Abstract
Cognitive maturation during adolescence is modulated by brain maturation. However, it is unknown how these processes intertwine in early onset psychosis (EOP). Studies examining longitudinal brain changes and cognitive performance in psychosis lend support for an altered development of high-order cognitive functions, which parallels progressive gray matter (GM) loss over time, particularly in fronto-parietal brain regions. We aimed to assess this relationship in a subsample of 33 adolescents with first-episode EOP and 47 matched controls over 2 years. Backwards stepwise regression analyses were conducted to determine the association and predictive value of longitudinal brain changes over cognitive performance within each group. Fronto-parietal GM volume loss was positively associated with decreased working memory in adolescents with psychosis (frontal left (B = 0.096, p = 0.008); right (B = 0.089, p = 0.015); parietal left (B = 0.119, p = 0.007), right (B = 0.125, p = 0.015)) as a function of age. A particular decrease in frontal left GM volume best predicted a significant amount (22.28%) of the variance of decreased working memory performance over time, accounting for variance in age (14.9%). No such association was found in controls. Our results suggest that during adolescence, EOP individuals seem to follow an abnormal neurodevelopmental trajectory, in which fronto-parietal GM volume reduction is associated with the differential age-related working memory dysfunction in this group.
Keywords: working memory, attention, executive function, early onset psychosis, brain volume, adolescence, gray matter, magnetic resonance imaging, MRI
1. Introduction
The development of cognitive function plays a vital role in the ability of an individual to relate to the world. Altered cognitive functioning, particularly in high-order cognitive processes (i.e., sustained attention, working memory and executive function) has consistently been shown in early psychosis [1,2], leading to significant social, functional and vocational impairments and poor quality of life [3,4,5,6]. Cognitive maturation is largely influenced by the maturation of brain cortical structures. In particular, high-order cognitive processes have generally been associated with fronto-parietal brain regions, which appear to be the last regions to mature. Of particular importance is the prefrontal cortex, in which the connections with the parietal lobes are involved with cognitive abilities required for planning, decision making and complex problem solving, such as set-shifting, selective and sustained attention and working memory [7,8,9,10,11]. Reduced brain gray matter (GM) volume over time is characteristic in psychosis [12,13]. Our own clinical studies lend support for an altered development of higher brain cognitive functions [14], which parallels progressive GM loss in early onset psychosis (EOP) over time, particularly in fronto-parietal brain regions [15,16,17,18,19]. However, the relationship between these processes is not well understood.
Adolescence is a time of substantial brain and cognitive development. Gray matter changes/loss in the frontal, parietal, temporal and occipital lobes have been described as part of normal development [20], and these changes have shown to be accelerated in adolescent psychosis, particularly in early stages, for review see [21]. In this regard, the maturation of cognitive functions may coincide with synaptogenesis in the frontal and parietal cortex (i.e., synaptic pruning [22]) in adolescence and young adulthood, so that a disruption of those maturational processes may lead to the cognitive dysfunction observed in these individuals. However, little research has been done in this early onset psychosis (EOP) population. Our own previous studies in EOP have shown that cognitive impairment is present at the time of the first episode [23], and that cognitive development seems to be arrested at the 2 year follow-up, at least for some high-order functions, such as sustained attention or working memory [14]. Research has identified the development of high-order cognition to begin as early as 12 months of age [24,25], and exhibits a rapid development during childhood and adolescence [26]. Additionally, different aspects of higher cognitive functions are found to display different developmental trajectories [24]: Sustained attention, a basic underlying cognitive process required to complete any activity, is the first one to develop. While working memory and sequencing ability may be developed by the age of 15 to 19, strategic planning, problem solving and goal-directed behavior may have been achieved between the ages of 20 and 25 [24]. Thus, the development of high-order cognitive abilities is a continued process up until the late 20s, when brain development is considered to be complete [27]. However, the onset of psychosis may interfere with the developmental process [28]. In this regard, early neurodevelopmental abnormalities may underlie the differential acquisition of cognitive abilities in individuals with psychosis.
Previous studies investigating associations of GM volume and cognition in schizophrenia have been conducted mostly at a single point and in adult or young adult populations with inconsistent findings. Cognitive performance in these participants have been associated with whole-brain GM volumes [29,30,31,32,33,34], total brain volume [35], volumes of the frontal [36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48], parietal [41], temporal [40,46,49], cingulate [42], temporo-occipital [50], temporolimbic [51,52] or hippocampal [43,47] brain areas. Variability across cognitive and neuroimaging measurement strategies, antipsychotic treatment, patient heterogeneity, age of onset, duration of psychosis and duration of untreated psychosis may have contributed to inconsistent results in these studies. Two studies in particular have investigated the GM volume–cognition relationship in young adults with FEP [53,54]. A cross-sectional study investigating the relationship between brain volume and a combined measure of cognitive function (including measures of attention, working memory and verbal fluency such as the forward and backward digit span test from the Wechsler Memory Scale and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, COWAT) suggest an association between GM volume in the frontal, parietal and temporal cortices, specifically inferior regions of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and cognitive performance in 122 FEP individuals (with mean age 28.9 (8.6) years) [53]. A longitudinal study investigating the association of GM volume and cognitive function (working memory, executive functioning and processing speed) over the early course of FEP found an association between GM volume decline over 80 months in the left parietal lobe, and worse executive function performance at baseline [54]. In the same study, an association between GM loss in the globus pallidus and left inferior parietal lobule, and a lower processing speed at baseline was found in a sample of 16 drug-naïve non-affective FEP participants (mean age 24.08 (7.21)) [54]. Finally, in a cross-sectional investigation in a mixed sample of 41 individuals with chronic schizophrenia and 4 FEP subjects, (mean age 40.49 (11.67)) significant correlations were found between generalized cognitive deficits (lower premorbid IQ, verbal memory, attention and processing speed) and GM reductions within left fronto-temporal regions [55]. Only a previous attempt in our group examined cognitive baseline predictors of 2 year GM volume reductions in first episodes of adolescent EOP, reporting a specific relationship between low IQ at baseline and reduced GM in bilateral frontal cortices in the subgroup of 34 participants with early onset schizophrenia (mean age 15.2 (1.7)) [19]. In addition, low performance in working memory at baseline was associated with decreased GM volume in the frontal lobe (bilaterally) in this same subsample [19]. To the best of our knowledge, no previous studies have explored this brain–cognition relationship longitudinally, neither in first-episode EOP nor in the context of a particularly sensitive developmental window such as adolescence.
