Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2021 Mar 22;36(7):1720–1726. doi: 10.1002/mds.28585

Saccade, Pupil, and Blink Responses in Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder

Julia E Perkins 1,, Annette Janzen 2, Felix P Bernhard 2, Karén Wilhelm 2, Donald C Brien 1, Jeff Huang 1, Brian C Coe 1, David Vadasz 2, Geert Mayer 2, Douglas P Munoz 1,3,, Wolfgang H Oertel 2,†,
PMCID: PMC8359943  PMID: 33754406

Abstract

Background

Parkinson's disease (PD) patients exhibit deficits in saccade performance, pupil function, and blink rate. Isolated REM (rapid eye movement) Sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) is a harbinger to PD making them candidates to investigate for early oculomotor abnormalities as PD biomarkers.

Objectives

We tested whether saccade, pupillary, and blink responses in RBD were similar to PD.

Methods

RBD (n = 22), PD (n = 22) patients, and healthy controls (CTRL) (n = 74) were studied with video‐based eye‐tracking.

Results

RBD patients did not have significantly different saccadic behavior compared to CTRL, but PD patients differed from CTRL and RBD. Both patient groups had significantly lower blink rates, dampened pupil constriction, and dilation responses compared to CTRL.

Conclusion

RBD and PD patients had altered pupil and blink behavior compared to CTRL. Because RBD saccade parameters were comparable to CTRL, pupil and blink brain areas may be impacted before saccadic control areas, making them potential prodromal PD biomarkers. © 2021 The Authors. Movement Disorders published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of International Parkinson and Movement Disorder Society

Keywords: prodromal Parkinson's disease, eye movement, Parkinson's disease, biomarker


A major challenge in Parkinson's disease (PD) research is the discovery of a disease‐modifying therapy.1, 2 This therapy would be most effective during prodromal PD. To successfully prove such a therapy effective, biomarkers for prodromal PD must be identified. Such biomarkers should: (1) be related to the disease process and easily measurable; (2) reflect progression of prodromal PD; and (3) be responsive to therapy. Here, we investigate whether saccade, pupil, and blink behavior can provide suitable biomarkers. To test this hypothesis, we chose patients with the parasomnia “isolated REM (rapid eye movement) Sleep Behavior Disorder” (RBD).3 RBD is characterized by the loss of muscle atonia during REM sleep accompanied by dream enactment. The annual rate of phenoconversion of RBD to PD or another alpha‐synucleinopathy disorder (αSYND) is approximately 6% and nearly 80% of RBD individuals will develop an αSYND within 10–15 years.4, 5, 6 For these reasons, RBD is considered a specific prodromal phenotype for PD7, 8, 9 and suitable for biomarker research.

PD is clinically diagnosed using cardinal motor symptoms, namely bradykinesia, muscle rigidity, and resting tremor.10 These symptoms are caused, at least in part, by the loss of dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc).11, 12, 13, 14 An alternative is to objectively investigate oculomotor system abnormalities. Video‐based eye‐tracking provides a simple, non‐invasive, and effective way to assess brain function related to oculomotion, pupillary function, and blink rate. Measuring saccade, pupil, and blink behavior during sensory, motor, and cognitive tasks allows for the assessment of multiple brain circuits.15, 16, 17, 18 The interleaved pro‐ and anti‐saccade task (IPAST) pseudo‐randomly combines pro‐saccade (look towards a peripheral stimulus) and anti‐saccade (look in the opposite direction of peripheral stimulus) trials, which scrutinize areas that are implicated in PD, such as the basal ganglia (BG).14, 15, 19, 20, 21, 22 This task is also associated with pupil size changes related to locus coeruleus (LC) function.23 PD patients have specific deficits: increase in direction errors and slowed saccadic reaction time (SRT) during anti‐saccade trials, hypometric amplitudes during the pro‐saccade trials,24, 25 and dampened pupil constriction and dilation.22 The combination of saccade, pupil, and blink behavior have not yet been systematically studied in prodromal PD.

A recent study comparing anti‐saccade behavior between RBD and healthy, age‐matched controls (CTRL)26 reported increased direction errors on horizontal anti‐saccade trials for RBD patients. Here, we explore whether RBD patients have comparable deficits in saccadic, pupil, and blink behavior during IPAST to PD patients (here, and previously reported)22, 24, 25, 27, 28, 29 and reported RBD patients.26 Differences between RBD and CTRL identify potential prodromal PD biomarkers in this cross‐sectional study.

Methods

Here, we provide only a brief description of methodology that is expanded in Supplementary Materials S1.

