Skip to main content
Cureus logoLink to Cureus
. 2021 Aug 9;13(8):e17034. doi: 10.7759/cureus.17034

The Relationship Between Four Measures of Religiosity and Cross-National Variations in the Burden of Dementia

Ravi P Rajkumar 1,
Editors: Alexander Muacevic, John R Adler
PMCID: PMC8357016  PMID: 34395146

Abstract

Background

Several researchers have identified a possible protective effect of religiosity on the risk of dementia. Specific aspects of religiosity may be associated with this attenuation of risk, and it may be partially mediated through an effect on depressive symptoms or social support. However, this effect has only been demonstrated in selected cohorts to date.

Methods

This study was based on a cross-national analysis of associations. Correlations between World Health Organization estimates of the burden of dementia and four survey-derived measures of religiosity were examined across 101 countries, while controlling for estimates of late-life depression and social capital.

Results

Specific aspects of religiosity, such as attendance at religious services (Pearson’s r= -0.57), daily prayer (r = -0.58), and perception of religion as very important (r = -0.65), were associated with lower national levels of Alzheimer’s and other dementias (p< 0.01 for all correlations). This effect was partially mediated through an inverse relationship between religiosity and depression, but remained significant even after controlling for it and on multivariate analyses (β = -0.38 to -0.57, p< 0.01 for all measures). There was no evidence for a mediating effect of social capital.

Conclusions

Specific religious beliefs and practices may have a protective effect on dementia risk at the population level. These may involve group effects that require further study, such as reductions in depression in the elderly, or may involve beneficial effects on the stress response and cellular ageing in vulnerable individuals; however, the latter cannot be inferred with certainty from a group-level analysis. These results are consistent with earlier research and suggest a potential role for religious-based preventive strategies at the population level.

Keywords: dementia, alzheimer’s disease, religion, spirituality, depression, resilience, brain awareness, telomere length, social capital

Introduction

Dementia is a term used to refer to a group of generally irreversible conditions characterized by marked impairments in several domains of cognition. Alzheimer’s disease (AD), a neurodegenerative disorder with a distinctive pathophysiology characterized by the accumulation of amyloid neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles, is the leading cause of dementia worldwide [1]. A significant proportion of the risk for dementia, and particularly for AD, is determined by genetic factors [2]. However, the development and subsequent progression of dementia is influenced by several environmental factors, some of which, such as diet, exposure to environmental pollutants, and comorbid conditions such as depression or diabetes mellitus, are amenable to modification [3,4]. Likewise, certain factors have been identified as potentially protective against the development of dementia, including specific dietary patterns, levels of specific nutrients such as folate, and involvement in physical or cognitive activity [5,6]. The protective effect of religious belief and practice against cognitive deterioration, and specifically against dementia, has attracted a certain amount of research interest in the past decade [7]. For example, affiliation with specific religious groups has been associated with a reduced risk of AD [8], while engagement in specific religious behaviors, such as daily prayer, has been associated with a reduced risk of cognitive decline at a later age [9]. Similarly, studies of patients with AD have found that religious activity is associated with better functioning in cognitive domains such as memory, language and constructional abilities, as well as with a slower rate of cognitive decline [10,11].

Two significant questions arise from an analysis of this research. First, what are the mechanisms that mediate the protective effect of religiosity on cognitive decline and dementia? Though no research has addressed this specific question to date, it has been observed that religious involvement in adults is associated with lower levels of depressive symptoms, which are positively correlated with telomere length [12]. This cellular process is of particular interest because shorter telomere length is significantly associated with an increase in the risk of certain types of dementia, such as AD [13]. Moreover, depression, particularly in late life, is a well-established risk factor for the development and progression of dementia [3,14,15]. Thus, the protective effect of religiosity on dementia could be mediated through its mitigating effect on depressive symptoms, as a relationship has been observed between the severity of these symptoms and the risk of AD [16]. Second, which are the aspects of religiosity that may exert a protective effect against cognitive decline in general and dementia in particular? This question is of significance because religiosity cannot be considered as a unitary construct: specific factors such as commitment to religious beliefs, the practice of prayer or meditation (“contemplation”), and engagement in altruistic behavior may have distinct effects on health. [17] Research in older adults has shown that religious attendance is negatively correlated with depressive symptoms, while private religious practice had no or even opposite effects on these symptoms, and the effects of religious contemplation on depression vary depending on whether an individual has a high or low genetic risk for this disorder [17-19]. Likewise, a study of elderly men from Israel found that religious education and observance was associated with a paradoxical increase in the risk of AD, in contrast with the majority of studies reporting a protective effect of religious practice against dementia [20]. Another important indirect mechanism that needs to be considered in such analyses is the relationship between religion and social support, particularly at the community level. Though religious and social capital cannot be directly equated with each other, there is evidence for a significant link between the two [21]. Though religiosity can be associated with a positive effect on health via increased social capital, a paradoxical effect can also be observed in some individuals and groups, with religious norms or practices being viewed as a constraint, leading to reductions in well-being [22].