The aim of this study is to explore the relationship between brain changes in frontal and parietal GM volume, and changes in high-order cognitive functions (particularly sustained attention, working memory and executive function performance) in adolescents with a first episode of EOP and controls over a 2 year period, using data from the longitudinal child and adolescent first-episode psychosis study (CAFEPS) [56]. Progressive brain changes in specific frontal and parietal volumes in the CAFEPS sample have previously been described, with individuals with EOP presenting with greater loss of GM volume than controls, over the two years after the onset of psychotic symptoms [13,17,18]. Moreover, a significant cognitive impairment in sustained attention, working memory and executive function over time was found in EOP individuals in the CAFEPS sample over the same period of time [14]. Therefore, we aimed to investigate the relationship between these two events in those participants with EOP and controls who underwent baseline and follow-up brain and cognitive assessments in the context of the CAFEPS study. Therefore, we aimed to take these results a step further, to investigate the relationship between these two events in those participants with EOP and controls who underwent baseline and follow-up brain and cognitive assessments using the same previously validated methodology. The study of EOP participants whose brain and cognitive development is still in progress at the time of the first episode provides a unique opportunity to compare their brain and cognitive maturation with that of controls, in search of specific patterns resulting from plausible neurodevelopmental processes occurring at this particular stage. We hypothesized that a progressive loss of frontal and parietal GM volume over the 2 year follow-up will be associated with poorer cognitive development in high-order cognitive functions that have traditionally been associated to fronto-parietal areas, such as sustained attention, working memory and executive function, in adolescents with a first episode of EOP.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
The detailed description of participants, recruitment and complete methodology of the multicenter longitudinal follow-up CAFEPS study in Spain has been reported previously [56]. A total of 110 individuals with a first episode of psychosis and their corresponding 98 matched controls were consecutively recruited in child and adolescent outpatient and inpatient units in 6 hospitals in Spain. The recruitment took place between March 2003 and November 2005, according to the following inclusion and exclusion criteria. Participants were included if they/their legal guardians provided written informed consent and were aged between 7 and 17 years old and presented with a first episode of EOP of less than 6-months duration. Concomitant Axis I disorder, DSM-IV mental retardation, presence of any neurologic or pervasive developmental disorder, substance abuse, pregnancy or history of head trauma with loss of consciousness were exclusion criteria. Substance use was not an exclusion criterion if symptoms persisted 14 days after a negative routine urine drug test at first admission to the clinical centers. Controls were recruited from the same sociodemographic areas through dissemination by the hospital staff, word-of-mouth and advertisements. The study was approved by all the corresponding Institutional Review Boards at each of the participating clinical centers. All participants met MRS safety criteria. For this study purpose, only those participants that completed both baseline and longitudinal cognitive and magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) assessments were included in the analyses. Thus, a subsample of 33 first-episode EOP subjects (mean age 15.82; range [11,12,13,14,15,16,17]) and 47 matched controls (mean age 15.26; range [13,14,15,16,17]) (mean inter-scan interval (SD) = 25.11 [2.48] months) were considered from the original CAFEPS sample (see Figure 1). There were no differences in age, sex or clinical or functional characteristics between those participants with EOP who completed and the ones who did not complete the correspondent baseline and 2 year follow-up MRI and cognitive assessments (i.e., had either no valid baseline and/or follow-up MRI and/or cognitive data—see Supplementary Table S1).
2.2. Clinical Assessment
Diagnosis or its absence was established at baseline according to DSM-IV criteria, using the Spanish version of the semi-structured diagnostic interview; the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL) [57,58]; with which accuracy of diagnosis of psychosis was reviewed at the 2 year follow-up. The diagnosis established at follow-up was used for descriptive purposes only, and was grouped into three main diagnostic categories: schizophrenia (N = 13) bipolar disorder (N = 11) and other psychoses (N = 9; including schizoaffective disorder N = 2, depression with psychotic features N = 1, psychosis not otherwise specified N = 6).
The same trained psychiatrist conducted clinical and functional assessments for each EOP participant at both baseline and 2 year visits. Severity of symptoms was assessed using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [59,60]. Prior to recruitment, inter-rater reliability for the PANSS was determined in an independent sample of 10 individuals with psychosis, using the interclass correlation coefficient (ICC), which was always superior to 0.80. Level of general occupational, social and psychosocial functioning was determined using the Children Global Assessment of Functioning (C-GAF) scale [61]. Duration of untreated psychosis was calculated by asking EOP subjects/parents at the first interview about the approximate date of first appearance of positive psychotic symptoms within the first psychotic episode and by reviewing clinical case notes [62]. Neurodevelopmental history (normal or pathological acquisition of psychomotor, language and reading and writing milestones) was also explored by asking the parents at baseline. Chlorpromazine equivalents were used to derive the dosage of antipsychotic treatment at baseline and follow-up, and to calculate the cumulative doses taken during the scan interval [63,64].
2.3. Cognitive Assessment
Cognitive function was assessed at baseline and at two year follow-up using a comprehensive neuropsychological battery, including those cognitive functions consistently described as being affected in psychosis [1,2] (i.e., sustained attention, working memory, learning and memory and executive function). Participants completed this cognitive evaluation when considered to be psychopathologically stable (i.e., within 4 weeks after recruitment to the study). Of those, only high-order cognitive measures considered to be related to the frontal [42,47,65,66,67,68,69,70] and parietal regions [71,72] were selected, comprising the following cognitive test and their correspondent derived neuropsychological variables of sustained attention (digits forwards subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence scale, WAIS-III (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WAIS-III; 1997), time to complete Trail Making Test part A, TMT-A [73], total number of correct items from Words and Colors Stroop Test [74], total number of correct responses and average reaction time from the Continuous Performance Test, CPT [75]); working memory (digits backwards and letter-number sequencing subtests, WAIS-III [76]); and executive function (TMT B/A [73], number of errors, number of perseverative errors and number of categories of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, WCST [77], Stroop interference score and total number of correct words from the Verbal Fluency Test, FAS (Benton y Hamsher, 1989) and total number of correct words from the Controlled Oral Word Association Test, COWAT [78]) (See Table 1).
Table 1.
Cognitive Domain | Neuropsychological Variable |
---|---|
Sustained Attention | WAIS-III Digits Forward |
Time to complete TMT-A | |
Number of correct items Stroop 1 words and 2 colours | |
Number of correct responses CPT | |
Average reaction time CPT | |
Working Memory | WAIS-III Digits Backward |
WAIS-III Number–Letter Sequencing | |
Executive Function | TMT Derived Score TMTB—TMTA |
Number of words FAS | |
Number of words COWAT | |
Stroop Interference score | |
WCST number of perseverative errors | |
WCST number of errors | |
WCST number of categories |
WAIS-III, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd Edition; TMT-A, Trail Making Test, Part A; TMT-B, Trail Making Test, Part B; CPT, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test; TAVEC, Spanish version of the California Verbal Learning Test; FAS, Verbal fluency test; COWAT, Control Oral Word Association Test (Category Test); WCST, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test.