Participants

This study was reviewed and approved by the human research ethics board of Queen's University, Canada and the Faculty of Medicine at the Phillips‐University of Marburg, Germany. Participant demographic and clinical assessment details are provided in Table S1.

RBD Patients

Twenty‐two RBD patients were recruited and screened with the REM Sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire (RBDSQ).30 The diagnosis RBD was confirmed using the criteria of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders,31 and video‐assisted polysomnography (PSG).

PD Patients

Twenty‐two PD patients diagnosed by UK Brain Bank criteria32 were recruited and clinically examined using the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale Part III (MDS‐UPDRS III) and the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA).

Controls

Seventy‐four healthy controls were recruited in Kingston, Canada. All controls were age‐matched with RBD and PD patients of the study. CTRL also completed the MoCA.

Recording Apparatus

Eye position, pupil size, and eye blinks were measured with a video‐based monocular eye‐tracker (see Supplementary Materials S1).

Interleaved Pro‐ and Anti‐ Saccade Task

Participants were seated in a dark room in front of a computer screen to perform IPAST (see Supplementary Materials S1, Fig. 1A). The pro‐saccade condition required an automatic visuomotor response (look at the stimulus), while the anti‐saccade condition required suppression of the automatic response and generation of a voluntary saccade in the opposite direction15, 33 (details in Supplementary Materials S1). A total of 240 trials were collected.

FIG. 1.

FIG. 1

(A) Fixation breaks displayed by group during pro‐ (filled) and anti‐ (empty) saccade task. (B) Percent of express saccades during pro‐saccade trials (saccadic reaction time [SRT] >90 ms <140 ms). (C) Median SRTs during the pro (filled) and anti‐ (empty) saccade task. (D) Median amplitude of primary saccades initiated during correct pro‐saccade trials. (E) Percent of direction errors made during the express latency epoch (SRT >90 ms <140 ms) made across groups, and (F) percent of direction errors made during the regular latency epoch (SRT >140 ms) made across groups. One‐way ANOVA with a post hoc Tukey's honest significant difference (HSD) revealed significance between groups *P ≤ 0.05, **P ≤ 0.01, ***P ≤ 0.001. CTRL, healthy, age‐matched controls; RBD, rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder; PD, Parkinson's disease. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Data Analysis

For each trial, eye movements, pupil size, and blink rate were categorized by an auto‐marking script (details in Supplementary Materials S1). All statistical comparisons were performed in SPSS using a one‐way, repeated measures ANOVA with a Tukey's honest significant difference (HSD) post hoc comparison unless stated otherwise.

Results

Saccade Metrics

Fixation Breaks

There was a significant difference in fixation breaks (Fig. 1A) between groups during pro‐saccade trials (F[2, 119] =5.483, P = 0.005). Post hoc comparison revealed that RBD and PD patients made significantly more fixation breaks than CTRL (RBD: P = 0.005; PD: P = 0.023). There were no significant differences during anti‐saccade trials (F[2, 119] =0.068, P = 0.934).

Saccadic Reaction Time

SRTs were broken down into two separate epochs: express (90 ms ≤ SRT ≤ 140 ms) and regular latency saccades (SRT > 140 ms) (see Supplementary Materials S1). There was a significant between group difference in express saccades in the pro‐saccade task (F[2, 119] =3.980, P = 0.021) (Fig. 1B). Post hoc comparison revealed that there was no significant difference between RBD patients and CTRL (P = 0.395), or RBD and PD (P = 0.537). PD patients made significantly more express saccades than CTRL (P = 0.019). Regular latency SRTs during pro‐saccade trials (Fig. 1C) did not differ significantly between groups (F[2, 119] =0.151 P = 0.860). Anti‐saccade RTs differed between groups (F[2, 119] =3.555, P = 0.032). Post hoc comparison revealed that PD patients had significantly longer SRTs than RBD (P = 0.013) and CTRL (P = 0.027).

Saccade Amplitude

Mean saccade amplitude differed significantly between groups (Fig. 1D) (F[2, 119] =9.047, P < 0.000). Post‐hoc comparison determined that PD patients had significantly smaller amplitude pro‐saccades compared to CTRL (P < 0.000) and RBD patients (P = 0.005).

Direction Errors

We separated direction errors into express (90 ms ≤ SRT ≤ 140 ms) (Fig. 1E) or regular (SRT > 140 ms) (Fig. 1F) latency. There was a significant difference between groups for express (F[2, 119] =4.322, P = 0.015) and regular (F[2, 119] =3.663, P = 0.029) latency direction errors. Post hoc comparison determined no difference in express latency errors made between RBD and CTRL (P = 0.813) or PD (P = 0.175). PD patients made more express errors than CTRL (P = 0.011). Post hoc comparison determined no difference between regular latency errors made between RBD and CTRL (P = 0.636). PD patients made more regular latency errors than RBD (P = 0.021) and CTRL (P = 0.040).