Therefore, any analysis of the relationship between religiosity and AD should take into account both the different aspects of religious belief and practice, and the moderating effects of depression and social cohesion. The current study aimed to address these factors by examining the correlations between four different aspects of religiosity and the disease burden due to dementia across countries, while controlling for the effects of late-life depression and social capital.

Materials and methods

Data sources

The current study was a cross-national association study. There are no large-scale estimates of the prevalence of dementia across nations. However, disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for a given disorder provide a reasonable approximation to disease prevalence, when adjusted for population size and life expectancy, as DALYs are calculated based on either estimated incidence or estimated prevalence [23]. Information on DALYs for Alzheimer’s and other dementias, provided as an aggregate, were obtained from the World Health Organization’s Global Health Estimates for the year 2017, which provided estimated total DALYs for dementia for 183 countries and regions of the world. Raw values for DALYs were extracted from this source. As these values were computed for each country as a whole, they were divided by the estimated population for each country to provide adjusted DALYs for Alzheimer’s and other dementias (DALY-Dem), which were used as the independent variable in this study. Information on population sizes at the time the estimates were calculated was provided in the WHO dataset [24]. These DALYs were calculated based on the estimated prevalence of each particular disorder. Complete technical details of the calculation process are available in the WHO’s technical report on this data set [25].

To obtain information on various aspects of religiosity, data was obtained from the Pew Research Center’s 2018 publication, entitled “The Age Gap In Religion Around The World”, This report, based on surveys of individuals from a total of 105 countries and regions, obtained information on four aspects of religiosity:

1. religious affiliation;

2. attendance at religious services at least once a week, in those countries where weekly religious attendance is the norm in the majority religion (Christianity, Islam or Judaism);

3. daily participation in prayer;

4. respondents’ perception of religion as being “very important” in their lives.

All these responses were coded as dichotomous (yes/no) variables, and data for each country was provided as the percentage of positive responses. For example, among respondents in the United States of America, 77% reported affiliation to a particular religion, 36% reported weekly attendance at religious services, 55% reported praying at least once a day or more often, and 53% considered religion to be a very important aspect of their lives. In countries where weekly religious attendance is not the norm, such as India and Japan, the second of these variables were omitted [26]. This data set was selected because of its coverage of a large number of countries, and because it covered several distinct aspects of religious belief and practice, such as affiliation and prayer, which were identified as potentially important in earlier research. As this data was available for 101 of the 183 countries in the WHO data set, and as religiosity was the primary independent variable of interest, these 101 countries were selected for analysis in this study.

To analyze the possibility of a mediating effect of late-life depression on the link between religiosity and dementia, DALYs for depression for all adults aged 60 and above were obtained from the WHO Global Health Estimates [24] and adjusted for population size (DALY-Dep). This variable provides an indirect estimate of the prevalence of late-life depression [25]. Likewise, to assess the potential confounding effect of social cohesion, scores on the Social Capital pillar of the Legatum Prosperity Index were obtained from the World Bank’s database. This variable provides a composite measure of family and community networks, social cohesiveness, and level of trust in the government and other institutions. Scores on this index range from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating greater social capital [27].

A complete list of all the countries included in this study, with data on measures of dementia, depression, religiosity and social capital, is included in Table 1.

Table 1. Raw data used for bivariate and multivariate analyses.

DALY-DEM: population-adjusted DALY for Alzheimer’s and other dementias; DALY-DEP: population-adjusted DALY for depression in adults aged 60 and above; Rel-Affil: religious affiliation; Rel-Attend: weekly attendance at religious services; Rel-Pray: daily prayer; Rel-Important: religion considered “very important”; N/A: data not available for the respective country.