Psychologists that were trained in the use of the cognitive battery conducted all neuropsychological assessments. Inter-rater reliability for administration and scoring of the cognitive scales was determined using the ICC in an independent sample of 10 subjects prior to the baseline assessments (>0.85 for all instruments). Raw test scores were converted to z-scores (mean = 0, standard deviation, SD = ±1) based on the cognitive performance of the control group at baseline. The sample was divided into three age groups to minimize the effect of age and education on cognitive performance (aged 11–14, aged 15–16 and aged 17 in accordance with our previous research in the CAFEPS sample [14,23]. A summary score for each cognitive domain at both baseline and 2 year assessment times, and a measure of change at follow-up (2 year follow-up minus baseline) was calculated based on z-scores. The z-score sign was changed from plus to minus and vice versa for the higher scores, to always reflect better performance as required (i.e., time to complete the TMT-A and TMT-B, WCST errors). Z-scores were also truncated at ±4, to avoid outlying variables.
2.4. Brain Imaging
2.4.1. MRI Acquisition and Processing
Participants underwent anatomical brain MRI at baseline and at the 2 year follow-up visit in five different 1.5-T scanners: two Siemens Symphony, two General Electric Signa and one Philips ACS Gyroscan. Each individual was scanned with the same scanner at both baseline and follow-up. Data were collected from each center and processed at one site. MRI scans were acquired in axial orientation for each subject, a T1-weighted 3D gradient echo (voxel size 1 × 1 × 1.5 mm) and a T2-weighted Turbo-Spin-Echo sequence (voxel size 1 × 1 × 3.5 mm). Full details about the acquisition parameters at each site, comparability between machines for this study and the limitations involved in the analysis of multicenter data are provided elsewhere [79]. In keeping with our previous studies using this sample [17,18,80], lobar volumes of GM were obtained by using an automated method based on the Talairach proportional grid system [79,81,82,83,84]. Processed images were visually inspected and manually corrected by a single blind operator. Brain images from a sample of five randomly selected participants were used to test the consistency of the rater prior to conducting the whole segmentation procedure in the images of the study sample. The intra-class correlation coefficients ranged from 0.96 to 0.99 for lobar GM measurements [79].
2.4.2. Segmentation and Regions of Interest (ROI) Definition
MRI images were processed using locally developed software, incorporating a variety of image processing and quantification tools [79,84]. To obtain lobar volume measurements, we used a method for semi-automated segmentation of the brain based on the Talairach proportional grid system, which includes a grid template in which all sectors are assigned to particular regions [81,82]. A two-step procedure was followed [84]: (1) An initial segmentation of cerebral tissues into GM, white matter (WM) and CSF was obtained using Statistical Parametric Mapping 2 (SPM2) routines for multimodal (T1 & T2) segmentation. The SPM algorithm for tissue segmentation includes a multi-modal method to eliminate the effect of radiofrequency field inhomogeneities [83], which was proven to be more robust than single-modality in a multicenter setup [79]. (2) The Talairach grid was built on each edited brain MRI by manually selecting the position of the anterior and posterior commissures (AC, PC), and establishing a third point position in the mid-sagittal plane. The AC-PC line was set in the axial horizontal plane, and the inter-hemispheric plane in the vertical orientation [85]. Our software application automatically finds the outer brain limits in Talairach orientation, and 3D grids are then built for each brain. The Talairach grid, a piecewise linear transformation and a tessellation of the brain into a 3D grid of 1056 cells, represents homologous brain regions across subjects [85]. The ROI measurements were obtained by superimposing the 3D tissue masks corresponding to GM, WM and CSF onto each subject’s Talairach grid, where the regions of interest were defined as sets of Talairach grid cells [79,82,83]. Volumes for each tissue type in Talairach space were measured on the MRI registered to the Talairach space, by summing up the data from the Talairach grid cells associated with each ROI [84].
The validity of this procedure has already been proven suitable for volumetric studies [81,82,84] in our own longitudinal [17,18] and other multicenter studies [79,80,86].
Based on our own previous findings on brain volume changes and cognitive development in this sample [14,17], the ROIs selected and included in the analysis were the frontal and parietal lobes. These fronto-parietal regions were among the brain regions most likely to present volume changes over time in adolescent first-episode subjects [17,18,87,88], and have been associated with high-order cognitive processes, such as sustained attention, working memory and executive function [89,90,91,92,93,94,95,96,97,98,99]. Volumes were obtained in both left and right hemispheres. Intracranial volume (ICV) was derived as the sum of the total GM, WM and CSF volumes, including the cerebellum [17,18]. This data was exported to SPSS, version 25.0 (Chicago, Illinois) for statistical analyses.
The longitudinal change in volume was measured as the difference between follow-up (vol2) and initial volume at baseline (vol1) of the ROI, and described as a percentage calculated as follows [17,18]:
Longitudinal Change = ((vol2 − vol1)/vol1) × 100 |
2.5. Statistical Analyses
Normal distribution of variables was examined with the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test. Means and standard deviations (SD) were used to describe continuous variables. Frequencies were used to describe discrete variables. Differences between EOP individuals and controls in demographic, neuropsychological and clinical data were assessed by means of Student’s t-test, Fisher’s test and chi-square tests, according to the type of variable.
In order to examine the relationships between change in frontal and parietal GM volumes and change in cognitive performance in the sustained attention, working memory and executive function domains (composite z-scores), a series of backwards stepwise regression analyses were conducted. These models were built using relative within-subject longitudinal volume change (described above as a percentage) in bilateral frontal and parietal GM volumes, respectively, as independent variables, and changes at follow-up (2 year follow-up minus baseline) in cognitive outcomes as dependent variables. In congruence with our previous studies using this sample, the models included age, months of inter-scan interval and inter-scan ICV change as covariates of no interest. These three covariates were included in the model because of their potential effect on volume data. Assuming that ICV would be constant, inter-scan ICV change was included to remove a potential effect of spurious method variance associated with change from ICV1 to ICV2 [17,18]. In order to increase predictive power and avoid overfitting the model, a backwards stepwise regression model was built for each dependent variable, including one ROI at a time as a predictive variable together with the variables of no interest. These models were built for EOP participants and controls separately.
A final model was built in order to further analyze the predictive value of those independent variables that demonstrated significant associations with the primary variables of interest, entering only those variables, which emerged as significant predictors for each cognitive outcome.