Pupil Metrics

Figure 2 shows averaged pupil responses during fixation and the gap period prior to target appearance. There were differences in amount of constriction and dilation between groups.

FIG. 2.

FIG. 2

Mean pupil traces for each patient group are represented during the pro‐ (A) and anti‐ (B) saccade conditions over time. (C) Constriction size was defined as the pupil size at the greatest constriction after fixation appearance. (D) Dilation magnitude was defined as the pupil size at stimulus onset minus the pupil size at the time of greatest constriction during FI, reflecting the increase of pupil size after constriction. (E) Median blink rate during the inter‐trial interval (ITI). *P ≤ 0.05, **P ≤ 0.01, ***P ≤ 0.001. CTRL, healthy, age‐matched controls; RBD, rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder; PD, Parkinson's disease. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com]

Constriction

There was a significant difference in pupil constriction size on pro‐saccade trials between groups (F[2, 104] =3.671, P = 0.029) (Fig. 2C). Post hoc comparison revealed that this was driven by a difference between RBD patients and CTRL (P = 0.038). RBD and PD patients did not differ from one another (P = 0.900). PD patients' pupils constricted significantly less than CTRL's during pro‐saccade trials (P = 0.035). There were group differences for constriction during anti‐saccade trials (F[2, 104] =2.834, P = 0.063).

Dilation

There was a significant difference between groups in pupil dilation during pro‐saccade trials (Fig. 2D) (F[2, 104] =6.684, P = 0.002). Post hoc comparison revealed a significant difference between RBD and CTRL patients (P = 0.045), and CTRL and PD patients (P = 0.001), but not RBD and PD (P = 0.216) during pro‐saccade trials. There was a significant difference between groups during the anti‐saccade trials (Fig. 2D) (F[2, 104] =4.346, P = 0.015). Post hoc comparison revealed there was no significant difference between RBD and CTRL (P = 0.234), and RBD and PD patients (P = 0.564) during the anti‐saccade trials. There was a significant difference between PD and CTRL (P = 0.018).

Blink Rate

There was a significant difference in blink rate between groups during the inter‐trial interval (ITI) (H [2]=6.957, P = 0.0155) (Fig. 2E). A post hoc pairwise comparison of groups determined that RBD (P = 0.0135) and PD (P = 0.024) patients made significantly fewer blinks than CTRL. There was no significant difference in blink rate between patients with RBD and PD (P = 0.336).

Discussion

We demonstrated reduced pupil constriction and dilation and reduced blink rate for RBD as well as for PD in comparison to CTRL. Thus, pupil and blink behavior may be sensitive indicators for neurodegeneration in RBD. Likewise, changes in these two parameters may represent objective, quantifiable biomarkers for prodromal PD pathophysiology.

We replicated previous findings of pro‐ and anti‐saccadic behavior in PD.24, 27, 28, 34, 35 Specifically, PD patients had significantly longer SRTs during the anti‐saccade trials (Fig. 1C), made more express saccades on pro‐saccade trials (Fig. 1B), had significantly lower amplitude pro‐saccades (Fig. 1D), and made more express and regular latency direction errors on anti‐saccade trials (Fig. 1E,F, respectively). This is analogous to features presented in Lu et al,35 where both on‐ and off‐medication PD participants differed from CTRL aside from pro‐saccade latency, as shown here (Fig. 1C). The discrepancy between our findings and previously reported RBD patients26 in respect to direction errors in the anti‐saccade task needs further detailed and long‐term investigations (further discussion in Supplementary Materials S1). In line with previous PD studies,22, 25, 28 both RBD and PD patients demonstrated significantly less pupil constriction during pro‐saccade trials (Fig. 2C). However, only PD patients had significantly less dilation during anti‐saccade trials (Fig. 2D). PD patients made significantly fewer blinks compared to CTRL in previous studies.10, 19, 36, 37 Here, both patients groups’ blink rate was less than CTRL during the ITI (Fig. 2E). It is likely that natural or spontaneous blinks tended to occur during the ITI to not interfere with task performance. This mechanism may be altered in PD and RBD. The similarity in reduced blink rate in both RBD and PD patients suggests that this deficit may be independent of the dopaminergic system. In RBD, the decrease in the dopaminergic nigrostriatal system must, by definition, be below the threshold for manifestation of cardinal motor symptoms.