Country Rel-Affil Rel-Attend Rel-Pray Rel-Important Life Expectancy Social Capital DALY-DEM DALY-DEP
Afghanistan 100 61 96 92 65 N/A 19.5371 0.405319
Albania 99 7 15 15 79 46.53 68.5257 0.737851
Algeria 99 48 88 73 77 42.27 57.3391 0.653058
Argentina 89 20 40 43 77 51.97 25.9373 0.67883
Armenia 98 10 45 53 75 41.51 64.4103 0.946976
Australia 57 17 18 18 83 67.6 88.4513 0.866774
Austria 84 11 8 12 82 61.77 70.9282 0.726522
Azerbaijan 100 2 76 38 73 42.93 31.8203 0.670179
Bangladesh 100 54 57 80 73 44.19 21.5703 0.748953
Belarus 97 16 25 21 74 N/A 79.0284 1.25488
Belgium 62 6 11 11 82 58.47 124.754 0.953756
Bolivia 96 42 56 71 72 49.7 38.4598 0.782262
Brazil 92 45 61 72 76 52.93 37.1312 0.877154
Bulgaria 95 9 15 19 75 46.16 33.3193 1.16751
BurkinaFaso 100 N/A N/A 93 62 48.9 16.1152 0.609112
Cameroon 98 70 82 90 59 48.87 21.9322 0.78796
Canada 67 20 25 27 82 66.23 98.5658 0.650306
Chad 97 77 83 86 54 43.07 18.3353 0.772152
Chile 84 19 39 41 80 51.15 39.5101 0.766578
China 13 1 1 3 77 41.55 83.1589 1.03326
Colombia 94 50 73 77 77 51.09 18.6425 0.646877
CostaRica 91 52 78 76 80 54.97 32.1958 0.800518
Croatia 93 24 41 42 78 45.61 63.8135 1.14022
Czechia 28 7 9 7 79 48.93 62.7006 1.06864
Dem Rep Congo 96 78 69 88 61 N/A 23.4234 0.634666
Denmark 70 3 10 9 81 64.49 106.015 0.80037
Djibouti 100 87 87 89 67 N/A 27.0938 0.843594
Dominican Rep 82 48 74 78 74 54.72 39.7449 0.761234
Ecuador 95 38 63 76 77 49.66 14.6951 0.722411
Egypt 100 62 72 72 72 42.73 42.0503 0.645085
ElSalvador 88 61 77 85 73 48.15 65.87 0.755188
Estonia 55 2 9 6 78 49.73 46.6112 1.22095
Ethiopia 100 82 65 98 67 44.49 18.509 0.754114
Finland 78 4 18 10 82 62.81 230.562 1.02238
France 72 12 10 11 83 53.71 113.492 1.02203
Georgia 100 17 38 51 74 42.04 101.016 0.956398
Germany 76 10 9 10 81 63.21 104.621 0.87386
Ghana 99 84 76 89 64 48.09 21.9859 0.687885
Greece 96 16 30 56 82 47.27 67.7143 0.955406
Guatemala 94 75 82 89 74 55.2 13.4222 0.661368
Guinea-Bissau 100 81 83 91 58 N/A 15.3805 0.909283
Honduras 90 64 78 90 75 48.41 28.9696 0.546374
Hungary 79 9 16 14 76 45.58 80.2587 1.1919
India 100 N/A 75 80 70 48.37 26.0625 0.918895
Indonesia 100 72 84 93 72 61.88 50.0402 0.362357
Iran 100 38 87 78 77 49.67 51.4841 0.837567
Iraq 100 42 87 82 71 N/A 23.8168 0.454869
Ireland 85 20 19 22 82 63.09 82.2126 0.794026
Israel 97 30 27 36 83 54.27 51.7549 0.734828
Italy 85 23 21 21 83 53.01 121.644 0.873735
Japan 44 N/A 33 10 84 46.98 94.5045 0.925204
Jordan 100 64 76 85 75 50.58 38.9594 0.274095
Kazakhstan 95 22 20 22 73 46.41 35.366 0.885384
Kenya 100 81 79 87 67 61.48 12.3624 0.627162
Kyrgyzstan 98 21 24 47 72 54.23 8.3949 0.657594
Latvia 79 6 18 11 75 47.81 51.7504 1.1527
Lebanon 100 35 51 57 79 43.64 107.533 0.724917
Liberia 100 79 80 90 64 47.7 16.1533 0.79646
Lithuania 94 9 15 16 76 44.09 47.2381 1.23342
Malaysia 99 45 61 77 76 56.76 50.0082 0.697277
Mali 100 79 81 94 59 47.54 20.2599 0.459382
Mexico 93 45 40 45 75 44.05 19.0905 0.640608
Moldova 98 15 49 42 72 44.18 33.867 1.15369
Morocco 100 55 80 91 77 39.76 59.2417 1.02361
Mozambique 87 84 68 87 61 48.21 19.7035 0.822104
Netherlands 51 12 20 20 82 62.07 135.879 0.91574
Nicaragua 93 55 75 88 74 52.55 45.4845 0.595798
Niger 99 88 87 86 62 N/A 14.8862 0.616876
Nigeria 100 89 95 88 55 51.52 25.6729 0.881544
Norway 57 7 18 19 83 65.06 106.061 0.804389
Pakistan 100 59 67 94 67 42.3 20.8512 0.677379
Panama 93 48 69 61 79 57.7 22.8752 0.681703
Paraguay 99 32 82 56 74 49.36 50.2763 0.626023
Peru 96 36 51 73 77 50.34 45.4304 0.522839
Philippines 100 53 82 91 71 59.57 11.7412 0.369085
Poland 93 42 29 30 78 48.36 33.1145 0.901472
Portugal 85 25 38 36 81 54.87 107.662 1.07379
Romania 99 24 45 50 75 47.58 40.5299 1.04536
Russia 85 7 18 16 73 45.37 70.1112 1.19845
Rwanda 99 80 62 90 69 47.39 17.2921 0.699888
Senegal 100 69 88 98 68 49.28 17.7764 0.576252
Serbia 96 7 27 34 76 N/A 57.3308 1.13403
Slovakia 75 23 31 23 77 48.28 77.9364 0.958891
South  Africa 93 55 52 75 64 55.81 42.0005 0.812857
South Korea 54 29 32 16 83 N/A 58.2176 0.957509
Spain 70 15 23 22 83 57 133.051 0.843629
Sweden 58 6 11 10 83 61.31 140.86 1.02645
Switzerland 79 11 8 9 84 61.64 123.312 0.820514
Tajikistan 100 31 48 50 71 48.8 20.1714 0.333199
Tanzania 99 82 56 93 65 50.12 18.8612 0.668812
Tunisia 100 47 67 78 77 42.38 95.2244 0.906417
Turkey 99 44 60 68 78 46.82 105.761 0.742656
Uganda 100 82 66 86 63 53.8 13.7045 0.793234
Ukraine 93 17 30 23 72 42.49 85.1167 1.33946
United Kingdom 77 8 6 10 81 62.22 168.417 0.918095
United States 77 36 55 53 79 65.45 127.902 0.912895
Uruguay 63 14 29 29 78 57.57 76.387 0.769681
Uzbekistan 99 9 26 29 72 N/A 9.02312 0.582761
Venezuela 93 26 47 67 72 43.61 15.3241 0.755696
Vietnam 36 N/A 14 18 75 51.54 63.9787 0.574915
Zambia 99 86 78 91 64 47.98 13.7311 0.59566