Pearson’s linear correlation coefficients were used to rule out potential effects of medication on brain changes, and of medication and symptom changes on cognitive performance over time by examining their associations.
All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS v25 (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, IBM Corporation). Statistical significance was set at α < 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Sociodemographic and Clinical Characteristics
Individuals with EOP were not significantly different from controls, in terms of age, sex, parental socioeconomic status, years of education, developmental history, race or handedness (see Table 2). There were significant differences between groups for estimated IQ at baseline, consistent with previous reports on this sample [14,23] and for months of inter-scan interval in our sample (see Table 2). The mean duration of the illness, defined as the time between the appearance of their first positive symptoms and their baseline MRI scan, was 3.12 ± 2.75 months for the EOP sample included in the current analyses. Duration of antipsychotic treatment at baseline was 3 ± 1.87 weeks (mean daily dose 277.7 ± 150.4 mg in chlorpromazine equivalents [63,64].
Table 2.
EOP | Controls | Test a | p-Value | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
n = 33 | n = 47 | |||||
Mean | DS | Mean | DS | |||
Age (Years) [range] | 15.82 [11,12,13,14,15,16,17] | 1.467 | 15.26 [13,14,15,16,17] | 1.343 | t = 1.776 | p = 0.080 |
Sex (Female/Male) | 14/19 | 17/30 | X2 = 0.319 | p = 0.572 | ||
Education (Years) | 8.885 | 1.805 | 8.77 | 1.371 | t = 0.232 | p = 0.817 |
Parental Socioeconomic Status b (1/2/3/4/5) | 7/10/7/5/4/ | 3/14/13/3/14 | X2 = 7.915 | p = 0.095 | ||
Race/Ethnicity (Caucasian/Other) | 32/1 | 44/3 | X2 = 0.459 | p = 0.639 Fisher’s exact test | ||
Diagnosis at follow-up c (SZ/BP/Others) | 13/11/09 | - | - | - | - | |
Handedness (Right/Left/Mixed) | 27/6/00 | 42/3/2 | X2 = 3.931 | p = 0.140 | ||
Psychomotor Development (No. Normal/Pathology) | 31/2 | 44/3 | X2 = 0.003 | p = 0.953 Fisher’s exact test | ||
Language Development (No. Normal/Pathology) | 27/6 | 42/5 | X2 = 0.930 | p = 0.347 Fisher’s exact test | ||
Reading and Writing Development (No. Normal/Pathology) | 29/4 | 44/3 | X2 = 0.800 | p = 0.439 Fisher’s exact test | ||
Estimated IQ (Baseline) | 81.21 | 16.435 | 104.32 | 14.223 | t = −5.998 | p = 0.000 |
PANSS Negative (Baseline) | 18.82 | 8.921 | - | - | - | - |
PANSS Positive (Baseline) | 24.97 | 5.987 | - | - | - | - |
GAF (Baseline) | 34.64 | 15.56 | - | - | - | - |
Duration of untreated psychosis (Days) | 53.58 | 52.431 | - | - | - | - |
MRI Between-scan follow-up period (Months) | 24.45 | 2.152 | 25.57 | 2.483 | F = 0.33 | p = 0.039 |
Duration of antipsychotic treatment at baseline MRI (Weeks) | 3 | 1.87 | - | - | - | - |
Cumulative antipsychotic dosage (mg) Chlorpromazine equivalents d | 149.369, 2 | 104.428, 42 | - | - | - | - |
Antipsychotic treatment at baseline (No.) (Olanzapine/Risperidone/Quetiapine/Ziprasidone) e | 9/18/8/2 | - | - | - | - | |
Antidepressive Treatment at baseline (No.) (Paroxetine/Venlafaxine/Fluoxetine) | 1/2/02 | - | - | - | - | |
Mood Stabilizers at baseline (No.) (Lithium/Oxcarbazepine/Valproate) | 3/1/01 | - | - | - | - |
Abbreviations: GAF, Children’s Global Assessment of Functioning; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging; PANSS, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale. IQ = intellectual quotient. [a] Two sample t-test was used for comparisons between quantitative measures, and Fisher’s exact or Pearson’s chi-square test was used for comparisons between qualitative measures (p < 0.05). [b] Parental socioeconomic status assessed with the Hollingshead Scale (ranging from 1 to 5) (Hollingshead and Redlich 1954). A rating of 5 corresponds to the highest socioeconomic status and a rating of 1 to the lowest. [c] Diagnosis: SZ, schizophrenia; BP, bipolar disorder; Others (schizoaffective disorder, depression with psychotic features and psychosis not otherwise specified) (see Section 2 “Methods”). [d] Chlorpromazine equivalents were used to derive the dosage of antipsychotic treatment at baseline and follow-up, and to calculate the cumulative doses taken during the scan interval. [e] Patients were polymedicated, increasing the sample size for medication. At baseline four patients were taking two antipsychotics.
3.2. Relationship and Predictive Value of Change in Brain Volume Measures over Change in Cognitive Performance at Two Year Follow-Up
A total of four backwards regression models were built for each cognitive outcome (sustained attention, working memory, executive function) including one ROI for time together with the variables of no interest as predictors, for EOP individuals and controls separately (see Methods). For the sake of conciseness and clarity, only the main significant results are provided in Table 3. Tables showing all the regression models can be seen in Supplementary Table S2.
Table 3.
DV *: CHANGE IN WORKING MEMORY OVER TIME | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
PREDICTORS: Change in Frontal Left GM Volume, age, months of inter-scan interval and interscan ICV change. | |||||
Model Summary | R | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Standard Error of the Estimate | |
0.550 | 0.303 | 0.256 | 1.039 | ||
Model 4 | B | Standard Error | Beta | t | p |
Change in Frontal Left GM Volume | 0.096 | 0.034 | 0.441 | 2.854 | 0.008 |
Age | 0.027 | 0.010 | 0.412 | 2.666 | 0.012 |
PREDICTORS: Change in Frontal Right GM Volume, age, months of inter-scan interval and interscan ICV change. | |||||
Model Summary | R | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Standard Error of the Estimate | |
0.523 | 0.274 | 0.225 | 1.061 | ||
Model 4 | B | Standard Error | Beta | t | p |
Change in Frontal Right GM Volume | 0.089 | 0.035 | 0.400 | 2.570 | 0.015 |
Age | 0.023 | 0.010 | 0.347 | 2.230 | 0.033 |
PREDICTORS: Change in Parietal Left GM Volume, age, months of inter-scan interval and interscan ICV change. | |||||
Model Summary | R | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Standard Error of the Estimate | |
0.556 | 0.309 | 0.263 | 1.035 | ||
Model 4 | B | Standard Error | Beta | t | p |
Change in Parietal Left GM Volume | 0.119 | 0.041 | 0.451 | 2.910 | 0.007 |
Age | 0.028 | 0.010 | 0.426 | 2.752 | 0.010 |
PREDICTORS: Change in Parietal Right GM Volume, age, months of inter-scan interval and interscan ICV change. | |||||
Model Summary | R | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Standard Error of the Estimate | |
0.523 | 0.274 | 0.225 | 1.061 | ||
Model 4 | B | Standard Error | Beta | t | p |
Change in Parietal Right GM Volume | 0.125 | 0.049 | 0.403 | 2.571 | 0.015 |
Age | 0.025 | 0.010 | 0.389 | 2.480 | 0.019 |
FINAL MODEL. PREDICTORS: Change in Frontal Left GM Volume, Change in Frontal Right GM Volume, Change in Parietal Left GM Volume, Change in Parietal Right GM Volume, age. | |||||
Model Summary | R | R2 | Adjusted R2 | Standard Error of the Estimate | |
0.554 | 0.318 | 0.272 | 1.028 | ||
Model 4 | B | Standard Error | Beta | t | p |
Change in Frontal Left GM Volume | 0.107 | 0.034 | 0.490 | 3.131 | 0.004 |
Age | 0.362 | 0.129 | 0.441 | 2.815 | 0.009 |
* Dependent variable (DV), gray matter (GM).
We found that longitudinal change (decrease) in frontal GM volume was positively associated with decreased working memory performance in individuals with EOP (left (B = 0.096, p = 0.008); and right (B = 0.089, p = 0.015)) as a function of age (see Table 3). In addition, decreased parietal GM volume was positively associated with decreased working memory performance in this group ((left (B = 0.119, p = 0.007), right (B = 0.125, p = 0.015)) also as a function of age (see Table 3). Specifically, for the first multiple regression model, including change in frontal left GM volume over time as an independent variable, a decrease in frontal left GM volume over time explained a significant amount (18.9%) of the variance of change (decrease) in working memory, when variance for other variables in the final model was accounted for (age 16.4%). In the second multiple regression model, with change in frontal right GM volume over time entered as an independent variable, both change in frontal right GM volume and age variables added statistically significantly to the variance of change in working memory, after adjustment of other predictors in the model. In particular, decreased frontal right GM volume explained 16% of the variance of decrease in working memory performance over time, accounting for variance in age (12%). In the final solution of the third multiple regression model (including change in parietal left GM volume as an independent variable), both change in parietal left GM volume and age variables added statistically significantly to the variance of change in working memory, with decreased parietal left GM volume explaining 19.5% of the variance of decrease working memory performance over time. This was independent of age, which accounted for 17.5% of the variance in this model. The fourth regression model, with change in parietal right GM volume as an independent variable, explained a significant proportion of the variability of change in working memory performance over time. Specifically, a decrease in parietal right GM volume over time explained a significant amount (16%) of the variance of change (decrease) in working memory performance over time, when variance for other variables in this final solution was accounted for (age 14.9%).
A final multiple regression model was built in order to further examine the predictive value of the significant results obtained, regarding the reported relationship between variables of longitudinal change in frontal and parietal GM volume (left and right), age and working memory performance over time. In this model, a particular decrease in frontal left GM volume over the two year follow-up best predicted a significant amount (22.3%) of the variance of decreased working memory performance, together with age which accounted for 18% of the variance of longitudinal performance in this cognitive domain (Table 3).
No significant relationships were found for any of the brain GM volume measures and cognitive variables of sustained attention or executive function performance over time in individuals with EOP. There were no significant relationships between any of the longitudinal change in brain GM volume measures and cognitive variables in the control group.
These results were independent of antipsychotic exposure and symptom changes (see Supplementary Table S3).
4. Discussion
This is the first study to longitudinally examine the relationship between brain GM changes and high-order cognitive function development in a sample of adolescents with EOP and a sample of controls, with the latter adding to the novelty of our findings. Our results suggest a particular association between decreased GM volume of frontal and parietal lobes and a poorer working memory performance over the first 2 years of psychosis in adolescents with a first episode of EOP as a function of age. In particular, within EOP individuals but not controls, loss of GM volume in frontal and parietal regions and younger age at the time of the FEP were associated with decreased working memory function over time. No such association was found with other high-order cognitive functions (i.e., for sustained attention or executive function) in this sample. Further, specifically frontal left GM loss together with age have shown to be the best predictors of working memory performance over time in adolescents with EOP. No associations were found between any of the measures of brain GM volume change and the cognitive variables in the control group.
These findings are congruent with our previous studies in this sample, on which a specific arrest in cognitive development was observed for working memory function in the larger sample of EOP from which our sample was derived [14]. In particular, although EOP individuals presented with significant cognitive impairment with regards to controls at baseline and at the 2 year follow-up, both controls and EOP individuals improved in all cognitive measures over time, except for EOP working memory [14]. These changes occurred in tandem with a prominent reduction in frontal and parietal GM volume in EOP with regards to controls in this same larger sample, and a decrease was particularly evident for these fronto-parietal regions in the left hemisphere [17,18]. Our study now demonstrates the relationship between these two events as a function of age. Specifically, in the subset of individuals who completed both cognitive and brain imaging measures at baseline and 2 year follow up, our results suggest EOP is characterized by decreased working memory function over time, associated with age and accelerated brain GM volume loss in frontal and parietal structures, two neuroanatomical locations traditionally associated with working memory function [89]. Moreover, a specific reduction of GM brain volume in the frontal left structure was particularly predictive of working memory performance as a function of age.
Impairment in working memory has been previously linked to alterations in brain structure [68] and function [100] in psychosis. In accordance with our findings, previous cross-sectional volumetric studies in FEP have found reduced frontal [19,44,45,53] and temporo-parietal [53] GM volume to be associated with poor working memory function, particularly evident in the left frontal region [44]. More specifically, frontal and temporo-parietal gray matter volume has previously been associated with a cognitive index, including measures of sustained attention (forward digit span test), working memory (backward digit span, a test used in our study) and executive functioning (verbal fluency COWAT test) in adults with FEP [53]. Association between spatial working memory (assessed using the spatial working memory task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, CANTAB) and GM volume in the left frontal regions was also particularly evident in young adults with first-episode schizophrenia [44]. In a different study, lower GM volume in the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex was found to be specifically related to working memory impairment (assessed using the digits span test, as in our study) in adults in the early course of schizophrenia spectrum disorders [45]. Congruent findings with these FEP volumetric studies could reflect a unique pattern of associations in the early course of psychosis. Variability across neuroimaging analysis methodology or measurement strategies for assessing working memory is another possibility. In this regard, our results are also in accordance with previous morphometric studies in FEP, in which reduced cortical thickness in the frontal [101], frontal, parietal, temporal and cingulate [102] or temporal cortices [103] has been associated with working memory function in adults with psychosis. In detail, the study from Gutiérrez-Galve et al. [101] used measures of working memory span (spatial span task) and working memory manipulation (spatial working memory task) derived from the CANTAB, and found associations for working memory span only with frontal cortical area in FEP. Haring et al. [102] also used the spatial working memory test from the CANTAB in FEP individuals, and found associations with frontal, temporal, cingular and occipital cortical parameters in addition to associations of these brain regions with set-shifting, strategy usage and sustained attention cognitive abilities. Ehrlich et al. [103] used letter-number sequencing as a measure of working memory and found associations with cortical thickness in lateral prefrontal cortex in controls, whereas participants with established schizophrenia demonstrated associations with cortical thickness in a distinct right middle and superior temporal lobe brain region. Opposite to this later finding and consistent with previous studies, we have shown that some brain–cognitive associations tend to be specific to individuals with FEP [32,38,44,53,103,104,105]. However, it is important to consider the previously mentioned methodological differences and sample characteristics when trying to generalize findings across studies. In this regard, our study provides complementary findings of longitudinal/developmental brain volume and high order cognitive function associations using verbal/spatial working memory tasks in adolescents with psychosis compared to controls, which adds to the relevance of our findings.
Normative studies have consistently emphasized the role of working memory in cognitive development. Working memory function is crucial for general intellectual development [106,107] and a major predictor of high-order cognitive function development and school achievement [108]. Working memory ability progressively increases with age, from infancy until late adolescence [109,110,111,112], a time during which the brain undergoes major morphological and functional changes [20,26,27,113,114]. In typical development, fronto-parietal GM volume generally peaks at 11–12 years, whereas gains in working memory are extended up to 15–19 years of age, paving the way for the continuous development of executive function up to the mid-twenties [24]. Sustained attention, a basic underlying cognitive process required to complete any planning activity, is the first one to develop [10,89]. These were the three high-order cognitive domains assessed in our longitudinal study in adolescence. Therefore, the age range of our participants (11 to 17 years old) might represent a particularly sensitive period for the development of GM in the brain, and of cognitive functions, such as working memory (a cognitive milestone of this particular stage), in which the onset of psychosis might precipitate accelerated fronto-parietal brain volume changes, which in turn alter age-dependent working memory performance during the first two years after the first episode. The influence of age in cognitive [28] and brain abnormalities in psychosis had already been outlined by previous studies in our group [115]. In accordance with this notion, an accelerated loss of fronto-parietal GM volume was observed in our EOP sample (particularly relevant in the left frontal region), associated with working memory dysfunction over the first two years after the first psychotic episode as a function of age, indicating poorer working memory performance over time in younger participants with EOP. Interestingly, no such relationship was found for sustained attention or executive function, highlighting the relevance of these associations for this age range. In our study, these are associations specific of EOP, as they were not observed for adolescents in the control group. In other words, results partially confirmed our hypothesis that reduced fronto-parietal GM volume is associated with high-order cognitive function in EOP. In contrast to associations with working memory function, we did not find an association between fronto-parietal GM volumes and sustained attention or executive function in our sample. Our findings suggest that fronto-parietal brain GM loss (particularly in the left frontal lobe) may involve disruption of working memory development in adolescent psychosis, and thus be considered as a potential biomarker. The neurodevelopmental hypothesis emerges as a plausible explanation of the etiopathological mechanisms that might underlie cognitive development in our EOP sample at this particular point.
Limitations of the present study include the relatively small sample size, which prevented us from examining brain and cognitive changes in specific diagnostic subgroups, and may lead to unrepresentative groups of participants in comparison to the main study from which this sample was derived [56]. On that account, no differences were observed between those individuals with EOP who completed both cognitive and MRI assessment at baseline and 2 year follow-up and those who did not complete them. Second, the short follow-up of 2 years might not be fully representative of a broader pattern of cognitive development and brain changes in this stage of adolescence. We are currently conducting 10 year follow-up assessments for the same sample of individuals with EOP and controls, in order to further explore the trajectories of cognitive and brain development and its relationships over time in these subjects. Third, even though we found no effect of symptom severity or antipsychotic medication, the absence of a medication-free comparison group prevents the assessment of the impact that antipsychotic medication may have had on our findings. Fourth, our analysis protocol is restricted to frontal and parietal brain lobes and high-order broad cognitive domains, such as sustained attention, working memory and executive function, which constrains our findings to other cognitive domains/specific cognitive processes or to subcortical structures or larger-scale brain changes. Fifth, a potential interference of practice effects in neuropsychological testing might be another limitation. This interference is an inherent methodological limitation in longitudinal neuropsychological studies. It seems to be higher in repeated measures in short periods of time, and it seems common to all neuropsychological tasks [116]. Last, this was an exploratory study. Thus, corrections for multiple comparisons were not done, which could be perceived as a limitation. However, there is no current agreement among statisticians about the need to make p-value adjustments while analyzing data [117,118,119,120,121]. Since multiple testing adjustments control false positives at the potential expense of false negatives, we chose to report all comparisons and p-values and regard our findings as tentative [117,119,122] Future studies should evaluate individual components of the high-order cognitive functions evaluated, specifically of working memory performance and adopt a whole-brain and fine-grained interdisciplinary approach that incorporates multimodal imaging, biochemical, genetic and functional measures. This is in order to better understand the neural bases of cognitive function in psychosis, particularly the complex interplay between the observed longitudinal brain changes and cognitive outcomes, in a field where there is still a lack of effective treatments.
Strengths include the short duration of the illness and of the antipsychotic treatment at baseline. The homogeneity of the sample, in terms of age and education, limits the impact of sociodemographic factors and maturational processes in our findings. The follow-up duration was established carefully to be the same for all participants, reducing the confounding factors that could be accounting for longitudinal changes. This study may also have important clinical implications for the development and implementation of both cognitive training programs and novel pharmacological treatments. Adolescence is a time of continued brain and cognitive development. It thus represents a critical time window of neural plasticity when different brain regions and superior cognitive functions are still maturing. Considering our findings, the implementation of effective cognitive and pharmacological strategies targeting brain and cognitive changes at the time of the first episode are thus of utmost importance, to mitigate the effects of psychosis on neurodevelopmental processes that might underlie persistent cognitive deficits in psychosis.