The impairment of RBD pupil and blink responses − comparable to PD patients − is consistent with the hypothesis that lower areas of the brain stem38 and LC20 are affected in RBD by underlying αSYND. Pupillary function is controlled by a circuitry between the LC and the parasympathetic and sympathetic pupil pathways,22 and the LC is also involved in blink behavior.19, 39 The clinical phenotype of isolated RBD (ie, no motor impairment) is considered to be associated with a lesion in the sublaterodorsal nucleus of the LC/subcoeruleus complex. Thus, it is likely that the noradrenergic neurons in the LC are affected by the underlying αSYND before the dopaminergic system. The next step is to determine when these ocular abnormalities arise within RBD patients, and if they progress alongside the disorder continuing towards phenoconversion.

Author Roles

(1) Research project: A. Conception; B. Organization; C. Execution; (2) Data Collection; (3) Statistical Analysis: A. Design; B. Execution; C. Review and Critique; (4) Manuscript: A. Writing of the First Draft; B. Review and Critique.

J.E.P.: 1B, 1C, 2, 3A, 3B, 3C, 4A

A.J.: 1B, 1C, 2

F.P.B.: 2

K.W.: 2

D.C.B.: 3A

J.H.: 3A

B.C.C.: 3A

D.V.: 2

G.M.: 2

D.P.M.: 1A, 1B, 3C, 4B

W.H.O.: 1A, 1B, 3C, 4B

Supporting information

AppendixS1: Supporting information

Acknowledgments

We thank members of the Munoz laboratory for comments on statistical analysis and an earlier version of this manuscript. We are grateful to Elisabeth Sittig, Christine Höft, and Mahboubeh Habibi for their support in the phenotyping and long‐term documentation of subjects suffering from isolated REM sleep behavior disorders.

Relevant conflicts of interest/financial disclosures: Nothing to report.

Funding agencies: This work was funded by the International Research Training Group (IRTG) The Brain in Action (IRTG‐1901), German Research Foundation (DFG) to W.H.O., ParkinsonFonds Deutschland to W.H.O. and A.J., Canadian Institutes for Health Research (#MOP‐FDN‐148418) to D.P.M., and Ontario Brain Institute to D.P.M. J.E.P. was supported by NSERC–CREATE grant. D.P.M. was supported by the Canada Research Chairs Program. W.H.O. is Hertie‐Senior Researcsee h‐Professor supported by the Charitable Hertie‐Foundation, Frankfurt/Main, Germany. D.P.M. has no financial disclosures or conflict of interest. All other authors have no conflicts of interest, or financial disclosures.

Contributor Information

Julia E. Perkins, Email: 14jm23@queensu.ca.

Wolfgang H. Oertel, Email: oertelw@med.uni-marburg.de.