Data analysis

In order to minimize the possible confounding effect of life expectancy on DALY estimates, particularly in low- and middle-income countries, DALY-Dem and DALY-Dep values were standardized in a linear manner for an ideal maximum life expectancy of 90 years. For this purpose, data on national life expectancies were obtained from the World Bank’s global database [28]. All study variables were tested for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test; as they did not conform to a Gaussian distribution (p < 0.05 for all variables), they were transformed using a logarithmic transformation. As transformation using natural (base e) logarithms still yielded significant deviations from normality, the base 10 logarithm was used to transform each variable included in this study.

Bivariate analyses were carried out using Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) to examine potential relationships between DALY-Dem and all four aspects of religiosity, as well as between these variables, DALY-Dep and social capital. All tests were two-tailed, with the threshold for significance set at p < 0.05 after applying Bonferroni’s correction for a 7 x 7 table. To identify potential multicollinearity between study variables, a threshold value of r ≥ 0.8 was used. In the event of multicollinearity between two or more variables, the variable showing the strongest individual correlation with DALY-Dem was included in the multivariate analysis. The strengths of observed correlations were graded according to standard guidelines as follows: poor (r < 0.3), fair (0.3 < r < 0.6), moderate (0.6 < r < 0.8) and very strong (r > 0.8) [29].

In order to test whether the relationship between dementia and religiosity was primarily mediated through variations in depression or social capital, a partial correlation analysis was undertaken between DALY-Dem and all four indices of religiosity, controlling for DALY-Dep and social capital, both individually and in combination. In these analyses, the significance level was set at p < 0.05 after applying Bonferroni’s correction for a 5 x 5 table.

Variables identified as significantly associated with DALY-Dem in bivariate analyses were included in a multivariate linear regression analysis, to identify the relative contributions of each variable to variations in DALY-Dem and the significance of each association. To assess for multicollinearity in this analysis, the variance inflation factor (VIF) was computed for each independent variable, and the data was re-analyzed after excluding any variable with a VIF greater than or equal to 4.

Results

Data on a total of 101 countries were included in the final analysis. The results of unadjusted bivariate analyses are presented in Table 2. It was observed that in these unadjusted analyses, all four indices of religiosity were negatively correlated with DALY-Dem, indicating a possible direct or indirect protective effect. All these associations remained significant after corrections for multiple comparisons. The magnitude of these correlations ranged from “fair” to “moderate”. Among these four indices, the strongest negative correlation was observed for the perception of religion as very important (r = -0.65, p < 0.01) and the weakest was observed for religious affiliation (r = -0.43, p < 0.01). There was a modest positive correlation between DALY-Dep and DALY-Dem (r = 0.47, p < 0.01). DALY-Dep was significantly correlated with three indices of religiosity - religious service attendance, daily prayer and perception of religion as very important - but not with religious affiliation, and the strength of these associations was “fair”. It was also noted that there was significant multicollinearity between religious service attendance, daily prayer and perception of religion as very important (r = 0.84-0.92 for correlations between these variables); though religious affiliation was positively correlated with these variables (r = 0.59-0.70), this did not reach the threshold of concern for multicollinearity. Social capital showed a trend towards a positive correlation with DALY-Dem, but this was not significant after correction for multiple comparisons (r = 0.29, p = 0.235). There were no significant correlations observed between social capital and indices of religiosity.

Table 2. Bivariate correlations between country-wise DALYs for dementia, measures of religiosity, DALYs for depression in adults aged over 60, and social capital.

DALY-DEM: disability-adjusted life years for dementia; DALY-DEP: disability-adjusted life years for depression in those aged 60 or above. *denotes a significant correlation at p < 0.05 after applying Bonferroni’s correction.

Variable DALY-DEM Religious affiliation Religious service attendance Daily prayer Religion considered “very important” DALY-DEP Social capital
DALY-DEM - -0.43* -0.57* -0.58* -0.65* 0.47* 0.29
Religious affiliation   - 0.59* 0.70* 0.70* -0.27 -0.14
Religious service attendance     - 0.84* 0.89* -0.49* -0.18
Daily prayer       - 0.94* -0.47* -0.13
Religion considered “very important”         - -0.52* -0.25

The results of partial correlation analyses are presented in Table 3. Even after controlling for depression above the age of 60, significant negative correlations were observed between DALY-Dem and all four indices of religiosity, though the magnitude of all these correlations was reduced and remained in the “fair” range (r = -0.36 to -0.54). These associations remained significant after correction for multiple comparisons, and the pattern observed was similar to that seen in the uncorrected analyses, with the strongest correlation reported for perception of religion as very important, and the weakest for religious affiliation. A similar pattern emerged when controlling for social capital, and when controlling for both depression and social capital.

Table 3. Partial correlations between country-wise DALYs for dementia and measures of religiosity, conditioned on DALYs for depression in adults aged over 60 and social capital.

DALY-DEM: disability-adjusted life years for dementia; SC: social capital. All correlations are partial Pearson’s correlation tests. *denotes a significant correlation at p < 0.05 after applying Bonferroni’s correction.

Variable Religious affiliation Religious service attendance Daily prayer Religion considered “very important”
DALY-DEM, conditioned on DALY-DEP -0.36* -0.44* -0.46* -0.54*
DALY-DEM, conditioned on SC -0.39* -0.58* -0.54* -0.62*
DALY-DEM, conditioned on both -0.28* -0.42* -0.38* -0.47*

In view of significant multicollinearity between religious service attendance, daily prayer and perception of religion as very important, the latter was selected for inclusion in multivariate analyses as it showed the largest magnitude of correlation with DALY-Dem in both direct and partial correlation analyses. For the purposes of multivariate linear regression analysis, DALY-Dem was selected as the dependent variable and DALY-Dep, religious affiliation and perception of religion as very important were entered as independent variables. Social capital was not entered as an independent variable in this analysis as it did not attain the threshold for significance in bivariate analyses. The results of this analysis are presented in Table 4. The final model explained approximately 43% of the variation in DALY-Dem (R2 = 0.45; adjusted R2 = 0.43). In this model, both DALY-Dep (β = 0.18, p = 0.048) and perception of religion as very important (β = -0.57, p < 0.001) were significantly associated with DALY-Dep, while the association with religious affiliation was not significant. Variance inflation factors ranged from 1.4 to 2.5 for each variable, ruling out significant multicollinearity between these variables.

Table 4. Multivariate linear regression analysis of variables associated with disability-adjusted life years for dementia.

DALY-Dep: disability-adjusted life years for depression above the age of 60. *denotes significance at p < 0.05.

Variable Regression coefficient (β) Significance level (p) Part correlation Variance inflation factor (VIF)
DALY-Dep 0.18 0.048* 0.15 1.4
Religious affiliation 0.01 0.890 0.01 2.0
Perception of religion as very important -0.57 <0.001* -0.36 2.5

As a secondary measure, similar multivariate linear regression analyses were carried out including DALY-Dep and religious affiliation as independent variables, but using each of the other indices of religiosity (weekly religious attendance and daily prayer) in turn. Each of these models explained around 37% of the variance in DALY-Dem (adjusted R2 = 0.37 for both models). In the model including weekly religious attendance, DALY-Dep (β = 0.25, p = 0.008) and religious attendance (β = -0.38, p < 0.001), but not religious affiliation, were significantly associated with DALY-Dem. In the model including daily prayer, DALY-Dep (β = 0.25, p = 0.006) and daily prayer (β = -0.41, p = 0.001) were associated with DALY-Dem, while affiliation was not significantly associated with dementia. VIFs for all variables in these analyses were in the range of 1.3-2.4, indicating no significant concerns regarding multicollinearity.

Discussion

The results of this preliminary analysis suggest that various aspects of religiosity may exert a protective effect against dementia, and may account for a modest proportion of the variations in the burden of dementia across countries. In general, this effect was stronger for measures of religious practice and belief, such as attendance at religious rituals, daily prayer and perception of the importance of religion, and somewhat weaker for affiliation with a given religion. These associations remained significant when controlling for a measure of late-life depression, as well as in multivariate analyses.

The associations between different aspects of religiosity and dementia observed in this study are consistent with prior research, which has identified a potentially protective effect of specific religious behaviors or practices on the onset and progression of dementia in general and AD in particular [9-11]. In other words, practices such as daily prayer or meditation, rather than membership status in a particular religious denomination, may have a greater and more significant protective effect. It was also observed that the largest potential protective effect against dementia in this dataset was observed for individual perceptions of religion as very important, indicating that the subjective aspects of religiosity are of equal if not greater importance than engagement in specific rituals. The high level of multicollinearity between variables measuring the aspects of religiosity did not permit a more specific examination of the relative contributions of each variable. Moreover, as the data analyzed was at the population level, it was not possible to capture inter-individual or intra-national variations in religiosity (for example, between economically developed or deprived regions, or between urban and rural areas) and their impact on dementia.

These results also suggest that religiosity is significantly and negatively correlated with depression at a population level. This suggests that countries or regions characterized by a higher level of religious practice may have lower levels of depression in the elderly, and this may in turn exert a positive influence on the subsequent risk of dementia at a group level. This finding is in line with the results of research in individual subjects [8,30]. However, given that the correlations between indices of religiosity and DALYs due to dementia remained significant in partial correlation and multivariate analyses, it is likely that other mechanisms may be involved in mediating this association. These may include behavioral factors, such as greater involvement in prosocial and altruistic behaviors related to religious belief, which has been shown to positively influence cognition in older adults [31]. Alternately, religious participation has been shown to exert a beneficial effect on diurnal variations in cortisol secretion [32]. Elevations in morning cortisol have been found to be associated with both mild cognitive impairment and AD, and it is possible that some aspects of religious belief or practice may attenuate such elevations and thus reduce the risk of cognitive deterioration, independent of their effects on depressive symptoms [33]. In this connection, it has also been observed that increased cortisol reactivity is associated with telomere shortening, which is a potential risk factor for some forms of dementia [13,34]. A further possibility that deserves consideration is that religiosity was associated with greater telomere length in individuals with a genetic risk of dementia, suggesting that religiosity may attenuate the risk of dementia associated with specific allelic variations [35]. On the basis of these studies, is plausible that protective effects on telomere length, or on related mechanisms of cellular ageing, may represent a “final common pathway” through which specific aspects of religious practice and behaviour could attenuate the risk of cognitive decline and dementia in the elderly [36]. These explanations should be regarded as speculative, as they are based on research in individuals, while the data in this study was derived on entire populations.

Given the difficulties in inferring causality from studies of association or correlation, indirect mechanisms mediating this association should also be considered. For example, specific genetic variants in certain populations may be associated a reduced risk of dementia independent of religious practice, or there may be an additive effect between the presence of “protective” genetic polymorphisms and specific religious activities, such as meditation [37]. The protective effects of religiosity may also be greater for specific religious groups or sub-groups, and may be critically influenced by factors such as gender and physical fitness [8]. Finally, engagement with religion may have indirect beneficial effects such as greater social support [38] or adherence to particular dietary patterns [39] both of which are independently associated with a reduced risk of cognitive impairment [40,41]. Data from the countries analyzed in this study suggests that the link between religiosity and dementia may not be mediated by social capital, though other indirect mechanisms at the population level still require investigation.

These results are subject to certain important limitations. First and foremost, this study was based on data from entire countries; therefore, while it is possible to extrapolate these findings to an individual level to a limited extent, this should be done cautiously to avoid the ecological fallacy. Second, data on the country-level burden of both dementia and depression was obtained from WHO estimates. As noted by the WHO researchers, these estimates are associated with a certain margin of error, particularly in low- and middle-income countries with lower levels of health infrastructure. Third, the WHO data provided aggregate DALYs for “Alzheimer’s and other dementias”; therefore, it is not possible to conclude whether the results of this study are valid for dementia in general, or for AD in particular. Fourth, information on religiosity was available only as aggregate responses or percentages, allowing for only a rough estimation of the different facets of religious belief and behavior, particularly in multi-cultural or multi-religious societies. Fifth, as religious affiliation was expressed as a simple dichotomous variable in the Pew Research dataset, it was not possible to assess the differential effects of specific religions, such as Islam, Hinduism, Buddhism, Judaism and Christianity, on dementia. This is significant because “protective” effects have been specifically reported for Christian and Buddhist religious practices, but an inverse relationship was noted in a sample of Jewish men [8,20,37]. Sixth, given the association-based design of this study, it is not possible to draw definite conclusions about a protective effect, or whether such an effect is direct or indirect. Seventh, because countries were chosen as the unit of analysis, it was not possible to examine the effects of individual-level variables, such as gender, on the associations reported here. Eighth, due to a lack of large-scale data, the analyses were not corrected for other factors, such as environmental pollution, which vary significantly across countries and are independently associated with the risk of dementia. Finally, the observed correlations were fair to moderate in size, indicating that religiosity is only one of a multitude of factors influencing the risk of dementia.

