186* 2.46 (df = 4; 43) 0.045
Onset F 9 CDI −0.202 −0.061 0.042 (−0.145, 0.022) 0.077 1.041 (df = 4; 50) 0.154
Onset Sp 9 CDI −0.246 −0.072 0.04 (−0.151, 0.007) 0.099 1.366 (df = 4; 50) 0.08
Onset Sp 10 CDI −0.413 −0.12 0.039 (−0.196, −0.044) 0.267** 3.920 (df = 4; 43) 0.003 a

E x PDS F 9 CDI −0.109 −0.032 0.042 (−0.114, 0.050) 0.142 1.327 (df = 6; 48) 0.451
E x PDS Sp 9 CDI −0.081 −0.023 0.040 (−0.100, 0.055) 0.157 1.491 (df = 6; 48) 0.570
E x PDS Sp 10 CDI −0.109 −0.030 0.040 (−0.108, 0.049) 0.222 1.955 (df = 6; 41) 0.461
E x Onset F 9 CDI 0.945 0.089 0.030 (0.030, 0.149) 0.275** 3.026 (df = 6; 48) 0.005
E x Onset Sp 9 CDI 1.029 0.093 0.029 (0.037, 0.149) 0.303** 3.478 (df = 6; 48) 0.002
E x Onset Sp 10 CDI 0.171 0.018 0.036 (−0.053, 0.089) 0.281 2.673 (df = 6; 41) 0.618

E x C x PDS F 9 CDI −0.613 −0.244 0.066 (−0.374, −0.114) 0.387*** 2.776 (df = 10; 44) 0.0006
E x C x PDS Sp 9 CDI −0.551 −0.210 0.064 (−0.336, −0.084) 0.373** 2.611 (df = 10; 44) 0.002
E x C x PDS Sp 10 CDI −0.378 −0.137 0.086 (−0.306, 0.032) 0.313 1.684 (df = 10; 37) 0.121
E x C x Onset F 9 CDI 1.883 0.148 0.029 (0.093, 0.205) 0.562**** 5.634 (df = 10; 44) 5E-06
E x C x Onset Sp 9 CDI 1.817 0.138 0.027 (0.084, 0.191) 0.567**** 5.754 (df = 10; 44) 8E-06
E x C x Onset Sp 10 CDI 1.294 0.126 0.05 (0.028, 0.223) 0.432* 2.809 (df = 10; 37) 0.016

Note. Regression table including main effects, two, and three-way interactions predicting Fall 9th grade depression (CDI) (F 9 CDI), Spring 9th grade CDI (Sp 9 CDI), and Spring 10th grade CDI (Sp 10 CDI) with log transformed Cortisol (C) and Estradiol (E), Pubertal Development Stage (PDS), and perceived pubertal onset (Onset). All analyses included in table control for menstrual cycle phase and ethnicity. DV = dependent variable.

a

Bold items indicate significance at Bonferroni adjusted significance of p<0.005

*

p<0.05

**

p<0.01

***

p< 0.001

****

p<0.0001

Main Effects

In our sample of ninth grade participants, estradiol and PDS measured in the fall of 9th grade were independently and positively associated with CDI scores in the fall of 9th grade, and cortisol and PDS were additionally positively associated with CDI scores in the spring of 9th grade, though inclusion of covariates, described in the Methods section, rendered all of these associations statistically insignificant. Main effects analysis of exploratory variables, which included the perceived pubertal onset relative to peers subscale from the PDS also revealed significant associations with CDI scores in the spring of 10th grade. Analyses revealed a negative association between perceived pubertal onset relative to peers and spring 10th grade CDI scores, such that perceptions of earlier pubertal onset in the fall of 9th grade were associated with increased CDI scores in the spring of 10th grade, a result that remained significant after controlling for covariates.

Two Way Interactions Predicting CDI at Three Time Points

Results for all two-way interaction analyses, controlling for covariates are presented in table 3. In our sample of 9th grade participants, the interaction between PDS and estradiol did not predict CDI scores at any time point (fall or spring of 9th grade, or spring of 10th grade). However, an exploratory analysis revealed a significant association in which perceived pubertal onset relative to peers, rather than pubertal stage, interacted with estradiol to predict CDI scores in the fall and spring of 9th grade, but not in the spring of 10th grade. Because the Bonferroni correction reduced the p-value rejection threshold (p≤0.005), only results with spring 9th grade CDI are discussed here (supplemental materials contain figures for all findings significant at p≤0.05). An examination of lower order interactions indicated a statistically significant association between estradiol and spring 9th grade CDI scores amongst participants reporting late perceived pubertal onset relative to peers (b=0.228, SE = 0.062, p=0.0006).

Three Way Interactions Predicting CDI at Three Time Points

Results for all exploratory three-way interactions, controlling for covariates, are presented in table 3.

E x C x PDS

Among 9th grade participants, estradiol, cortisol, and pubertal stage interacted to predict CDI in the fall and spring of 9th grade. As can be seen in the leftmost panels of figures 2 and 3, fall 9th grade estradiol was positively associated with fall (Figure 2 Panel A: b=0.54, SE=0.11, p = 0.00002) and spring 9th grade (Figure 3 Panel A: b=0.43, SE=0.11, p = 0.0003) CDI scores in participants reporting early pubertal stage (−1 SD) and high cortisol levels (+1 SD). This lower order effect suggests a positive relationship between estradiol and CDI scores for late blooming participants, but only among late blooming participants with high cortisol levels. As visual inspection of this relationship revealed a possible high leverage data point, analyses were re-run without this point. As relationships remained significant, the datapoint was included in the final figures. In participants reporting more advanced pubertal stage (+1 SD), we saw an opposite, though non-significant, trend toward a negative association between estradiol and CDI scores in the fall of 9th grade (Figure 2 Panel C: b= −0.22, SE=0.11, p = 0.061) in participants with high cortisol levels (+1 SD). These results were not significant in the spring of 9th grade (Figure 3 Panel C: b= −0.18, SE=0.11, p = 0.10). Results of nonparametric bootstrapping (resampled 10,000 times with replacement) indicated these results were highly unlikely to be due to random sampling error (99% confidence interval of the interaction terms for the fall 9th grade and spring 9th grade CDI analyses were (0.0991, 0.6936) and (0.1205, 0.6265) respectively).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Fall 9th Grade C,E, PDS Correlate with Fall 9th CDI

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Fall 9th Grade C,E PDS Correlate with Spring 9th CDI

E x C x PDS Onset

Among 9th grade participants, estradiol, cortisol, and perceived onset of pubertal development predicted CDI scores in the fall and spring of 9th grade, and in the spring of 10th grade. Because the Bonferroni correction reduced the rejection threshold (p-value ≤0.005), results from the 10th grade analysis (p=0.02) will be included in the supplemental section only. As can be seen in the leftmost panels of figures 4 and 5, fall 9th grade estradiol was positively associated with fall (Figure 4 Panel A: b= 0.34, SE=0.06, p < 0.00001) and spring 9th grade (Figure 5 Panel A: b= 0.31, SE=0.06, p < 0.00001) CDI scores in participants who reported late perceived pubertal onset relative to peers and had high cortisol levels (+1 SD). This lower order effect revealed a positive association between estradiol and CDI scores for late blooming participants who also had high cortisol levels. Among participants who reported early perceived pubertal onset relative to peers who also had high cortisol levels, we saw the opposite pattern, such that estradiol was negatively associated with CDI scores in the fall of 9th grade (Figure 4 Panel C: b= −0.51, SE=0.12, p = 0.00008) and spring of 9th grade (Figure 5 Panel C: b= −0.48, SE=0.11, p = 0.0001). Results of nonparametric bootstrapping (resampled 10,000 times with replacement) indicated these results were highly unlikely to be due to random sampling error (99% confidence interval of the interaction terms for the fall 9th grade and spring 9th grade CDI analyses were (0.2603, 0.8547) and (0.1789, 0.8252) respectively).

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Fall 9th Grade C,E, Pubertal Onset Correlate with Fall 9th CDI

Figure 5.

Figure 5.

Fall 9th Grade C,E, Pubertal Onset Correlate with Spring 9th CDI

Discussion

The model described in the introduction proposed that estradiol and pubertal stage (both measured in the fall of 9th grade) would interact to predict changes in high school females’ depressive symptomatology (CDI; Kovacs & Beck, 1977). This turned out not to be the case (see supplement for analyses, in which CDI change was the dependent variable -- all n.s.). Further, estradiol and pubertal stage did not interact to predict CDI scores at any of the three time points (fall 9th grade, spring 9th grade, or spring 10th grade CDI). In light of these null findings, we wondered whether pubertal stage might lack the requisite sensitivity needed to capture an adolescent’s feeling of being out-of-step with her peers. In pursuit of this possibility, we replaced pubertal stage with the more face valid measure of perceived pubertal onset relative to peers and indeed, we saw that perceived pubertal onset interacted with estradiol to predict spring 9th grade CDI scores. Simple slopes analysis of this two-way interaction revealed that estradiol was positively correlated with CDI scores, but only in participants who reported late perceived pubertal onset relative to peers.

The inclusion of cortisol in our model (as an additional exploratory variable meant to capture stress) revealed an additional number of significant associations. We found that, among late bloomers (participants reporting either later perceived pubertal onset or earlier stage puberty relative to peers) with high cortisol, estradiol was positively associated with CDI scores in both the fall and spring of 9th grade. Finally, we found that among more developed participants (those who reported either early perceived pubertal onset or later stage puberty relative to peers) with high cortisol, estradiol was negatively associated with CDI scores in both the fall and spring of 9th grade.

This finding, that pubertal stage and perceived onset statistically moderate the association between hormones and CDI scores, suggests that hormones may be differentially associated with depressive symptoms, depending on perceived pubertal status: both stage and perceived onset. Further, the relationship between estradiol and CDI scores in both late and early bloomers with high cortisol was significant not only at the start of the freshman year of high school, but 8 months later in the spring of 9th grade, suggesting that the relationship between hormones and pubertal development during critical periods, such as the start of high school, is durable, and might be associated with long-lasting mental health risk.

Our exploratory inclusion of perceived pubertal onset relative to peers was based on literature suggesting the importance of perceived pubertal onset relative to peers, in addition to current developmental stage, in explorations of risk for internalizing symptoms during puberty (Moore, Harden, & Mendle, 2014). Additionally, as our hypothesis suggested that estradiol might increase social sensitivity and social drive, subjective reports of perceived pubertal onset relative to peers seemed to encapsulate a more socially aware measure of pubertal development than pubertal stage alone. Our understanding of our own development is significant in comparison to those around us. Indeed, analyses of pubertal development that explore the social comparison that occurs when individuals self-report pubertal development (Carter, Blazek, & Kwesele, 2020; Mendle, Beltz, Carter, & Dorn, 2019; Thompson et al., 2016), provide support for models that measure perceptions of pubertal onset in addition to stage.

The inclusion of cortisol as an exploratory variable in our work was suggested by the dual hormone hypothesis, in which testosterone’s role in status-relevant behavior is argued to depend on concentrations of cortisol, such that testosterone is associated with increases in status-seeking behavior when cortisol is low (Mehta & Josephs, 2010; Mehta & Prasad, 2015; but see, also Dekkers et al., 2019). As mentioned in the introduction of this manuscript, only one study has extended the dual hormone hypothesis to include other gonadal hormones. Tackett et al. (2015) reported that estradiol was positively associated with externalizing behaviors among adolescents with high levels of the personality traits of disagreeableness and emotional instability, but, in support of the dual hormone hypothesis, only in adolescents with low levels of cortisol. Our finding, that estradiol is positively associated with CDI in late bloomers with high cortisol, and negatively associated with CDI in early bloomers with high cortisol, extends the findings from Tackett et al. to internalizing symptoms and highlights the importance of considering developmental status in models of adolescent mental health risk.

As mentioned at the start of this report, considerable research suggests that the importance of peer approval among adolescent females translates to an increased vulnerability to peer-induced social stress. Because adrenarche is associated with an increase in stress sensitivity, and because gonadarche is associated with an increase in sensitivity to peers, gonadal and adrenal hormones may together provide a clearer picture of risk for depressive symptomatology (Adam et al., 2010; Bockting et al., 2012; Dahl et al., 1991; Goodyer, Herbert, Tamplin, & Altham, 2000; Gore, Aseltine, & Colten, 1993; Hankin, Mermelstein, & Roesch, 2007; Harkness, Stewart, & Wynne-Edwards, 2011; Larson & Ham, 1993; Rudolph et al., 2000; Rudolph, 2002; Wagner & Compas, 1990). Though we were not able to examine the impact of individual negative peer-related events on emotions and mental health in our sample, a future examination of mental health risk that examines dual hormone relationships in the context of peer-related stressors might further elucidate the mechanisms by which social stress increases risk for developing adolescent psychopathology.

These novel data raise many questions and invite speculation as to mechanisms that may underlie the development of depressive symptom risk in late-blooming 9th grade females. Because gonadal hormones play an important role in establishing the development of a social lens through which adolescents view their interpersonal world, adolescents are selectively attuned to social events in the environment and are especially sensitive to peer-based approval and validation. Further, incorporation of ideas suggested by the dual hormone hypothesis suggests that our understanding of social status and status motivation would benefit from the incorporation of gonadal and adrenal hormones. The current data show that late bloomers who are shouldering the burden of surging gonadal and adrenal hormones may be at increased risk for emerging psychopathology due to a heightened awareness of, and sensitivity to, their developmental out-group status. In other words, the confluence of developmental deviance from peers with increased awareness of and sensitivity to social difference may be creating a hospitable environment for nascent depressive symptomatology.

Unexpectedly, we found a trend toward a protective effect (lower average CDI scores) of estradiol among 9th grade participants who had high levels of cortisol and reported perceiving an earlier onset of development relative to peers (‘early bloomers’) at the start of 9th grade (see Figures 4 and 5). If estradiol serves to bring the social environment into sharp focus, and the social environment is not risky, but rather supportive, as may be the case for 9th grade females who, although once unusual for their early pubertal timing, are now, in 9th grade, in-step with many of their peers, then perhaps elevated hormone levels are best characterized as a differential susceptibility factor, rather than as a diathesis (Belsky & Pluess, 2009). There is evidence showing a positive association between perceived social support (adolescents’ perception of how much support is available if needed) and well-being during the adolescent period (Chu, Saucier, & Hafner, 2010), and a positive relationship between social standing amongst peers and well-being (Åslund et al., 2009). Additionally, our findings are interesting to consider in light of the ‘biological sensitivity to context’ framework (Boyce & Ellis, 2005). Under conditions of adversity (here, developing out of step with peers), high stress reactivity is argued to be associated with negative outcomes, whereas under conditions of support and protection (here developing in step with peers) high stress reactivity is argued to be associated with positive, protective outcomes (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Ellis, Essex, & Boyce, 2005; Ellis & Boyce, 2008; Ellis & Boyce, 2011; Ellis, Shirtcliff, Boyce, Deardorff, & Essex, 2011). Keeping in mind that our study examined baseline cortisol levels rather than stress reactivity, we would like to consider the extent to which this framing may still be a useful lens through which to view our findings. Using the theory of biological sensitivity to context, females who perceive that they are in step with the physical, emotional, and cognitive changes of their peers, may benefit from the heightened social awareness that is associated with elevated cortisol and estradiol concentrations. It is also possible that a supportive environment, per se, is not necessary for females who are in-step with most of their peers, but rather it is the lack of a risky environment, in which current development is perceived as too far outside the perceived norm, that is the significant difference. Given this hypothesis, we would expect to see elevated risk for depressive symptoms not only in late bloomers but also in early bloomers. We propose that the protective (negative) relationship between estradiol and CDI in early bloomers with high cortisol observed in our sample may be an artifact of not having captured self-reports of pubertal development amongst early bloomers as they were in the midst of early development. Though we were unable to directly test these associations in our study, future research examining the effects of the subjective experience of feeling out of step with peers over the course of the pubertal period might be an illuminating area to consider.

In our sample of 9th grade participants, estradiol was positively associated with CDI scores in late bloomers with high cortisol levels. The coincidence of high estradiol and relatively low pubertal maturation that we see in a subset of our participants, is worthy of further discussion. Because gonadal hormones drive pubertal development, one might assume a positive correlation between estradiol and PDS. In fact, in females between the ages of 9 and 12 (Ikegami et al., 2001; Zhang et al., 2008), estradiol does appear to increase in concert with physical pubertal development. Interestingly, however, as variability in estradiol levels in females increases, starting in mid-puberty and continuing into adulthood, the positive correlation between estradiol and pubertal development seen early in adolescence, when gonadal hormones are first surging, weakens and, in some cases, disappears altogether (Norjavaara, Ankarberg, & Albertsson-Wikland, 1996; Rapkin, Tsao, Turk, Anderson, & Zeltzer, 2006; Sehested et al., 2000). Further, research examining associations between estradiol levels and pubertal development is characterized, by-and-large, by a wealth of small-to-medium effect sizes (Ducharme et al., 1976; Shirtcliff et al., 2009; Vermeersch et al., 2008). One explanation for these underwhelming estradiol-puberty relationships, especially in later pubertal development, may be related to fluctuations in estradiol that occur throughout the menstrual cycle (Peper & Dahl, 2013; Vermeersch et al., 2008). In completing our analyses for this paper, we considered the possibility that our findings may have been due, in part, to hormonal fluctuations related to menstrual cycle changes. There is evidence suggesting that certain stages of the menstrual cycle are linked to the presence of heightened psychopathological symptoms (Bisaga et al., 2002; Nillni, Toufexis, & Rohan, 2011; Lahmeyer, Miller, & DeLeon-Jones, 1982; Wu, Zhou, & Huang, 2014). To address this, we conducted a preliminary analysis using menstrual cycle stage information collected from a subsample of participants included in our analysis who reported the date of the first day of their most recent period. Although no significant relationship was found, this may be due to the small number of participants who reported menstrual cycle data from this sample. We further included menstrual cycle stage as a covariate in all of our reported analyses. In addition to the influence of menstrual cycle stage on estradiol, estradiol levels have also been associated with adolescent behaviors that increase at the start of puberty, but do not necessarily increase with pubertal stage, such as risk taking and reduced inhibition (Vermeersch et al., 2008). Thus, although the modest developmental correlations between estradiol and PDS observed in some populations are not present in our sample, it is possible that the gonadal hormone levels in our sample are representative of something other than a marker of pubertal development.

The robustness of the relationship between CDI scores and estradiol in late bloomers with high cortisol– its persistent significance over the course of eight months– suggests that there may be something especially important about feeling out-of-step at a time when many of one’s peers have achieved greater gains in pubertal maturation, or perhaps, that there may be something significant about feeling out-of-step at the beginning of high school (Petersen & Taylor, 1980; Thompson et al., 2016). Evidence from studies on 9th grade students suggest that the transition to high school is particularly stressful, with many previously healthy students experiencing rapid declines in mental health that persist well into the adult years (Chen, Haas, Gillmore, & Kopak, 2011; Copeland, Shanahan, Costello, & Angold 2009; Lien, Haavet, & Dalgard, 2010). The combination of surging hormones that drive social attention and delayed development relative to peers is particularly daunting when framed within the high school environment; new territory in which the comparison is not just to same-age peers, but to more mature 17- and 18-year-old seniors.

Limitations and Future Directions

Our predicted model did not predict change in depressive symptoms over time, nor was it associated with depressive symptoms at individual time points. Instead, only our exploratory models were significantly associated with CDI scores in the fall and spring of 9th grade. The failure of our models to predict change in depressive symptoms may be related to a number of constraints, including but not limited to the demographic characteristics of our sample. In the current study, we did not oversample for depression symptoms, and as such, our study may not have been sufficiently powered to detect changes in participant depressive symptoms over time. Additionally, although there is some evidence that hormone levels predict depressive symptoms (Hérnandez-Hérnandez, Martínez-Mota, Herrera-Pérez, & Jiménez-Rubio, 2019), much research has yet to examine the specific depression symptoms that are most strongly associated with hormone levels (exceptions include Graham, Denson, Barnett, Calderwood, & Grisham, 2018; Slavich & Sacher, 2019). It might be the case that more hormonally relevant depressive symptoms, such as rumination, social isolation, or social stress, may be more strongly associated with changes in hormones over time. Future research might consider addressing these questions in larger samples with a higher percentage of individuals above the clinical cut-offs for depressive disorders in order to examine whether variation over time or in response to treatment may be linked to hormone levels.

Another limitation of our study is its small sample size (n=79). Small sample sizes, especially when combined with interaction terms and an exploratory analytical approach, are characterized by low power, and thus, suffer from a variety of issues, including an increased risk of Type II error and inflated effect sizes (Button et al., 2013). Further, the inclusion of multiple, correlated, dependent variables, coupled with the addition of flexibility in the addition or omission of covariates can increase the chance of ‘finding’ a significant effect when in fact such an effect does not exist (Simmons, Nelson, and Simonsohn, 2011). To address the last of these issues, we have included a supplemental section that includes analyses both with and without covariates. In addition, to address the broader issue of small sample size, we are planning a replication analysis of these data in a larger sample of participants (n~300), which will be preregistered with the Open Science Framework (Foster & Deardorff, 2017). We plan to report the results of this larger analysis regardless of the significance of the outcome. In this larger analysis, we also plan, if possible, to use liquid chromatography dual mass spectrometry to measure hormone levels, rather than using Chemiluminescent Enzyme Immunoassay. This choice is in line with work suggesting that immunoassay is a less reliable measurement method than mass spectrometry for measurement of steroid hormone levels (Schultheiss, Dlugash, & Mehta, 2019; Prasad, Lassetter, Welker, & Mehta, 2019). Issues related to the use of immunoassay techniques, which include heightened measurement error when steroid hormone levels are naturally low, are especially important to consider when examining our own findings, as estradiol is naturally present at low levels (Amatoury, Lee, Maguire, Ambler, & Steinbeck, 2016). In fact, it is possible that the results of our study as a whole are due to measurement error related to the use of immunoassay to measure cortisol and estradiol. As such, these results should be considered in light of the recent shift toward use of more sensitive and specific analytical tools for the measurement of steroid hormones in saliva.

Future research might also consider examining the contribution of adrenal and gonadal hormones in a wider range of adolescent females, so as to disentangle pubertal stage from biological age in terms of hormonal risk for depression. Because our sample included a restricted range of age and PDS scores (1.8 to 4.0), we were unable to disentangle these factors. Further, according to the models of adolescent social sensitivity, attention to one’s environment is, in part a function of the maturation of these endocrine systems (Nelson et al., 2005). It follows that it might be the case that adolescents with high levels of estradiol and cortisol are at greater risk of depression symptomatology when exposed to risky environments, such as those that exist for late bloomers (e.g., environments characterized by bullying, peer victimization, low socioeconomic status, relative to peers, and low perceived academic standing relative to peers -- Espelage, Bosworth, & Simon, 2000; Jackson & Goodman, 2011; Murberg & Bru, 2004; Troop-Gordon, 2017). An examination that incorporates a wider range of pubertal development might help to elucidate how relationships between hormones and pubertal development might look in adolescents at much earlier stages of pubertal development. We predict that in a sample with a broader distribution of pubertal development, our findings might replicate, such that in individuals at earlier stages of development with high cortisol levels, estradiol would still be positively correlated with CDI scores. It should also be noted that examinations of pubertal timing and perceived onset and their relationship with mental health is complex, and models of these relationships are often difficult to interpret (Beltz, Corley, Bricker, Wadsworth, & Berenbaum, 2014). As such, the implications of our findings should be considered conservatively.

In sum, these findings contribute to the theory surrounding ‘deviant’ developmental progress during the puberty and contribute to the ever-growing dual hormone literature. Whether ‘too early’ or ‘too late’, these preliminary findings appear to suggest that, depending on the hormonal milieu, the perception that one is different from peers poses significant risk for burgeoning psychopathological symptoms. This analysis hopefully highlights the importance of promoting additional research that examines mental health in late bloomers. Their story may be more complex than what we see in early bloomers, but it is no less troubling.

Supplementary Material

supplementary material

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Fall 9th Grade E Interacts with PDS Onset to Predict Spring 9th CDI

Funding Acknowledgement.

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institutes of Health under award number R01HD084772. This research was also supported by grant, P2CHD042849, Population Research Center, awarded to the Population Research Center at The University of Texas at Austin by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Footnotes

1

The TLSASR datasets are currently being processed for posting on the Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research (ICPSR) server.

2

Hormone data analyzed in this paper came from the first year of TLSASR data collection and can be found at this link along with syntax used for analysis. Future waves of data will be made available upon processing.

Conflicts of interest: none

References

  1. Adam EK, Doane LD, Zinbarg RE, Mineka S, Craske MG, & Griffith JW (2010). Prospective prediction of major depressive disorder from cortisol awakening responses in adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(6), 921–931. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Adam EK, & Kumari M (2009). Assessing salivary cortisol in large-scale, epidemiological research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(10), 1423–1436. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.06.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Aiken LS, & West SG (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Thousand Oaks, CA, US: Sage Publications, Inc. [Google Scholar]
  4. Amatoury M, Lee JW, Maguire AM, Ambler GR, & Steinbeck KS (2016). Utility of salivary enzyme immunoassays for measuring estradiol and testosterone in adolescents: A pilot study. International Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 30(1). 10.1515/ijamh-2015-0126 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Angold A, Costello EJ, Erkanli A, & Worthman CM (1999). Pubertal changes in hormone levels and depression in girls. Psychological Medicine, 29(5), 1043–1053. 10.1017/S0033291799008946 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Angold A, Costello EJ, & Worthman CM (1998). Puberty and depression: The roles of age, pubertal status and pubertal timing. Psychological Medicine, 28(1), 51–61. 10.1017/S003329179700593X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Angold Adrian. (2003). Adolescent depression, cortisol and DHEA. Psychological Medicine, 33(4), 573–581. 10.1017/S003329170300775X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Angold Adrian, & Rutter M (1992). Effects of age and pubertal status on depression in a large clinical sample. Development and Psychopathology, 4(01), 5. 10.1017/S0954579400005538 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Åslund C, Leppert J, Starrin B, & Nilsson KW (2009). Subjective Social Status and Shaming Experiences in Relation to Adolescent Depression. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(1), 55–60. 10.1001/archpedi.163.1.55 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Balzer BWR, Duke S-A, Hawke CI, & Steinbeck KS (2015). The effects of estradiol on mood and behavior in human female adolescents: A systematic review. European Journal of Pediatrics, 174(3), 289–298. 10.1007/s00431-014-2475-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Belsky J, & Pluess M (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885–908. 10.1037/a0017376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Beltz AM, Corley RP, Bricker JB, Wadsworth SJ, & Berenbaum SA (2014). Modeling Pubertal Timing and Tempo and Examining Links to Behavior Problems. Developmental Psychology, 50(12), 2715–2726. 10.1037/a0038096 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bisaga K, Petkova E, Cheng J, Davies M, Feldman JF, & Whitaker AH (2002). Menstrual Functioning and Psychopathology in a County-Wide Population of High School Girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), 1197–1204. 10.1097/00004583-200210000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blakemore S-J (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Blakemore S-J, Burnett S, & Dahl RE (2010). The role of puberty in the developing adolescent brain. Human Brain Mapping, 31(6), 926–933. 10.1002/hbm.21052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bockting CLH, Lok A, Visser I, Assies J, Koeter MW, & Schene AH (2012). Lower cortisol levels predict recurrence in remitted patients with recurrent depression: A 5.5 year prospective study. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 281–287. 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.044 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Boileau K, Barbeau K, Sharma R, & Bielajew C (2019). Ethnic differences in diurnal cortisol profiles in healthy adults: A meta-analysis. British Journal of Health Psychology, 24(4), 806–827. 10.1111/bjhp.12380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Boyce WT, & Ellis BJ (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary–developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 271–301. 10.1017/S0954579405050145 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brooks-Gunn J, & Warren MP (1988). The Psychological Significance of Secondary Sexual Characteristics in Nine- to Eleven-Year-Old Girls. Child Development, 59(4), 1061–1069. JSTOR. 10.2307/1130272 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brooks-Gunn J, & Warren MP (1989). Biological and Social Contributions to Negative Affect in Young Adolescent Girls. Child Development, 60(1), 40. 10.2307/1131069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Brooks-Gunn Jeanne, Graber JA, & Paikoff RL (1994). Studying links between hormones and negative affect: Models and measures. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4(4), 469–486. 10.1207/s15327795jra0404_2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Burt SA, McGue M, DeMarte JA, Krueger RF, & Iacono WG (2006). Timing of Menarche and the Origins of Conduct Disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(8), 890–896. 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.890 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Button KS, Ioannidis JPA, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ESJ, & Munafò MR (2013). Power failure: Why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365–376. 10.1038/nrn3475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carter R, Blazek JL, & Kwesele C (2020). Perceptions of pubertal timing relative to peers: Comparison targets and social contexts of comparison. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 26(2), 221–228. 10.1037/cdp0000287 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Casey BJ, Jones RM, & Hare TA (2008). The Adolescent Brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124(1), 111–126. 10.1196/annals.1440.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chen AC-C, Haas S, Gillmore MR, & Kopak A (2011). Trajectories of Depressive Symptoms from Adolescence to Young Adulthood: Chinese Americans vs. Non-Hispanic Whites. Research in Nursing & Health, 34(3), 176–191. 10.1002/nur.20429 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Chu PS, Saucier DA, & Hafner E (2010). Meta-Analysis of the Relationships Between Social Support and Well-Being in Children and Adolescents. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29(6), 624–645. 10.1521/jscp.2010.29.6.624 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Colich NL, Kircanski K, Foland-Ross LC, & Gotlib IH (2015). HPA-axis reactivity interacts with stage of pubertal development to predict the onset of depression. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 55, 94–101. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.02.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, & Angold A (2009). Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatric Disorders as Predictors of Young Adult Disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(7), 764. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Puig-Antich J, Nguyen NA, Al-Shabbout M, Meyer VA, & Perel J (1991). 24-Hour cortisol measures in adolescents with major depression: A controlled study. Biological Psychiatry, 30(1), 25–36. 10.1016/0006-3223(91)90067-V [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dekkers TJ, van Rentergem JAA, Meijer B, Popma A, Wagemaker E, & Huizenga HM (2019). A meta-analytical evaluation of the dual-hormone hypothesis: Does cortisol moderate the relationship between testosterone and status, dominance, risk taking, aggression, and psychopathy? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 96, 250–271. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Ducharme J-R, Forest MG, Peretti ED, Sempé M, Collu R, & Bertrand J (1976). Plasma Adrenal and Gonadal Sex Steroids in Human Pubertal Development. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 42(3), 468–476. 10.1210/jcem-42-3-468 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Ellis BJ, & Boyce WT (2008). Biological Sensitivity to Context. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(3), 183–187. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00571.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Ellis BJ, & Boyce WT (2011). Differential susceptibility to the environment: Toward an understanding of sensitivity to developmental experiences and context. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 1–5. 10.1017/S095457941000060X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, & Boyce WT (2005). Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary–developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology, 17(2), 303–328. 10.1017/S0954579405050157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Ellis BJ, Shirtcliff EA, Boyce WT, Deardorff J, & Essex MJ (2011). Quality of early family relationships and the timing and tempo of puberty: Effects depend on biological sensitivity to context. Development and Psychopathology, 23(01), 85–99. 10.1017/S0954579410000660 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Espelage DL, Bosworth K, & Simon TR (2000). Examining the Social Context of Bullying Behaviors in Early Adolescence. Journal of Counseling & Development, 78(3), 326–333. 10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb01914.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Foster ED, & Deardorff A (2017). Open Science Framework (OSF). Journal of the Medical Library Association : JMLA, 105(2), 203–206. 10.5195/jmla.2017.88 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  39. Galvao TF, Silva MT, Zimmermann IR, Souza KM, Martins SS, & Pereira MG (2014). Pubertal timing in girls and depression: A systematic review. Journal of Affective Disorders, 155, 13–19. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.10.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ge X, & Natsuaki MN (2009). In Search of Explanations for Early Pubertal Timing Effects on Developmental Psychopathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(6), 327–331. 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01661.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Goddings A-L, Burnett Heyes S, Bird G, Viner RM, & Blakemore S-J (2012). The relationship between puberty and social emotion processing. Developmental Science, 15(6), 801–811. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01174.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Goodyer IM, Herbert J, Tamplin A, & Altham PME (2000). Recent life events, cortisol, dehydroepiandrosterone and the onset of major depression in high-risk adolescents. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177(06), 499–504. 10.1192/bjp.177.6.499 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Goodyer Ian M., Park RJ, & Herbert J (2001). Psychosocial and endocrine features of chronic first-episode major depression in 8–16 year olds. Biological Psychiatry, 50(5), 351–357. 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01120-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gore S, Jr RHA, & Colten ME (1993). Gender, Social-Relationship Involvement, and Depression. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3(2), 101–125. 10.1207/s15327795jra0302_1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  45. Graber JA (2013). Pubertal timing and the development of psychopathology in adolescence and beyond. Hormones and Behavior, 64(2), 262–269. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Graber JA, Lewinsohn PM, Seeley JR, & Brooks-Gunn J (1997). Is Psychopathology Associated With the Timing of Pubertal Development? Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12), 1768–1776. 10.1097/00004583-199712000-00026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Graham BM, Denson TF, Barnett J, Calderwood C, & Grisham JR (2018). Sex Hormones Are Associated With Rumination and Interact With Emotion Regulation Strategy Choice to Predict Negative Affect in Women Following a Sad Mood Induction. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00937 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Hamlat EJ, McCormick KC, Young JF, & Hankin BL (n.d.). Early pubertal timing predicts onset and recurrence of depressive episodes in boys and girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, n/a(n/a). 10.1111/jcpp.13198 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hamlat EJ, Shapero BG, Hamilton JL, Stange JP, Abramson LY, & Alloy LB (2015). Pubertal Timing, Peer Victimization, and Body Esteem Differentially Predict Depressive Symptoms in African American and Caucasian Girls. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 35(3), 378–402. 10.1177/0272431614534071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hankin BL, Badanes LS, Abela JRZ, & Watamura SE (2010). Hypothalamic–Pituitary–Adrenal Axis Dysregulation in Dysphoric Children and Adolescents: Cortisol Reactivity to Psychosocial Stress from Preschool Through Middle Adolescence. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 484–490. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Hankin BL, Mermelstein R, & Roesch L (2007). Sex differences in adolescent depression: Stress exposure and reactivity models. Child Development, 78(1), 279–295. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00997.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Harkness KL, Stewart JG, & Wynne-Edwards KE (2011). Cortisol reactivity to social stress in adolescents: Role of depression severity and child maltreatment. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 36(2), 173–181. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.07.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Hayward C, Killen JD, Wilson DM, Hammer LD, Litt IF, Kraemer HC, … Taylor CB (1997). Psychiatric risk associated with early puberty in adolescent girls. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(2), 255–262. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Herane‐Vives A, Angel V. de, Papadopoulos A, Wise T, Chua K-C, Strawbridge R, … Cleare AJ (2018). Short-term and long-term measures of cortisol in saliva and hair in atypical and non-atypical depression. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 137(3), 216–230. 10.1111/acps.12852 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Hernández-Hernández OT, Martínez-Mota L, Herrera-Pérez JJ, & Jiménez-Rubio G (2019). Role of Estradiol in the Expression of Genes Involved in Serotonin Neurotransmission: Implications for Female Depression. Current Neuropharmacology, 17(5), 459–471. 10.2174/1570159X16666180628165107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Herva A, Jokelainen J, Pouta A, Veijola J, Timonen M, Karvonen JT, & Joukamaa M (2004). Age at menarche and depression at the age of 31 years: Findings from the Northern Finland 1966 Birth Cohort Study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 57(4), 359–362. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ikegami S, Moriwake T, Tanaka H, Inoue M, Kubo T, Suzuki S, … Seino Y (2001). An ultrasensitive assay revealed age-related changes in serum oestradiol at low concentrations in both sexes from infancy to puberty. Clinical Endocrinology, 55(6), 789–795. 10.1046/j.1365-2265.2001.01416.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Jackson B, & Goodman E (2011). Low Social Status Markers: Do They Predict Depressive Symptoms in Adolescence? Race and Social Problems, 3(2), 119–128. 10.1007/s12552-011-9047-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Klapwijk ET, Goddings A-L, Burnett Heyes S, Bird G, Viner RM, & Blakemore S-J (2013). Increased functional connectivity with puberty in the mentalising network involved in social emotion processing. Hormones and Behavior, 64(2), 314–322. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Klump KL, Keel PK, Sisk C, & Burt SA (2010). Preliminary evidence that estradiol moderates genetic influences on disordered eating attitudes and behaviors during puberty. Psychological Medicine, 40(10), 1745–1753. 10.1017/S0033291709992236 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Kovacs M, & Beck A (1977). An empirical-clinical approach toward a definition of childhood depression. Depression in Childhood: Diagnosis, Treatment, and Conceptual Models, 1–25. [Google Scholar]