References

  • 1.Lang AE, Lozano AM. Parkinson's disease. N Engl J Med 1998;339:1044–1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Tysnes OB, Storstein A. Epidemiology of Parkinson's disease. J Neural Transm 2017;124:901–905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Schenck CH, Bundlie SR, Mahowald MW. Delayed emergence of a parkinsonian disorder in 38% of 29 older men initially diagnosed with idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder. Neurology 1996;46:388–393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Iranzo A, Fernández‐Arcos A, Tolosa E, et al. Neurodegenerative disorder risk in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder: study in 174 patients. PLoS One 2014;9(2):1–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Arnulf I. REM sleep behavior disorder: motor manifestations and pathophysiology. Mov Disord 2012;27:677–689. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Postuma RB, Berg D. Prodromal Parkinson's disease: the decade past, the decade to come. Mov Disord 2019;34:665–675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Berg A, Postuma RB, Adler CH, et al. MDS research criteria for prodromal Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2015;30:1600–1609. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Mahlknecht P, Seppi K, Frauscher B, et al. Probable RBD and association with neurodegenerative disease markers: a population‐based study. Mov Disord 2015;30:1417–1421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Postuma RB, Gagnon JF, Vendette M, et al. Quantifying the risk of neurodegenerative disease in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology 2009;72:1296–1300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Jankovic J. Parkinson's disease: clinical features and diagnosis. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2008;79:368–376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.DeLong MR. Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci 1990;13:281–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.DeLong MR, Wichmann T. Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal ganglia. Arch Neurol 2007;64:20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Obeso JA, Rodriguez‐Oroz MC, Rodriguez M, et al. Pathophysiology of the basal ganglia in Parkinson's disease. Trends Neurosci 2000;23:S8–S19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wu T, Wang J, Wang C, et al. Basal ganglia circuits changes in Parkinson's disease patients. Neurosci Lett 2012;524:55–59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Coe BC, Munoz DP. Mechanisms of saccade suppression revealed in the anti‐saccade task. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2017;372:20160192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Sweeney JA, Luna B, Srinivasagam NM, et al. Eye tracking abnormalities in schizophrenia: evidence for dysfunction in the frontal eye fields. Biol Psychiatry 1998;44:698–708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Tseng P‐H, Cameron IG, Pari G, et al. High‐throughput classification of clinical populations from natural viewing eye movements. J Neurol 2013;260:275–284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fedor J, Lynn A, Foran W, et al. Patterns of fixation during face recognition: differences in autism across age. Autism 2017;22(7):866–880. 10.1177/1362361317714989. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Mavridis M, Degryse A‐D, Lategan AJ, Marien MR, Colpaert FC. Effects of locus coeruleus lesions on parkinsonian signs, striatal dopamine and substantia nigra cell loss after 1‐methyl‐4‐phenyl‐1,2,3,6‐tetrahydropyridine in monkeys: a possible role for the locus coeruleus in the progression of Parkinson's disease. Neuroscience 1991;41:507–523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zarow C, Lyness SA, Mortimer JA, Chui HC. Neuronal loss is greater in the locus coeruleus than nucleus Basalis and Substantia Nigra in Alzheimer and Parkinson diseases. Arch Neurol 2003;60:337. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Wang C‐AA, Munoz DPA. Circuit for pupil orienting responses: implications for cognitive modulation of pupil size. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2015;33:134–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Wang CA, McInnis H, Brien DC, Pari G, Munoz DP. Disruption of pupil size modulation correlates with voluntary motor preparation deficits in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2016;80:176–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wang C‐A, Brien DC, Munoz DP. Pupil size reveals preparatory processes in the generation of pro‐saccades and anti‐saccades. Eur J Neurosci 2015;41:1102–1110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Cameron IGM, Watanabe M, Pari G, Munoz DP. Executive impairment in Parkinson's disease: response automaticity and task switching. Neuropsychologia 2010;48:1948–1957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Chan F, Armstrong IT, Pari G, Riopelle RJ, Munoz DP. Deficits in saccadic eye‐movement control in Parkinson's disease. Neuropsychologia 2005;43:784–796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hanuška J, Rusz J, Bezdicek O, et al. Eye movements in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: high antisaccade error rate reflects prefrontal cortex dysfunction. J Sleep Res 2019;28(5). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hood AJ, Amador SC, Cain AE, et al. Levodopa slows prosaccades and improves antisaccades: an eye movement study in Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2007;78:565–570. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Cameron IG, Pari G, Alahyane N, et al. Impaired executive function signals in motor brain regions in Parkinson's disease. Neuroimage 2012;60:1156–1170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Karson CN. Spontaneous eye‐blink rates and dopaminergic systems. Brain 1983;106:643–653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Stiasny‐Kolster K, Mayer G, Schäfer S, et al. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire ‐ a new diagnostic instrument. Mov Disord 2007;22:2386–2393. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.American Academy of Sleep Medicine . International classification of sleep disorders, revised. Diagnostic and Coding Manual. Chicago, Illinois: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2005. [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Gibb G, Lees AJ. The relevance of the Lewy body to the pathogenesis of idiopathic Parkinson's disease. Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51:745–752. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Munoz DP, Everling S. Look away: the anti‐saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004;5:218–228. 10.1038/nrn1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Amador SC, Hood AJ, Schiess MC, Izor R, Sereno AB. Dissociating cognitive deficits involved in voluntary eye movement dysfunctions in Parkinson's disease patients. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:1475–1482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Lu Z, Buchanan T, Kennard C, FitzGerald JJ, Antoniades CA. The effect of levodopa on saccades – Oxford quantification in parkinsonism study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2019;68:49–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Deuschl G, Goddemeier C. Spontaneous and reflex activity of facial muscles in dystonia, Parkinson's disease, and in normal subjects. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;64:320–324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Jongkees BJ, Colzato LS. Spontaneous eye blink rate as predictor of dopamine‐related cognitive function—a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2016;71:58–82. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Braak H, Del Tredici K, Rüb U, et al. Staging of brain pathology related to sporadic Parkinson's disease. Neurobiol Aging 2003;24:197–211. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Naegeli C, Zeffiro T, Piccirelli M, et al. Locus coeruleus activity mediates hyperresponsiveness in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2018;83:254–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

AppendixS1: Supporting information


Articles from Movement Disorders are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES