Skip to main content
Sage Choice logoLink to Sage Choice
. 2021 May 23;36(4):384–394. doi: 10.1177/07487304211013995

Regular Caffeine Intake Delays REM Sleep Promotion and Attenuates Sleep Quality in Healthy Men

Janine Weibel *,, Yu-Shiuan Lin *,†,, Hans-Peter Landolt §,, Christian Berthomier , Marie Brandewinder , Joshua Kistler *,, Sophia Rehm #, Katharina M Rentsch #, Martin Meyer **, Stefan Borgwardt , Christian Cajochen *,†,1,2, Carolin F Reichert *,†,1
PMCID: PMC8276335  PMID: 34024173

Abstract

Acute caffeine intake can attenuate homeostatic sleep pressure and worsen sleep quality. Caffeine intake—particularly in high doses and close to bedtime—may also affect circadian-regulated rapid eye movement (REM) sleep promotion, an important determinant of subjective sleep quality. However, it is not known whether such changes persist under chronic caffeine consumption during daytime. Twenty male caffeine consumers (26.4 ± 4 years old, habitual caffeine intake 478.1 ± 102.8 mg/day) participated in a double-blind crossover study. Each volunteer completed a caffeine (3 × 150 mg caffeine daily for 10 days), a withdrawal (3 × 150 mg caffeine for 8 days then placebo), and a placebo condition. After 10 days of controlled intake and a fixed sleep-wake cycle, we recorded electroencephalography for 8 h starting 5 h after habitual bedtime (i.e., start on average at 04:22 h which is around the peak of circadian REM sleep promotion). A 60-min evening nap preceded each sleep episode and reduced high sleep pressure levels. While total sleep time and sleep architecture did not significantly differ between the three conditions, REM sleep latency was longer after daily caffeine intake compared with both placebo and withdrawal. Moreover, the accumulation of REM sleep proportion was delayed, and volunteers reported more difficulties with awakening after sleep and feeling more tired upon wake-up in the caffeine condition compared with placebo. Our data indicate that besides acute intake, also regular daytime caffeine intake affects REM sleep regulation in men, such that it delays circadian REM sleep promotion when compared with placebo. Moreover, the observed caffeine-induced deterioration in the quality of awakening may suggest a potential motive to reinstate caffeine intake after sleep.

Keywords: caffeine, withdrawal, sleep, electroencephalography, REM sleep, circadian


Caffeine has been used for centuries (Camandola et al., 2019) and is considered to be today’s most popular stimulating substance around the globe (Fredholm et al., 1999). Approximately 80% of the world’s population consumes caffeinated aliments day by day (Heckman et al., 2010). The common pattern of caffeine intake in the morning and afternoon (Martyn et al., 2018; Lieberman et al., 2019) most likely originates from the motive to achieve both benefits in daytime alertness (Smith, 2002; Einöther and Giesbrecht, 2013) and a good subjective nighttime sleep quality despite of previous stimulant consumption (Snel and Lorist, 2011). These double-edged alerting but sleep-disrupting effects of caffeine have been attributed to its impact on the homeostatic component of sleep-wake regulation (Landolt, 2008). By antagonizing adenosine receptors (Fredholm et al., 1999), caffeine acutely reduces homeostatic sleep pressure (Landolt, 2008), as evident in caffeine-induced reductions of waking electroencephalography (EEG) theta activity (Landolt et al., 2004), slow-wave sleep (SWS), and slow-wave activity (SWA), while it increases activity in the sigma range (Landolt, Dijk, et al., 1995).

Importantly, structure and intensity of sleep are determined not only by homeostatic sleep need but also by the circadian timing system (Borbély, 1982; Lazar et al., 2015). One of the most prominent circadian sleep features is rapid eye movement (REM) sleep propensity, which usually peaks in the morning around 2 h after the nadir of core body temperature (Dijk and Czeisler, 1995). This natural peak right before usual wake time may facilitate the rearousal of the brain from sleep (Dijk and Czeisler, 1995) and may contribute to REM sleep’s substantial promotion of good subjective sleep quality (Akerstedt et al., 1994; Della Monica et al., 2018). Since the choice to consume caffeine might also strongly depend on an individual’s sleep quality, it remains to be established how daily caffeine intake affects REM sleep and its role in promoting sleep quality.

There is evidence for acute effects of caffeine on REM sleep. In animal models, the perfusion of the sleep factor adenosine increased the time spent in REM sleep (Portas et al., 1997; Basheer et al., 1999). Regarding nighttime sleep in humans, some studies—particularly those utilizing high caffeine dosages (but see Bonnet and Arand, 1992; Drake et al., 2013)—report a caffeine-induced reduction of REM sleep duration (Brezinova, 1974; Nicholson and Stone, 1980; Robillard et al., 2015) or shift in REM sleep episodes (Karacan et al., 1976; Nicholson and Stone, 1980), while others did not show any differences in REM sleep after caffeine administration (Bonnet and Arand, 1992; Landolt, Dijk, et al., 1995; Landolt, Werth, et al., 1995; Drapeau et al., 2006; Drake et al., 2013). When sleep was initiated around the peak of REM sleep propensity (i.e., 1 h after habitual wake time) following one night of sleep loss, caffeine intake right before bedtime increased wakefulness at the cost of REM sleep (Carrier et al., 2007, 2009). It remains unknown whether such effects during daytime recovery sleep can also be observed under conditions of regular daily daytime caffeine intake.

Regular daytime caffeine intake can lead to adaptations and thus mitigate both caffeine-induced wake promotion and nighttime sleep disturbances. Such adaptations are represented in the occurrence of withdrawal symptoms within around 36 h after acute cessation of caffeine, and comprise increased sleepiness (James, 1998; Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020), enhanced waking EEG theta activity (Sigmon et al., 2009), and reduced sleep EEG sigma activity (Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Kistler, et al., 2020). Thus, comparing withdrawal-induced effects on sleep against long-term abstinence and habitual use in the same individuals represents a valid tool to reliably estimate the consequences of daily caffeine intake. For this report, we took advantage of an existing data set with exactly these conditions, in which the start of each of the sleep episodes was individually scheduled around the circadian peak of REM sleep promotion. Previous sleep-wake history, light input, posture, meal intake, and circadian phase (estimated by dim-light melatonin onset) were carefully controlled and did not differ between conditions or within individuals (Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020). Based on the evidence summarized above, we explored whether the duration and timing of REM sleep change in response to daily daytime caffeine intake (over 10 days) and acute caffeine withdrawal, compared with a long-term (10-day) placebo baseline. In a second step, we tested whether changes in REM sleep relate to differences in subjective sleep quality.

Methods

Volunteers

Data sets were available from a total of 20 male study volunteers between 18 and 35 years old. All participants were regular caffeine consumers with a daily intake between 300 and 600 mg assessed by a survey tool based on Bühler et al. (2013) and its caffeine content classified according to Snel and Lorist (2011). Prior to study participation, all volunteers were screened for good health assessed by self-reports and a medical check performed by a study physician. Individuals reporting a body mass index (BMI) <18 or >26 were excluded. Good sleep quality was ensured by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; score ≤5) (Buysse et al., 1989) and a polysomnography (PSG) during which we screened for sleep apnea (index >10/h), period leg movements (index >15/h), and poor sleep efficiency (SE < 70%). Smoking, drug use, or extreme chronotype (score ≤30 or ≥70 in the Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (Horne and Ostberg, 1976)) resulted in the exclusion of participants. In addition, volunteers were not allowed to engage in shift work (<3 months prior to study) or to travel across more than two time zones (<1 month prior to study). To minimize the potential confounding influence by the menstrual cycle and the use of oral contraceptives on sleep (Shechter and Boivin, 2010) as well as caffeine elimination (Abernethy and Todd, 1985; Balogh et al., 1995), female individuals were not included in this study. Demographical data of the study sample can be found in Table 1.

Table 1.

Demographical data of the study sample.

Sample Characteristics (N = 20) M ± SD
Years of age 26.4 ± 4.0
Habitual caffeine intake (mg/day) 478.1 ± 102.8
BMI (kg/m2) 22.7 ± 1.4
Chronotype (MEQ) 52.8 ± 8.7
Sleep quality (PSQI) 2.8 ± 1.4
Habitual bedtime a (hh:mm) 23:21 ± 00:49
Habitual sleep duration a (h) 7.5 ± 0.4

Abbreviations: BMI = body mass index; MEQ = Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire (Horne and Ostberg, 1976); PSQI = Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (Buysse et al., 1989).

a.

Self-reported.

Design and Protocol

We employed a double-blind crossover study with three treatments: a caffeine, a withdrawal, and a placebo condition. Random permutations were performed to assign the volunteers to the order of the three conditions, for more details, see Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al. (2020). As depicted in Figure 1a, each condition comprised an ambulatory part of 9 days which was followed by an in-lab part of 43 h. On average, the three conditions were separated by 9.6 days (range: 1-33 days). In each condition, volunteers swallowed identical-appearing gelatin capsules 3 times daily (45, 255, and 475 min after awakening) containing either caffeine (150 mg; Hänseler AG, Herisau, Switzerland) or placebo (mannitol; Hänseler AG). To induce caffeine withdrawal in the withdrawal condition, the first capsule on day 9 of treatment contained caffeine but was followed by placebo capsules for the remaining administrations. The dosage and timing of administrations were chosen based on previous studies which investigated tolerance development to the effects of caffeine and its cessation (James, 1998; Keane and James, 2008) and to represent an everyday situation of most coffee consumers (Martyn et al., 2018; Lieberman et al., 2019). Volunteers were instructed to abstain from all other caffeine sources during the entire study duration. Compliance to the aforementioned regimen was verified by assessing caffeine levels in fingertip sweat collected prior to habitual bedtime during the ambulatory part of 9 days (see supplemental material of Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020). Moreover, volunteers were instructed to keep a regular sleep-wake rhythm (±30 min of self-selected bedtime and waketime, 8 h in bed) and to avoid naps. Their compliance was monitored by wrist actimetry (Actiwatch, Cambridge Neurotechnology, Cambridge, United Kingdom) with concurrent sleep logs. Sleep schedule remained constant across all three conditions except for three volunteers (two volunteers: +30 min in caffeine compared with placebo and withdrawal conditions; one volunteer: −30 min in placebo compared with caffeine and withdrawal conditions).

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Illustration of the research protocol. Adapted from Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al. (2020).

(a) Each participant took part in a placebo, a caffeine, and a withdrawal condition consisting of an ambulatory part of 9 days and an in-lab part of 43 h. (b) The in-lab protocol started with a baseline night scheduled to volunteers’ habitual bedtime. On the following day, we scheduled a 1-h nap in the evening and salivary caffeine levels were collected in regular intervals. Five hours after usual bedtime, an 8-h sleep episode was scheduled, and subjective sleep quality was assessed afterwards. Abbreviation: LSEQ = Leeds Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire.

As illustrated in Figure 1b, on day 9 of treatment, volunteers reported to the laboratory 5.5 h prior to habitual bedtime. Upon arrival, PSG electrodes were fitted and a sleep episode of 8 h took place at the volunteer’s habitual bedtime. The following day, saliva was regularly collected to quantify the levels of caffeine and its main metabolites. In the evening, we scheduled a 1-h nap (for details, see Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al. (2020)), which was followed by another wakefulness episode. To minimize masking effects during the in-lab protocol, no time-of-day information was provided, communication was restricted to staff members, and light (scheduled wakefulness: <8 lux; sleep episode: 0 lux) and posture (scheduled wakefulness: semirecumbent; sleep episode: supine) were controlled.

Approximately 5 h after volunteers’ habitual bedtime (mean: 04:22 h; range: 03:15-05:15 h), a sleep episode of 8 h was scheduled, which corresponds to the expected circadian peak of REM sleep promotion (Dijk and Czeisler, 1995; Münch et al., 2005), and was 13.5 and 44.5 h after the last caffeine intake in the caffeine and withdrawal condition, respectively. Upon awakening, volunteers rated their subjective sleep quality by the Leeds Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire (LSEQ) (Parrott and Hindmarch, 1978).

Approval was obtained from the local ethics committee (Ethikkommission Nordwest- und Zentralschweiz), and the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All volunteers provided a written informed consent and received financial compensation for study participation.

Caffeine Levels

Salivary caffeine levels were repeatedly assessed in intervals of approximately 2 h throughout the in-lab protocol to check compliance with the treatment requirements. Here, we focus on the levels within 5 h prior to the sleep episode. After saliva collection, samples were immediately stored at 5 °C, later centrifuged at 3000 r/min for 10 min, and subsequently frozen at −24 °C until assayed by liquid chromatography coupled to tandem mass spectrometry. The data of one volunteer collected in the withdrawal condition were lost.

Subjective Sleep Quality

To assess subjective sleep quality, volunteers completed the LSEQ (Parrott and Hindmarch, 1978) right after the end of the 8-h nighttime sleep episode. Volunteers rated 10 items on visual analog scales consisting of four different scales, that is, getting to sleep, quality of sleep, awake following sleep, and behavior following wakening.

EEG Recordings

We utilized EEG recordings to assess sleep structure. Six EEG derivations (F3, F4, C3, C4, O1, and O2), two electrooculographic, two electromyographic, and two electrocardiographic electrodes were placed according to the international 10-20 system and referenced online against the linked mastoids (A1, and A2). The EEG signal was recorded using V-Amp devices (Brain Products, Gilching, Germany) with a sampling rate of 500 Hz and a filter applied online at 50 Hz.

Sleep staging was performed with an automatic scoring algorithm (ASEEGA, version 4.4.23, PHYSIP, Paris, France) which has been successfully used in previous studies (Reichert et al., 2017; Gaggioni et al., 2019) and has been shown to reach good agreement with manual sleep scoring (Berthomier et al., 2020). The concordance of a subset of manually scored nights (50 nights of this study sample) with the automatic sleep scoring reached >80%. The derivation (C4O2) was used for sleep autoscoring. Sleep latencies to specific stages were defined based on the first epoch scored in the respective sleep stage. While in this article we focus on REM sleep, we report all other sleep stages as well. All sleep stages are expressed as percentage of total sleep time (TST). Spectral analyses were performed by employing a fast Fourier transform with a Hanning window on consecutive epochs of 30 s. Artifacts were automatically rejected, and power spectra in the delta band (0.1-4 Hz) during non-REM (NREM) sleep are reported as a measure for sleep pressure.

Three data sets were excluded from analyses of all-night variables concerning the entire sleep episode due to technical issues (placebo condition: n = 2; caffeine condition: n = 1) and one volunteer was excluded from all analyses of REM sleep parameters (placebo condition) based on potential missed REM sleep episode by the algorithm and disagreement with visual scorers.

Statistical Analyses

Data analyses were performed using SAS (version 9.4, SAS Institute, Cary, USA). Values exceeding 3 times the interquartile range (IQR) from the first and third quartile were treated as extreme outliers and removed from subsequent analyses when attributed to errors in data collection or subsequent handling. Based on these criteria, we observed n = 2 outliers in the data (outlier regarding REM sleep latency and delta power). We applied mixed model analyses of variance with the factors condition (placebo, caffeine, and withdrawal) and time (levels differ per variable). The degrees of freedom were based on the approximation of Kenward and Roger reported in Kenward and Roger (1997). Contrasts were calculated by applying the LSMEANS statement and were adjusted for multiple comparisons based on Tukey-Kramer. To investigate whether the ratings of sleep quality can be explained by REM sleep characteristics, we performed general linear models with the statistics software SPSS (version 24, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA) including REM sleep variables as covariates and subject as a random factor. A value of p < 0.05 was used to determine statistical significance. The thresholds for the sleep stages and subjective sleep quality, however, were adjusted according to the Bonferroni method (to p < 0.0055 and <0.0125, respectively) due to multiple testing. One volunteer was excluded from all analyses due to noncompliance with the treatment requirements (caffeine condition: n = 1).

Results

Salivary Caffeine Levels

The analyses of caffeine levels assessed within 5 h prior to the sleep episode revealed a significant main effect of condition (F2,55.9 = 21.79; p < 0.0001). Post hoc comparisons indicated that caffeine levels were still elevated in the caffeine compared with the placebo and withdrawal conditions (pall < 0.0001), as presented in Figure 2. Caffeine levels in the withdrawal condition did not significantly differ from placebo (p = 0.984).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Depicted are the salivary caffeine levels collected within 5 h prior to the sleep episode in the placebo (black open circles), caffeine (blue filled circles), and withdrawal (red semi-filled circles) conditions (means ± standard errors). Overall, caffeine levels were increased in the caffeine condition compared with both placebo and withdrawal conditions (*p < 0.05).

Sleep

EEG Recordings

We analyzed TST, sleep latencies, and sleep architecture of the 8-h sleep episode, as summarized in Table 2. TST and the proportion of each sleep stage did not significantly differ among the three conditions (pall > 0.1). In the caffeine condition, however, it took participants longer to enter REM sleep compared with the placebo and withdrawal conditions (pall < 0.05).

Table 2.

Sleep variables and results in the electroencephalographic variables.

Variable Placebo Caffeine Withdrawal Condition
TST (min) 366.19 ± 16.71 393.89 ± 13.94 393.20 ± 11.23 F2,35.2 = 2.32, p = 0.113
SE (%) 76.79 ± 3.44 82.36 ± 2.79 82.46 ± 2.30 F2,35.3 = 2.27, p = 0.118
N1 (% of TST) 3.78 ± 0.52 4.49 ± 0.79 4.20 ± 0.48 F2,35.9 = 0.40, p = 0.676
N2 (% of TST) 44.93 ± 1.80 45.57 ± 1.39 44.69 ± 1.45 F2,34.6 = 0.06, p = 0.942
N3 (% of TST) 24.21 ± 1.25 25.22 ± 1.25 24.40 ± 1.31 F2,35.4 = 0.27, p = 0.763
REM (% of TST) 27.82 ± 1.31 24.73 ± 1.55 26.72 ± 1.11 F2,34.9 = 1.87, p = 0.169
SL2 11.13 ± 2.29 9.13 ± 0.89 8.13 ± 1.11 F2,37.3 = 1.62, p = 0.212
RL 53.63 ± 5.53 78.74 ± 10.21* 53.95 ± 6.42 F2,36.4 = 6.30, p = 0.005
NA 8.61 ± 1.30 9.22 ± 1.18 8.50 ± 1.27 F2,35.4 = 0.14, p = 0.871

Abbreviations: TST = total sleep time (sum of N1, N2, N3, and REM); SE = sleep efficiency (TST/time in bed); N1 = stage 1; N2 = stage 2; N3 = slow-wave sleep; REM = rapid eye movement; SL2 = time from lights-off to first epoch of N2; RL = time from lights-off to first epoch of REM sleep; NA = number of awakenings. Depicted are the means and standard errors per condition.

*

Significant post hoc comparisons compared with placebo and withdrawal conditions (p < 0.05, adjusted according to Tukey-Kramer) of significant main effects (p < 0.0055, as threshold was adjusted according to Bonferroni).

In a next step, we investigated whether the accumulation of REM sleep proportion across the sleep opportunity differs among the three conditions. As shown in Figure 3, on average, REM sleep proportion was reduced in the caffeine compared with the placebo condition (main effect of condition: F2,39.1 = 6.75, p = 0.003). This effect was modulated by time (interaction of condition x time: F14,195 = 1.77, p = 0.046) indicating that this caffeine-induced reduction in the caffeine condition was particularly present in bin 2, 3, 5, 6 and 7, that is, in the second and third hour of sleep as well as in the fifth, sixth and seventh hour of the given sleep window. To control whether the observed changes in REM sleep accumulation are mainly driven by the prolonged latency to REM sleep, we analyzed the amount of REM sleep per hour of sleep (Figure 4, for illustration). In this analysis, the amount of REM sleep did neither significantly differ among the three conditions (F2,411 = 0.23; p = 0.796) nor was it modulated by the hour spent asleep (interaction of condition x time: F14,398 = 1.44; p = 0.130) which underlines the impact of REM sleep latency on its accumulation.

Figure 3.

Figure 3.

Accumulation of REM sleep and SWS proportion across the sleep opportunity of 8 h. REM sleep (% of TST) and SWS (% of TST) were collapsed into bins of 1 h and accumulated across the sleep episode. Depicted are means and standard errors of the placebo (black open circles), caffeine (blue filled circles), and withdrawal conditions (red semi-filled circles). The color-coded asterisks represent significant (*pall < 0.05) differences between the placebo and caffeine conditions corrected for multiple comparisons according to Curran-Everett (2000). Abbreviations: REM = rapid eye movement; TST = total sleep time; SWS = slow-wave sleep.

Figure 4.

Figure 4.

Amount of REM sleep in each hour spent asleep. Depicted are means and standard errors of the placebo (black), caffeine (blue), and withdrawal conditions (red) across the sleep opportunity of 8 h. Abbreviation: REM = rapid eye movement.

Importantly, the analyses of the accumulation of wakefulness and SWS (see Fig. 3) did neither yield a significant main effect of condition (wakefulness: F2,36.4 = 0.49, p = 0.615; SWS: F2,38.6 = 0.94, p = 0.401) nor a significant interaction of the factors condition and time (wakefulness: F14,205 = 0.86, p = 0.603; SWS: F14,196 = 0.54, p = 0.908).

Finally, we analyzed the spectral power in the delta band (0.1-4 Hz) across the first three NREM sleep cycles as an indicator for sleep pressure. There were no significant differences between the three conditions in NREM sleep delta power (main effect of condition: F2,26.8 = 1.19; p > 0.3) nor was it modulated by the NREM sleep cycles (interaction condition × time: F4,45.1 = 0.99; p > 0.4).

Subjective Sleep Quality

The parameters and results of subjective sleep quality are presented in Table 3. While the domains getting to sleep and quality of sleep did not significantly differ between the three conditions (pall > 0.05), volunteers reported more difficulty in awake following sleep and behavior following wakening, that is, increased tiredness in the caffeine compared with the placebo condition (pall < 0.01). Neither REM sleep duration nor REM (% of TST) had a significant effect on the ratings of awake following sleep (pall > 0.4) and behavior following wakening (pall > 0.3).

Table 3.

Parameters and results of subjective sleep quality as assessed by the Leeds Sleep Evaluation Questionnaire.

Variable Placebo Caffeine Withdrawal Condition
GTS 73.65 ± 3.41 66.60 ± 4.49 68.22 ± 3.77 F2,37.5 = 1.20, p = 0.313
QOS 46.78 ± 4.66 41.03 ± 5.59 47.10 ± 4.28 F2,37.5 = 0.74, p = 0.485
AFS 75.50 ± 2.84 58.61 ± 4.74* 67.88 ± 3.49 F2,37.1 = 6.35, p = 0.004
BFW 74.75 ± 3.49 56.92 ± 5.74* 66.73 ± 3.17 F2,37.4 = 7.04, p = 0.003

Abbreviations: GTS = getting to sleep; QOS = quality of sleep; AFS = awake following sleep; BFW = behavior following wakening. Reported are the means and standard errors per condition. Lower values represent poorer subjective sleep quality.

*

Significant post hoc comparisons to placebo (p < 0.05, adjusted according to Tukey-Kramer) of significant main effects (p < 0.0125, as threshold was adjusted according to Bonferroni).

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the effects of regular daytime caffeine intake on REM sleep promotion and subjective quality of sleep. An 8-h sleep window scheduled in the early morning hours allowed to investigate REM sleep expression at its circadian maximum (Dijk and Landolt, 2019) in a highly controlled laboratory setting. The volunteers in the caffeine condition entered REM sleep later compared with both placebo and withdrawal conditions, and its accumulation across the night was delayed compared with placebo. As a similar pattern was not observed during regular nighttime sleep (Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Kistler, et al., 2020), that is, at another circadian time, we speculate that these findings may potentially indicate caffeine-induced differences in circadian REM sleep promotion. Thus, similar to the earlier reported REM sleep reductions after caffeine right before bedtime (Carrier et al., 2007, 2009; Robillard et al., 2015), specific features of REM sleep (such as onset and accumulation) might be sensitive to caffeine intake, even if it is restricted to daytime and the last intake is 13.5 h apart from lights-off. Moreover, volunteers reported more difficulties in awakening from sleep and feeling more tired after daily caffeine intake compared with continuous placebo intake. Such subtle changes in subjective sleep quality may promote the maintenance of regular caffeine intake under conditions of delayed or shifted sleep.

While daily caffeine intake is highly popular, the effects of regular daytime consumption on sleep-wake regulation are not well understood. One reason might be difficulties to standardize the history of prior caffeine consumption and to control its daily intake. Moreover, a potential adaptation to the continuous availability of caffeine (Bonnet and Arand, 1992; Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020) requires a design sophisticated enough to disentangle an inherent insensitivity from a habituation to the stimulant’s effect (James, 1998). In this report, for which we took advantage from an existing highly controlled laboratory data set, we suggest that regular daytime caffeine intake has the potential to alter REM sleep, indexed as prolonged onset and delayed accumulation of REM sleep. As a few studies suggest an adaptation of several nighttime sleep features (Bonnet and Arand, 1992) and of the circadian timing of melatonin onset (Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020) in response to regular caffeine intake, the present results indicate that the habituation of REM sleep promotion to caffeine might either follow another (potentially slower) time course or is even absent. Moreover, caffeine-induced REM sleep differences were statistically not detectable after withdrawal over 44.5 h, indicating that a potential caffeine-related change in REM sleep promotion seems to be reversible. As withdrawal symptoms commonly reach peak intensity between 20 and 51 h after acute caffeine cessation (Juliano and Griffiths, 2004), volunteers have presumably already overcome the acute phase of caffeine withdrawal at the time of sleep initiation.

Caffeine-induced reductions of REM sleep propensity have earlier been demonstrated after administration of caffeine right before daytime sleep (Carrier et al., 2007, 2009). However, the same outcome observed from 13.5 h after the last intake in this study suggests that caffeine concentration is not the only determinant to induce REM sleep changes. This notion receives further support as high dosages do not consistently evoke an effect on REM sleep ((Karacan et al., 1976; Nicholson and Stone, 1980; Robillard et al., 2015) vs. (Bonnet and Arand, 1992; Drake et al., 2013)). However, in line with the present data, caffeine-induced reductions of REM sleep features have consistently been observed when sleep was scheduled in the morning hours or at daytime (Carrier et al., 2007, 2009). Thus, caffeine effects on REM sleep seem to depend on circadian factors and may be particularly prominent if sleep is scheduled around the peak of REM sleep promotion. This might be comparable to the impact of caffeine on the homeostatic component of sleep-wake regulation, which seems to be particularly sensitive to the effects of caffeine under conditions of high sleep pressure (Roehrs and Roth, 2008; Snel and Lorist, 2011). Taken together, an effect of caffeine on sleep appears to be more easily detectable if the experimental design allows the sleep feature, that is, REM sleep, to reach a certain level.

Changes in sleep pressure have been shown to interact with the circadian expression of REM sleep (Dijk and Czeisler, 1995; Wyatt et al., 1999) and might thus have modulated the present effects as the sleep episode was scheduled 5 h after volunteer’s habitual bedtime. However, it is important to note that we scheduled a 1-h nap episode in the evening prior to the 8-h sleep episode. In this nap, participants slept for around 30 min, and sleep pressure as indexed by EEG SWA did not significantly differ between conditions (Weibel, Lin, Landolt, Garbazza, et al., 2020). Moreover, additional analyses revealed no difference in all-night SWA and SWA of the first sleep cycle during nocturnal sleep after 16 h of wakefulness and the present reported sleep episode after 21 h of wakefulness among the three conditions (statistics not presented). Thus, our results suggest that the potential occurrence of increased sleep pressure did not strongly affect the present findings which can therefore likely be traced back to the circadian component of sleep regulation.

Interestingly, volunteers in the caffeine condition indicated worse sleep quality indexed as more difficulties with awakening following sleep and feeling more tired compared with placebo. While SWS is commonly related with recuperation and subjectively perceived good sleep quality (Keklund and Akerstedt, 1997), the duration of REM sleep has been recently reported to be an important determinant for the perception of sleep quality (Della Monica et al., 2018). Thus, it is tempting to associate the changes of REM sleep with the perception of poorer sleep quality, although REM sleep did not account for the perceived sleep quality in the present sample. Moreover, the changes in sleep quality which were particularly evident with awakening might also be influenced by sleep inertia. Thus, the observed perception of nonrestorative sleep might promote daily caffeine consumption to compensate the lack of feeling refreshed upon wake-up and therefore act as a negative reinforcer.

Our results need to be interpreted taking the following limitations into careful consideration. First, based on the potential influence of the menstrual cycle on sleep-wake regulation (Shechter and Boivin, 2010) and the use of oral contraceptives on caffeine elimination (Abernethy and Todd, 1985; Balogh et al., 1995), female individuals were excluded which limits the generalizability of the present findings. Second, the effects of caffeine on sleep vary with age (Drapeau et al., 2006; Carrier et al., 2009; Robillard et al., 2015), and thus, the present results only refer to young healthy individuals. Third, previous studies reported that a specific variation in adenosine receptor gene modulates the effects of caffeine on sleep (Rétey et al., 2007) which was however not controlled in this study. Fourth, while we investigated REM sleep propensity at one circadian phase only, the optimal design would involve several circadian phases and a control for sleep pressure to allow conclusions on circadian regulation of sleep (such as in so-called forced desynchrony studies, see (Wyatt et al., 2004)).

In summary, we have first evidence that regular caffeine intake during daytime delays REM sleep promotion, as evident in prolonged REM sleep latency and delayed REM sleep accumulation. In the light of the evidence at present, these caffeine-induced changes in REM sleep promotion may particularly occur when sleep is delayed and centered around the morning hours such as at the weekend or during shift work. Caffeine-induced REM sleep changes may contribute to a worse quality of awakening and thus reinforce the maintenance of daytime caffeine intake.

Acknowledgments

We thank our interns Andrea Schumacher, Laura Tincknell, Sven Leach, and the study helpers for their help in data acquisition and all the volunteers for participating in this study. We are grateful for the help in study organization by Dr. Ruta Lasauskaite and the medical screenings by Dr. med. Corrado Garbazza and Dr. med. Helen Slawik. This work was funded by the Swiss National Science Foundation (320030_163058), the Mathieu-Stiftung, the Nikolaus und Bertha Burckhardt-Bürgin-Stiftung, and the Janggen-Pöhn-Stiftung.

Footnotes

Conflict Of Interest Statement: The author(s) have no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References

  1. Abernethy DR, Todd EL. (1985) Impairment of caffeine clearance by chronic use of low-dose oestrogen-containing oral contraceptives. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 28:425-428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Akerstedt T, Hume K, Minors D, Waterhouse J. (1994) The meaning of good sleep: a longitudinal study of polysomnography and subjective sleep quality. J Sleep Res 3:152-158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Balogh A, Klinger G, Henschel L, Borner A, Vollanth R, Kuhnz W. (1995) Influence of ethinylestradiol-containing combination oral contraceptives with gestodene or levonorgestrel on caffeine elimination. Eur J Clin Pharmacol 48:161-166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Basheer R, Porkka-Heiskanen T, Stenberg D, McCarley RW. (1999) Adenosine and behavioral state control: adenosine increases c-Fos protein and AP1 binding in basal forebrain of rats. Mol Brain Res 73:1-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berthomier C, Muto V, Schmidt C, Vandewalle G, Jaspar M, Devillers J, Gaggioni G, Chellappa SL, Meyer C, Phillips C, et al. (2020) Exploring scoring methods for research studies: accuracy and variability of visual and automated sleep scoring. J Sleep Res 29:e12994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bonnet MH, Arand DL. (1992) Caffeine use as a model of acute and chronic insomnia. Sleep 15:526-536. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Borbély AA. (1982) A two process model of sleep regulation. Hum Neurobiol 1:195-204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Brezinova V. (1974) Effect of caffeine on sleep: EEG study in late middle age people. Br J Clin Pharmacol 1:203-208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bühler E, Lachenmeier DW, Schlegel K, Winkler G. (2013) Development of a tool to assess the caffeine intake among teenagers and young adults. Ernahrungs Umschau 61:58-63. [Google Scholar]
  10. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, III, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. (1989) The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 28:193-213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Camandola S, Plick N, Mattson MP. (2019) Impact of coffee and cacao purine metabolites on neuroplasticity and neurodegenerative disease. Neurochem Res 44:214-227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carrier J, Fernandez-Bolanos M, Robillard R, Dumont M, Paquet J, Selmaoui B, Filipini D. (2007) Effects of caffeine are more marked on daytime recovery sleep than on nocturnal sleep. Neuropsychopharmacology 32:964-972. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carrier J, Paquet J, Fernandez-Bolanos M, Girouard L, Roy J, Selmaoui B, Filipini D. (2009) Effects of caffeine on daytime recovery sleep: a double challenge to the sleep-wake cycle in aging. Sleep Med 10:1016-1024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Curran-Everett D. (2000) Multiple comparisons: philosophies and illustrations. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 279:R1-R8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Della Monica C, Johnsen S, Atzori G, Groeger JA, Dijk DJ. (2018) Rapid eye movement sleep, sleep continuity and slow wave sleep as predictors of cognition, mood, and subjective sleep quality in healthy men and women, aged 20-84 years. Front Psychiatry 9:255. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. (1995) Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J Neurosci 15:3526-3538. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Dijk DJ, Landolt HP. (2019) Sleep physiology, circadian rhythms, waking performance and the development of sleep-wake therapeutics. Handb Exp Pharmacol 253:441-481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Drake C, Roehrs T, Shambroom J, Roth T. (2013) Caffeine effects on sleep taken 0, 3, or 6 hours before going to bed. J Clin Sleep Med 9:1195-1200. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Drapeau C, Hamel-Hébert I, Robillard R, Selmaoui B, Filipini D, Carrier J. (2006) Challenging sleep in aging: the effects of 200 mg of caffeine during the evening in young and middle-aged moderate caffeine consumers. J Sleep Res 15:133-141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Einöther SJL, Giesbrecht T. (2013) Caffeine as an attention enhancer: reviewing existing assumptions. Psychopharmacology 225:251-274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fredholm BB, Bättig K, Holmen J, Nehlig A, Zvartau EE. (1999) Actions of caffeine in the brain with special reference to factors that contribute to its widespread use. Pharmacol Rev 51:83-133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gaggioni G, Ly JQM, Muto V, Chellappa SL, Jaspar M, Meyer C, Delfosse T, Vanvinckenroye A, Dumont R, Coppieters ‘t, Wallant D, et al. (2019) Age-related decrease in cortical excitability circadian variations during sleep loss and its links with cognition. Neurobiol Aging 78:52-63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Heckman MA, Weil J, Gonzalez de, Mejia E. (2010) Caffeine (1, 3, 7-trimethylxanthine) in foods: a comprehensive review on consumption, functionality, safety, and regulatory matters. J Food Sci 75:R77-R87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Horne JA, Ostberg O. (1976) A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 4:97-110. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. James JE. (1998) Acute and chronic effects of caffeine on performance, mood, headache, and sleep. Neuropsychobiology 38:32-41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Juliano LM, Griffiths RR. (2004) A critical review of caffeine withdrawal: empirical validation of symptoms and signs, incidence, severity, and associated features. Psychopharmacology 176:1-29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Karacan I, Thornby JI, Anch M, Booth GH, Williams RL, Salis PJ. (1976) Dose-related sleep disturbances induced by coffee and caffeine. Clin Pharmacol Therapeut 20:682-689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Keane MA, James JE. (2008) Effects of dietary caffeine on EEG, performance and mood when rested and sleep restricted. Hum Psychopharmacol 23:669-680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Keklund G, Akerstedt T. (1997) Objective components of individual differences in subjective sleep quality. J Sleep Res 6:217-220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kenward MG, Roger JH. (1997) Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. Biometrics 53:983-997. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Landolt HP. (2008) Sleep homeostasis: a role for adenosine in humans? Biochem Pharmacol 75:2070-2079. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Landolt HP, Dijk DJ, Gaus SE, Borbély AA. (1995) Caffeine reduces low-frequency delta-activity in the human sleep EEG. Neuropsychopharmacology 12:229-238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Landolt HP, Rétey JV, Tonz K, Gottselig JM, Khatami R, Buckelmuller I, Achermann P. (2004) Caffeine attenuates waking and sleep electroencephalographic markers of sleep homeostasis in humans. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:1933-1939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Landolt HP, Werth E, Borbély AA, Dijk DJ. (1995) Caffeine intake (200 mg) in the morning affects human sleep and EEG power spectra at night. Brain Res 675:67-74. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Lazar AS, Lazar ZI, Dijk DJ. (2015) Circadian regulation of slow waves in human sleep: topographical aspects. Neuroimage 116:123-134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Lieberman HR, Agarwal S, Fulgoni VL., III (2019) Daily patterns of caffeine intake and the association of intake with multiple sociodemographic and lifestyle factors in US adults based on the NHANES 2007-2012 surveys. J Acad Nutr Diet 119:106-114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Martyn D, Lau A, Richardson P, Roberts A. (2018) Temporal patterns of caffeine intake in the United States. Food Chem Toxicol 111:71-83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Münch M, Knoblauch V, Blatter K, Schroder C, Schnitzler C, Kräuchi K, Wirz-Justice A, Cajochen C. (2005) Age-related attenuation of the evening circadian arousal signal in humans. Neurobiol Aging 26:1307-1319. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nicholson AN, Stone BM. (1980) Heterocyclic amphetamine derivatives and caffeine on sleep in man. Br J Clin Pharmacol 9:195-203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Parrott AC, Hindmarch I. (1978) Factor analysis of a sleep evaluation questionnaire. Psychol Med 8:325-329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Portas CM, Thakkar M, Rainnie DG, Greene RW, McCarley RW. (1997) Role of adenosine in behavioral state modulation: a microdialysis study in the freely moving cat. Neuroscience 79:225-235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Reichert CF, Maire M, Gabel V, Viola AU, Götz T, Scheffler K, Klarhofer M, Berthomier C, Strobel W, Phillips C, et al. (2017) Cognitive brain responses during circadian wake-promotion: evidence for sleep-pressure-dependent hypothalamic activations. Sci Rep 7:5620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Rétey JV, Adam M, Khatami R, Luhmann UF, Jung HH, Berger W, Landolt HP. (2007) A genetic variation in the adenosine A2A receptor gene (ADORA2A) contributes to individual sensitivity to caffeine effects on sleep. Clin Pharmacol Ther 81:692-698. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Robillard R, Bouchard M, Cartier A, Nicolau L, Carrier J. (2015) Sleep is more sensitive to high doses of caffeine in the middle years of life. J Psychopharmacol 29:688-697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Roehrs T, Roth T. (2008) Caffeine: sleep and daytime sleepiness. Sleep Med Rev 12:153-162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shechter A, Boivin DB. (2010) Sleep, hormones, and circadian rhythms throughout the menstrual cycle in healthy women and women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Int J Endocrinol 2010:259345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sigmon SC, Herning RI, Better W, Cadet JL, Griffiths RR. (2009) Caffeine withdrawal, acute effects, tolerance, and absence of net beneficial effects of chronic administration: cerebral blood flow velocity, quantitative EEG, and subjective effects. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 204:573-585. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Smith A. (2002) Effects of caffeine on human behavior. Food Chem Toxicol 40:1243-1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Snel J, Lorist MM. (2011) Effects of caffeine on sleep and cognition. Prog Brain Res 190: 105-117. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Weibel J, Lin Y-S, Landolt H-P, Garbazza C, Kolodyazhniy V, Kistler J, Rehm S, Rentsch K, Borgwardt S, Cajochen C, et al. (2020) Caffeine-dependent changes of sleep-wake regulation: evidence for adaptation after repeated intake. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 99:109851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Weibel J, Lin Y-S, Landolt H-P, Kistler J, Rehm S, Rentsch K, Slawik H, Borgwardt S, Cajochen C, Reichert C. (2020) The impact of daily caffeine intake on nighttime sleep: signs of overnight withdrawal? Biorxiv. doi: 10.1101/2020.05.26.114769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Wyatt JK, Cajochen C, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. (2004) Low-dose repeated caffeine administration for circadian-phase-dependent performance degradation during extended wakefulness. Sleep 27:374-381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Wyatt JK, Ritz-De Cecco A, Czeisler CA, Dijk DJ. (1999) Circadian temperature and melatonin rhythms, sleep, and neurobehavioral function in humans living on a 20-h day. Am J Physiol 277:R1152-R1163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Biological Rhythms are provided here courtesy of SAGE Publications

RESOURCES