Conclusions

Despite these limitations, these results are of significance as they corroborate the findings of research in individual subjects, and support the contention that certain aspects of religiosity may have specific protective effects against cognitive decline and dementia in the elderly at the population level. They also provide some confirmation of the hypothesis that a “protective” relationship of religiosity on dementia risk is partially mediated by the severity of depressive symptoms; however, no consistent evidence for a mediation through social capital was found in this study. Based on these results, it is possible that further studies of specific aspects of religiosity may lead to a better delineation of this putative protective effect, and whether it is direct or mediated through correlates of religiosity, such as healthy dietary practices. Analyzing the interaction of measures of religiosity with genetic and environmental risk factors at the individual level would enable a better understanding of the cellular mechanisms through which such protective effects are mediated, and whether they are directly due to religion or due to the incidental effects of an associated environmental, social or lifestyle factor. This may eventually lead to the development of religiously-informed preventive interventions at the group level, particularly in populations with a high genetic risk of dementia, as well as a greater understanding of how population-level factors may influence the onset or development of this group of disorders.

The content published in Cureus is the result of clinical experience and/or research by independent individuals or organizations. Cureus is not responsible for the scientific accuracy or reliability of data or conclusions published herein. All content published within Cureus is intended only for educational, research and reference purposes. Additionally, articles published within Cureus should not be deemed a suitable substitute for the advice of a qualified health care professional. Do not disregard or avoid professional medical advice due to content published within Cureus.

The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

Human Ethics

Consent was obtained or waived by all participants in this study

Animal Ethics

Animal subjects: All authors have confirmed that this study did not involve animal subjects or tissue.

References

  • 1.Molecular pathogenesis of Alzheimer’s disease: an update. Sanabria-Castro A, Alvarado-Echeverría I, Monge-Bonilla C. Ann Neurosci. 2017;24:46–54. doi: 10.1159/000464422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Polygenic risk and hazard scores for Alzheimer's disease prediction. Leonenko G, Sims R, Shoai M, et al. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2019;6:456–465. doi: 10.1002/acn3.716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Meta-analysis of modifiable risk factors for Alzheimer's disease. Xu W, Tan L, Wang HF, et al. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2015;86:1299–1306. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2015-310548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gait and balance disturbances are common in young urbanites and associated with cognitive impairment. Air pollution and the historical development of Alzheimer's disease in the young. Calderón-Garcidueñas L, Torres-Solorio AK, Kulesza RJ, et al. Environ Res. 2020;191:110087. doi: 10.1016/j.envres.2020.110087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Modifiable predictors of dementia in mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cooper C, Sommerlad A, Lyketsos CG, Livingston G. Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172:323–334. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14070878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.What does (low) education mean in terms of dementia risk? A systematic review and meta-analysis highlighting inconsistency in measuring and operationalising education. Maccora J, Peters R, Anstey KJ. SSM Popul Health. 2020;12:100654. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100654. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.The effect of religion and spirituality on cognitive function: a systematic review. Hosseini S, Chaurasia A, Oremus M. Gerontologist. 2019;59:0–85. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnx024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Religious affiliation and the risk of dementia in Taiwanese elderly. Lin KP, Chou YC, Chen JH, et al. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2015;60:501–506. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2015.01.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Prayer at midlife is associated with reduced risk of cognitive decline in Arabic women. Inzelberg R, Afgin AE, Massarwa M, et al. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2013;10:340–346. doi: 10.2174/1567205011310030014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Does religiosity protect against cognitive and behavioral decline in Alzheimer's dementia? Coin A, Perissinotto E, Najjar M, et al. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2010;7:445–452. doi: 10.2174/156720510791383886. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Specific association between religiosity and cognitive functions in Alzheimer's disease. Jung J, Lee CH, Shin K, et al. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. 2019;34:254–260. doi: 10.1177/1533317519827416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Processes linking religious involvement and telomere length. Hill TD, Vaghela P, Ellison CG, Rote S. Biodemography Soc Biol. 2017;63:167–188. doi: 10.1080/19485565.2017.1311204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Which risk factors causally influence dementia? A systematic review of Mendelian randomization studies. Kuźma E, Hannon E, Zhou A, et al. J Alzheimers Dis. 2018;64:181–193. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Age of major depression onset, depressive symptoms, and risk for subsequent dementia: results of the German study on Ageing, Cognition, and Dementia in Primary Care Patients (AgeCoDe) Heser K, Tebarth F, Wiese B, et al. Psychol Med. 2013;43:1597–1610. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712002449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Risk of dementia in persons who have previously experienced clinically-significant depression, anxiety or PTSD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Kuring JK, Mathias JL, Ward L. J Affect Disord. 2020;274:247–261. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.05.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Depression as a risk factor for Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review of longitudinal meta-analyses. Sáiz-Vázquez O, Gracia-García P, Ubillos-Landa S, Puente-Martínez A, Casado-Yusta S, Olaya B, Santabárbara J. J Clin Med. 2021;10 doi: 10.3390/jcm10091809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Multidimensional understanding of religiosity/spirituality: relationship to major depression and familial risk. McClintock CH, Anderson M, Svob C, Wickramaratne P, Neugebauer R, Miller L, Weissman MM. Psychol Med. 2019;49:2379–2388. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718003276. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Examining the effects of religious attendance on resilience for older adults. Manning LK, Miles A. J Relig Health. 2018;57:191–208. doi: 10.1007/s10943-017-0438-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Religion and the presence and severity of depression in older adults. Hayward RD, Owen AD, Koenig HG, Steffens DC, Payne ME. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2012;20:188–192. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31822ccd51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Religious education and midlife observance are associated with dementia three decades later in Israeli men. Beeri MS, Davidson M, Silverman JM, Schmeidler J, Springer RR, Noy S, Goldbourt U. J Clin Epidemiol. 2008;61:1161–1168. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2007.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.A protective canopy: religious and social capital as elements of a theory of religion and health. Shapiro E. J Relig Health. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s10943-021-01207-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Religion, health, social capital and place: the role of the religious, social processes and the beneficial and detrimental effects on the health and wellbeing of inhabitants in deprived neighbourhoods in Malta. Satariano B. J Relig Health. 2020;59:1161–1174. doi: 10.1007/s10943-020-01006-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Incidence, prevalence, and hybrid approaches to calculating disability-adjusted life years. Schroeder SA. Popul Health Metr. 2012;10:19. doi: 10.1186/1478-7954-10-19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.World Health Organization. Global Health Estimates 2016 Summary Tables. [Jan;2021 ];World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GHE2016_DALYs-2016-country.xls Global Health Estimates. 2016
  • 25.World Health Organization. WHO methods and data sources for global burden of disease estimates 2000-2016: Global Health Estimates Technical Paper WHO/HIS/IER/GHE/2018.4. https://www.who.int/healthinfo/global_burden_disease/GlobalDALY_method_2000_2016.pdf 2018
  • 26.Pew Research Center. The age gap in religion around the world. [Jan;2021 ];https://www.pewforum.org/2018/06/13/the-age-gap-in-religion-around-the-world/ 2018
  • 27.The World Bank. GCI 4.0: Social capital. https://govdata360.worldbank.org/indicators/ha5376100 2021
  • 28.The World Bank. Life expectancy at birth, total (Years) [Aug;2021 ];https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SP.DYN.LE00.IN 2021
  • 29.User's guide to correlation coefficients. Akoglu H. Turk J Emerg Med. 2018;18:91–93. doi: 10.1016/j.tjem.2018.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Association between religious practice and risk of depression in older people in the subacute setting. Lac A, Austin N, Lemke R, Poojary S, Hunter P. Australas J Ageing. 2017;36:0–4. doi: 10.1111/ajag.12384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Altruistic behaviour, but not volunteering, has been associated with cognitive performance in community-dwelling older persons. Corrêa JC, Ávila MP, Lucchetti AL, Lucchetti G. Psychogeriatrics. 2019;19:117–125. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12372. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Religious participation predicts diurnal cortisol profiles 10 years later via lower levels of religious struggle. Tobin ET, Slatcher RB. Health Psychol. 2016;35:1356–1363. doi: 10.1037/hea0000372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Cortisol hypersecretion and the risk of Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Zheng B, Tal R, Yang Z, Middleton L, Udeh-Momoh C. Ageing Res Rev. 2020;64:101171. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2020.101171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Basal cortisol, cortisol reactivity, and telomere length: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Jiang Y, Da W, Qiao S, Zhang Q, Li X, Ivey G, Zilioli S. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2019;103:163–172. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.01.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Religiosity and telomere length: moderating effect of religiosity on the relationship between high-risk polymorphisms of the apolipoprotein E and TOMM40 gene and telomere length. Wang L, Koenig HG, He Z, Sun X, Shohaib SA, Wang Z. J Appl Gerontol. 2020;39:627–634. doi: 10.1177/0733464819865415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stress and Alzheimer's disease: a senescence link? Lyons CE, Bartolomucci A. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;115:285–298. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.05.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Lower prevalence of Alzheimer’s disease among Tibetans: association with religious and genetic factors. Huang F, Shang Y, Luo Y, et al. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016;50:659–667. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Relationship between religious social support and general social support with health behaviors in a national sample of African Americans. Debnam K, Holt CL, Clark EM, Roth DL, Southward P. J Behav Med. 2012;35:179–189. doi: 10.1007/s10865-011-9338-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Vegetarian diet, Seventh Day Adventists and risk of cardiovascular mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Kwok CS, Umar S, Myint PK, Mamas MA, Loke YK. Int J Cardiol. 2014;176:680–686. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2014.07.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Does social participation by the elderly reduce mortality and cognitive impairment? Hsu HC. Aging Ment Health. 2007;11:699–707. doi: 10.1080/13607860701366335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dietary pattern in relation to the risk of Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review. Samadi M, Moradi S, Moradinazar M, Mostafai R, Pasdar Y. Neurol Sci. 2019;40:2031–2043. doi: 10.1007/s10072-019-03976-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Cureus are provided here courtesy of Cureus Inc.

RESOURCES