  62. Lahmeyer HW, Miller M, & DeLeon-Jones F (1982). Anxiety and mood fluctuation during the normal menstrual cycle. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44(2), 183–194. 10.1097/00006842-198205000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Larson R, & Ham M (1993). Stress and “storm and stress” in early adolescence: The relationship of negative events with dysphoric affect. Developmental Psychology, 29(1), 130–140. 10.1037/0012-1649.29.1.130 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  64. Lee Y, & Styne D (2013). Influences on the onset and tempo of puberty in human beings and implications for adolescent psychological development. Hormones and Behavior, 64(2), 250–261. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Lien L, Haavet OR, & Dalgard F (2010). Do mental health and behavioural problems of early menarche persist into late adolescence? A three year follow-up study among adolescent girls in Oslo, Norway. Social Science & Medicine, 71(3), 529–533. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.05.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Long JA (2019). interactions: Comprehensive, User-Friendly Toolkit for Probing Interactions (Version 1.1.0) [R package]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=interactions.
  67. Long JA (2020). Jtools: Analysis and Presentation of Social Scientific Data. R (Version 2.1.0) [R package]. Retrieved from https://cran.r-project.org/package=jtools
  68. Marceau K, Ram N, Houts RM, Grimm KJ, & Susman EJ (2011). Individual differences in boys’ and girls’ timing and tempo of puberty: Modeling development with nonlinear growth models. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1389–1409. 10.1037/a0023838 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Marceau K, Ram N, & Susman EJ (2015). Development and Lability in the Parent–Child Relationship During Adolescence: Associations With Pubertal Timing and Tempo. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25(3), 474–489. 10.1111/jora.12139 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Mason WA, Chmelka MB, Trudeau L, & Spoth RL (2017). Gender Moderation of the Intergenerational Transmission and Stability of Depressive Symptoms from Early Adolescence to Early Adulthood. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 46(1), 248–260. 10.1007/s10964-016-0480-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. McEwen BS (2001). Invited Review: Estrogens effects on the brain: multiple sites and molecular mechanisms. Journal of Applied Physiology, 91(6), 2785–2801. 10.1152/jappl.2001.91.6.2785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mehta PH, & Josephs RA (2010). Testosterone and cortisol jointly regulate dominance: Evidence for a dual-hormone hypothesis. Hormones and Behavior, 58(5), 898–906. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mehta PH, & Prasad S (2015). The dual-hormone hypothesis: A brief review and future research agenda. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 163–168. [Google Scholar]
  74. Mendle J, Beltz AM, Carter R, & Dorn LD (2019). Understanding Puberty and Its Measurement: Ideas for Research in a New Generation. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 29(1), 82–95. 10.1111/jora.12371 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Mendle J, Harden KP, Brooks-Gunn J, & Graber JA (2010). Development’s Tortoise and Hare: Pubertal Timing, Pubertal Tempo, and Depressive Symptoms in Boys and Girls. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1341–1353. 10.1037/a0020205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Mendle J, Turkheimer E, & Emery RE (2007). Detrimental Psychological Outcomes Associated with Early Pubertal Timing in Adolescent Girls. Developmental Review : DR, 27(2), 151–171. 10.1016/j.dr.2006.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Mojtabai R, Olfson M, & Han B (2016). National Trends in the Prevalence and Treatment of Depression in Adolescents and Young Adults. Pediatrics, 138(6). 10.1542/peds.2016-1878 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Moore SR, Harden KP, & Mendle J (2014). Pubertal timing and adolescent sexual behavior in girls. Developmental Psychology, 50(6), 1734–1745. 10.1037/a0036027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Murberg TA, & Bru E (2004). School-Related Stress and Psychosomatic Symptoms among Norwegian Adolescents. School Psychology International, 25(3), 317–332. 10.1177/0143034304046904 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  80. Natsuaki MN, Klimes-Dougan B, Ge X, Shirtcliff EA, Hastings PD, & Zahn-Waxler C (2009). Early pubertal maturation and internalizing problems in adolescence: Sex differences in the role of cortisol reactivity to interpersonal stress. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 38(4), 513–524. 10.1080/15374410902976320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Nelson EE, Leibenluft E, McCLURE EB, & Pine DS (2005). The social re-orientation of adolescence: A neuroscience perspective on the process and its relation to psychopathology. Psychological Medicine, 35(2), 163–174. 10.1017/S0033291704003915 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Nillni YI, Toufexis DJ, & Rohan KJ (2011). Anxiety sensitivity, the menstrual cycle, and panic disorder: A putative neuroendocrine and psychological interaction. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(7), 1183–1191. 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.07.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Norjavaara E, Ankarberg C, & Albertsson-Wikland K (1996). Diurnal rhythm of 17 beta-estradiol secretion throughout pubertal development in healthy girls: Evaluation by a sensitive radioimmunoassay. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 81(11), 4095–4102. 10.1210/jcem.81.11.8923866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Nottelmann ED, Susman EJ, Inoff-Germain G, Cutler GB, Loriaux DL, & Chrousos GP (1987). Developmental processes in early adolescence: Relationships between adolescent adjustment problems and chronologic age, pubertal stage, and puberty-related serum hormone levels. The Journal of Pediatrics, 110(3), 473–480. 10.1016/S0022-3476(87)80521-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Oldehinkel AJ, Verhulst FC, & Ormel J (2011). Mental health problems during puberty: Tanner stage-related differences in specific symptoms. The TRAILS study. Journal of Adolescence, 34(1), 73–85. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.01.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Peper JS, & Dahl RE (2013). The Teenage Brain: Surging Hormones—Brain-Behavior Interactions During Puberty. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 134–139. 10.1177/0963721412473755 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Petersen AC, Crockett L, Richards M, & Boxer A (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status: Reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–133. 10.1007/BF01537962 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Petersen AC, & Taylor B (1980). The biological approach to adolescence: Biological change and psychological adaptation. Handbook of Adolescent Psychology, 117155. [Google Scholar]
  89. Pine DS, Cohen E, Cohen P, & Brook J (1999). Adolescent Depressive Symptoms as Predictors of Adult Depression: Moodiness or Mood Disorder? American Journal of Psychiatry, 156(1), 133–135. 10.1176/ajp.156.1.133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Prasad S, Lassetter B, Welker KM, & Mehta PH (2019). Unstable correspondence between salivary testosterone measured with enzyme immunoassays and tandem mass spectrometry. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 109, 104373. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. R Studio Team. (2018). RStudio: Integrated Development for R (Version 1.2.1335) [R Studio]. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. Retrieved from http://www.rstudio.com/ [Google Scholar]
  92. Rapkin AJ, Tsao JCI, Turk N, Anderson M, & Zeltzer LK (2006). Relationships among Self-Rated Tanner Staging, Hormones, and Psychosocial Factors in Healthy Female Adolescents. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 19(3), 181–187. 10.1016/j.jpag.2006.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Reynolds BM, & Juvonen J (2012). Pubertal Timing Fluctuations across Middle School: Implications for Girls’ Psychological Health. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41(6), 677–690. 10.1007/s10964-011-9687-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Romeo RD (2003). Puberty: A Period of Both Organizational and Activational Effects of Steroid Hormones On Neurobehavioural Development. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 15(12), 1185–1192. 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2003.01106.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Rose RM, Kreuz LE, Holaday JW, Sulak KJ, & Johnson CE (1972). Diurnal variation of plasma testosterone and cortisol. Journal of Endocrinology, 177–178. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Rowe DC (2002). On genetic variation in menarche and age at first sexual intercourse: A critique of the Belsky–Draper hypothesis. Evolution and Human Behavior, 23(5), 365–372. 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00102-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Rudolph KD (2002). Gender differences in emotional responses to interpersonal stress during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30(4), 3–13. 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00383-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Rudolph KD, Hammen C, Burge D, Lindberg N, Herzberg D, & Daley SE (2000). Toward an interpersonal life-stress model of depression: The developmental context of stress generation. Development and Psychopathology, 12(2), 215–234. 10.1017/S0954579400002066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Schultheiss OC, Dlugash G, & Mehta PH (2018). Hormone measurement in social neuroendocrinology. In Schultheiss OC & Mehta PH (Eds.), Routledge International Handbook of Social Neuroendocrinology (1st ed., pp. 26–40). Abingdon, Oxon ; New York, NY:  : Routledge, 2019.: Routledge. 10.4324/9781315200439-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  100. Seaton EK, & Carter R (2018). Pubertal timing, racial identity, neighborhood, and school context among Black adolescent females. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 24(1), 40–50. 10.1037/cdp0000162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Sehested A, Juul A, Andersson AM, Petersen JH, Jensen TK, Müller J, & Skakkebaek NE (2000). Serum Inhibin A and Inhibin B in Healthy Prepubertal, Pubertal, and Adolescent Girls and Adult Women: Relation to Age, Stage of Puberty, Menstrual Cycle, Follicle-Stimulating Hormone, Luteinizing Hormone, and Estradiol Levels*. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 85(4), 1634–1640. 10.1210/jcem.85.4.6512 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Shirtcliff EA, Dahl RE, & Pollak SD (2009). Pubertal Development: Correspondence Between Hormonal and Physical Development. Child Development, 80(2), 327–337. 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01263.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Shulman S, & Scharf M (2018). Adolescent psychopathology in Times of Change: The need for integrating a developmental psychopathology perspective. Journal of Adolescence, 65, 95–100. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.03.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Simmons JP, Nelson LD, & Simonsohn U (2011). False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–1366. 10.1177/0956797611417632 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Slap GB, Khalid N, Paikoff RL, Brooks-Gunn J, & Warren MP (1994). Evolving self-image, pubertal manifestations, and pubertal hormones: Preliminary findings in young adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescent Health, 15(4), 327–335. 10.1016/1054-139X(94)90606-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Slavich GM, & Sacher J (2019). Stress, sex hormones, inflammation, and major depressive disorder: Extending Social Signal Transduction Theory of Depression to account for sex differences in mood disorders. Psychopharmacology, 236(10), 3063–3079. 10.1007/s00213-019-05326-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Stice E, Presnell K, & Bearman SK (2001). Relation of early menarche to depression, eating disorders, substance abuse, and comorbid psychopathology among adolescent girls. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 608–619. 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.608 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Stoff DM, & Susman EJ (2005). Developmental Psychobiology of Aggression. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
  109. Sun S, & Wang S (2015). The Children’s Depression Inventory in Worldwide Child Development Research: A Reliability Generalization Study. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 24(8), 2352–2363. 10.1007/s10826-014-0038-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  110. Susman EJ, Dorn LD, & Chrousos GP (1991). Negative affect and hormone levels in young adolescents: Concurrent and predictive perspectives. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 20(2), 167–190. 10.1007/BF01537607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Tackett JL, Reardon KW, Herzhoff K, Page-Gould E, Harden KP, & Josephs RA (2015). Estradiol and cortisol interactions in youth externalizing psychopathology. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 55, 146–153. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.02.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Thompson SM, Hammen C, & Brennan PA (2016). The Impact of Asynchronous Pubertal Development on Depressive Symptoms in Adolescence and Emerging Adulthood Among Females. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 45(3), 494–504. 10.1007/s10964-015-0402-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Troop-Gordon W (2017). Peer victimization in adolescence: The nature, progression, and consequences of being bullied within a developmental context. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 116–128. 10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Udry JR (1979). Age at menarche, at first intercourse, and at first pregnancy. Journal of Biosocial Science, 11(4), 433–441. 10.1017/S0021932000012517 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Varlinskaya EI, Vetter-O’Hagen CS, & Spear LP (2013). Puberty and gonadal hormones: Role in adolescent-typical behavioral alterations. Hormones and Behavior, 64(2), 343–349. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.11.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Vermeersch H, T’Sjoen G, Kaufman J-M, & Vincke J (2008). Estradiol, testosterone, differential association and aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent girls. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 33(7), 897–908. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Vogel W, Klaiber EL, & Broverman DM (1978). Roles of the gonadal steroid hormones in psychiatric depression in men and women. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology, 2(4), 487–503. 10.1016/0364-7722(78)90107-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  118. Wagner BM, & Compas BE (1990). Gender, instrumentality, and expressivity: Moderators of the relation between stress and psychological symptoms during adolescence. American Journal of Community Psychology, 18(3), 383–406. 10.1007/BF00938114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Whittle S, Yücel M, Lorenzetti V, Byrne ML, Simmons JG, Wood SJ, … Allen NB (2012). Pituitary volume mediates the relationship between pubertal timing and depressive symptoms during adolescence. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(7), 881–891. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.10.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Wu M, Zhou R, & Huang Y (2014). Effects of menstrual cycle and neuroticism on females’ emotion regulation. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 94(3), 351–357. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Yeager DS, Lee HY, & Jamieson JP (2016). How to Improve Adolescent Stress Responses: Insights From Integrating Implicit Theories of Personality and Biopsychosocial Models. Psychological Science, 27(8), 1078–1091. 10.1177/0956797616649604 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Young EA, & Altemus M (2004). Puberty, Ovarian Steroids, and Stress. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1021(1), 124–133. 10.1196/annals.1308.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Zahn R, Moll J, Krueger F, Huey ED, Garrido G, & Grafman J (2007). Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(15), 6430–6435. 10.1073/pnas.0607061104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Zhang K, Pollack S, Ghods A, Dicken C, Isaac B, Adel G, … Santoro N (2008). Onset of Ovulation after Menarche in Girls: A Longitudinal Study. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 93(4), 1186–1194. 10.1210/jc.2007-1846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

supplementary material

RESOURCES