5. Conclusions
Our results highlight the relationship between brain volume changes in fronto-parietal regions, and the development of working memory function in adolescents with EOP as a function of age. These two events co-occur in a crucial time of adolescence that are particularly sensitive to working memory and frontal brain volume changes, which might explain the observed “disruption” in these neurodevelopmental processes in adolescents within our age range. A reduction in frontal left GM volume in particular can be considered as a potential predictive biomarker underlying the observed working memory impairment. Thus, our findings may guide the development of new therapeutic approaches based on the neurodevelopmental processes (brain and functional/cognitive changes) occurring at the time of the first episode in EOP. Boosting changes in working memory capacity might stimulate developmental changes in high-order cognitive function, which might lead to better clinical and functional outcomes in this population [123].
Supplementary Materials
The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm10173929/s1, Table S1: Comparison of Clinical and functional characteristics of completers and non-completers at baseline. Table S2: Associations and predictive value of change in brain volume measures over change in sustained attention and executive function at two-year follow-up in EOP (Initial prediction models for non-significant backwards regression analyses are displayed—see Section 2). Table S3: Pearson correlations between changes in symptomatology and antipsychotic medication and changes in cognitive performance and brain volume in patients with a first episode of psychosis.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, M.R.-C. and C.A.; methodology, M.R.-C., D.F. and J.J.; formal analysis, M.V.-A. and M.R.-C.; investigation, M.R.-C., M.V.-A., J.J., D.F., I.B. (Igor Bombin), J.C.-F., M.M., A.G.-P., E.d.l.S., M.P., D.M., B.P., M.G., I.B. (Inmaculada Baeza), C.P. and C.A.; resources, all authors; data curation, M.V.-A. and M.R.-C.; writing—original draft preparation, M.V.-A. and M.R.-C.; writing—review and editing, all authors; supervision, C.P. and C.A.; funding acquisition, J.C.-F., A.G.-P., B.P., M.G. and C.A. All authors have contributed to the editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Funding
This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Redes Temáticas-ISCIII G03-032; RETICS (REM-TAP Network) RD06/0011; CIBERSAM. MR-C is a Ramon y Cajal Research Fellow (RYC-2017-23144), Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities and was supported by a NARSAD independent investigator grant (No. 24628) from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation. JJ has received grant/research support from European Union Funds, Instituto de Salud Carlos III Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness, and Ramon y Cajal Research Fellowship program, Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities DF was partially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation. Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI14/00397, PI17/00481, PI20/00216), co-financed by ERDF Funds from the European Commission, “A way of making Europe”. IB was supported by ISCIII (INT19/00021). AG-P was supported by the University of the Basque Country 321218ELCY IT1232-19, and the Basque Government 2017111104, the Carlos III Healthcare Institute, the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF/FEDER) (PI18/01055). CP was supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) Senior Principal Research Fellowship (1105825), an NHMRC L3 Investigator Grant (1196508) MR-C, JJ, MM, MP, DM and CA were partially supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation. Instituto de Salud Carlos III, (PI18/00753), co-financed by ERDF Funds from the European Commission, “A way of making Europe”, CIBERSAM. Madrid Regional Government (B2017/BMD-3740 AGES-CM-2), European Union Structural Funds. European Union Seventh Framework Program under grant agreements FP7-HEALTH-2009-2.2.1-2-241909 (Project EU-GEI), FP7-HEALTH-2009-2.2.1-3-242114 (Project OPTiMISE), FP7- HEALTH-2013-2.2.1-2-603196 (Project PSYSCAN) and FP7- HEALTH-2013- 2.2.1-2-602478 (Project METSY); and European Union H2020 Program under the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking (grant agreement No. 115916, Project PRISM, and grant agreement No. 777394, Project AIMS-2-TRIALS), Fundación Familia Alonso and Fundación Alicia Koplowitz.
Institutional Review Board Statement
The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by all the corresponding Institutional Review Boards at each participating clinical center (i.e., ethics committee of Hospital Gregorio Marañon, Hospital Clinic Barcelona, Hospital Valdecilla, Hospital Santiago Apostol, Clinica Universidad Navarra) (project identification code: G03032; 15 May 2003).
Informed Consent Statement
Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study and/or their parents or legal guardians.
Conflicts of Interest
D.F. has received grants and served as consultant, advisor or CME speaker for the following entities: Angelini, Casen Recordati, Janssen, Lundbeck, Otsuka, the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (Instituto de Salud Carlos III), co-financed by EDRF funds from the EU, and the Fundación Alicia Koplowitz AG-P is employed by the University of the Basque Country and has been a consultant for the Ministry of Science of Spain and the Basque Government. A.G.P. also gives conferences, acts as consultant to or receives grants from Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Pfizer, Sanofi-Aventis, Alter, Angelini, Exeltis, Takeda. M.P. has received grants and served as consultant, advisor or speaker for Servier, Roche, Exeltis. D.M. has received honoraria from Otsuka-Lundbeck. IB has received support from Otsuka-Ludbeck, Janssen and Angelini for attending a conference or honoraria. C.P. has received a NHMRC Australian-based Senior Principal Research Fellowship (ID: 628386), NARSAD Distinguished Investigator Award (US; grant ID: 18722), and NHMRC Program Grant (ID: 566529); he has received grant support from Janssen-Cilag, Eli Lilly, Hospira (Mayne), Astra Zeneca and has provided consultancy to Janssen-Cilag, Eli Lilly, Hospira (Mayne), Astra Zeneca, Pfizer, Schering Plough, Lundbeck. He has undertaken investigator-initiated studies supported by Eli Lilly, Hospira, Janssen Cilag and Astra Zeneca. C.A. has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Angelini, Boehringer, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Minerva, Otsuka-Ludbeck, Roche, Sage, Servier, Shire, Schering Plough, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovion and Takeda, unrelated to the submitted work. The rest of the authors declare no conflict of interest.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Heinrichs R.W., Zakzanis K.K. Neurocognitive deficit in schizophrenia: A quantitative review of the evidence. Neuropsychology. 1998;12:426–445. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.12.3.426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mesholam-Gately R.I., Giuliano A.J., Goff K.P., Faraone S.V., Seidman L.J. Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: A meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology. 2009;23:315–336. doi: 10.1037/a0014708. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Green M.F. What are the functional consequences of neurocognitive deficits in schizophrenia? Am. J. Psychiatry. 1996;153:321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Holthausen E.A., Wiersma D., Cahn W., Kahn R.S., Dingemans P.M., Schene A.H., van den Bosch R.J. Predictive value of cognition for different domains of outcome in recent-onset schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 2007;149:71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Martínez-Arán A., Penadés R., Vieta E., Colom F., Reinares M., Benabarre A., Salamero M., Gastó C. Executive function in patients with remitted bipolar disorder and schizophrenia and its relationship with functional outcome. Psychother. Psychosom. 2002;71:39–46. doi: 10.1159/000049342. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.González-Ortega I., de Los Mozos V., Echeburúa E., Mezo M., Besga A., Ruiz de Azúa S., González-Pinto A., Gutierrez M., Zorrilla I., González-Pinto A. Working memory as a predictor of negative symptoms and functional outcome in first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2013;206:8–16. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.08.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Corbetta M., Kincade J.M., Shulman G.L. Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2002;14:508–523. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Corbetta M., Shulman G.L. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2002;3:201–215. doi: 10.1038/nrn755. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Curtis C.E., D’Esposito M. Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:415–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00197-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: A unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2011;15:483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Barbey A.K., Koenigs M., Grafman J. Dorsolateral prefrontal contributions to human working memory. Cortex. 2013;49:1195–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.05.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Pantelis C., Velakoulis D., McGorry P.D., Wood S.J., Suckling J., Phillips L.J., Yung A.R., Bullmore E.T., Brewer W., Soulsby B., et al. Neuroanatomical abnormalities before and after onset of psychosis: A cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI comparison. Lancet. 2003;361:281–288. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(03)12323-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bartholomeusz C.F., Cropley V.L., Wannan C., Di Biase M., McGorry P.D., Pantelis C. Structural neuroimaging across early-stage psychosis: Aberrations in neurobiological trajectories and implications for the staging model. Aust. N. Z. J. Psychiatry. 2017;51:455–476. doi: 10.1177/0004867416670522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Bombin I., Mayoral M., Castro-Fornieles J., Gonzalez-Pinto A., de la Serna E., Rapado-Castro M., Barbeito S., Parellada M., Baeza I., Graell M., et al. Neuropsychological evidence for abnormal neurodevelopment associated with early-onset psychoses. Psychol. Med. 2013;43:757–768. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pantelis C., Yücel M., Wood S.J., Velakoulis D., Sun D., Berger G., Stuart G.W., Yung A., Phillips L., McGorry P.D. Structural brain imaging evidence for multiple pathological processes at different stages of brain development in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2005;31:672–696. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pantelis C., Yücel M., Bora E., Fornito A., Testa R., Brewer W.J., Velakoulis D., Wood S.J. Neurobiological markers of illness onset in psychosis and schizophrenia: The search for a moving target. Neuropsychol. Rev. 2009;19:385–398. doi: 10.1007/s11065-009-9114-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Arango C., Rapado-Castro M., Reig S., Castro-Fornieles J., González-Pinto A., Otero S., Baeza I., Moreno C., Graell M., Janssen J., et al. Progressive brain changes in children and adolescents with first-episode psychosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2012;69:16–26. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.150. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Rapado-Castro M., Bartholomeusz C.F., Castro-Fornieles J., González-Pinto A., Otero S., Baeza I., Moreno C., Graell M., Janssen J., Bargalló N., et al. Gender effects on brain changes in early-onset psychosis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2015;24:1193–1205. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0669-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Castro-Fornieles J., Bargalló N., Calvo A., Arango C., Baeza I., Gonzalez-Pinto A., Parellada M., Graell M., Moreno C., Otero S., et al. Gray matter changes and cognitive predictors of 2-year follow-up abnormalities in early-onset first-episode psychosis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2018;27:113–126. doi: 10.1007/s00787-017-1013-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Shaw P., Kabani N.J., Lerch J.P., Eckstrand K., Lenroot R., Gogtay N., Greenstein D., Clasen L., Evans A., Rapoport J.L., et al. Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:3586–3594. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5309-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fraguas D., Díaz-Caneja C.M., Pina-Camacho L., Janssen J., Arango C. Progressive brain changes in children and adolescents with early-onset psychosis: A meta-analysis of longitudinal MRI studies. Schizophr. Res. 2016;173:132–139. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.12.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Casey B.J., Giedd J.N., Thomas K.M. Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. 2000;54:241–257. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00058-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Zabala A., Rapado M., Arango C., Robles O., de la Serna E., González C., Rodríguez-Sánchez J.M., Andrés P., Mayoral M., Bombín I. Neuropsychological functioning in early-onset first-episode psychosis: Comparison of diagnostic subgroups. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2010;260:225–233. doi: 10.1007/s00406-009-0046-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.De Luca C.R., Wood S.J., Anderson V., Buchanan J.A., Proffitt T.M., Mahony K., Pantelis C. Normative data from the CANTAB. I: Development of executive function over the lifespan. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2003;25:242–254. doi: 10.1076/jcen.25.2.242.13639. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Testa R.R., Pantelis C. The role of executive functions in psychiatric disorders. In: Allen N.B., Wood S.J., Pantelis C., editors. The Neuropsychology of Mental Illness. Cambridge University Press; Melbourne, Australia: 2009. pp. 117–137. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Blakemore S.J. Imaging brain development: The adolescent brain. Neuroimage. 2012;61:397–406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Arain M., Haque M., Johal L., Mathur P., Nel W., Rais A., Sandhu R., Sharma S. Maturation of the adolescent brain. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 2013;9:449–461. doi: 10.2147/ndt.S39776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Fagerlund B., Pantelis C., Jepsen J.R.M., Raghava J.M., Rostrup E., Thomas M.B., Nielsen M., Bojesen K., Jensen K.G., Stentebjerg-Decara M., et al. Differential effects of age at illness onset on verbal memory functions in antipsychotic-naïve schizophrenia patients aged 12–43 years. Psychol. Med. 2020;51:1570–1580. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720000409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Gur R.E., Turetsky B.I., Bilker W.B., Gur R.C. Reduced gray matter volume in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1999;56:905–911. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.56.10.905. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Zipursky R.B., Lambe E.K., Kapur S., Mikulis D.J. Cerebral gray matter volume deficits in first episode psychosis. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1998;55:540–546. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Sullivan E.V., Shear P.K., Lim K.O., Zipursky R.B., Pfefferbaum A. Cognitive and motor impairments are related to gray matter volume deficits in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1996;39:234–240. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00135-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Toulopoulou T., Grech A., Morris R.G., Schulze K., McDonald C., Chapple B., Rabe-Hesketh S., Murray R.M. The relationship between volumetric brain changes and cognitive function: A family study on schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 2004;56:447–453. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.06.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gur R.E., Cowell P.E., Latshaw A., Turetsky B.I., Grossman R.I., Arnold S.E., Bilker W.B., Gur R.C. Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2000;57:761–768. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.8.761. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Czepielewski L.S., Wang L., Gama C.S., Barch D.M. The Relationship of Intellectual Functioning and Cognitive Performance to Brain Structure in Schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 2017;43:355–364. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Kareken D.A., Gur R.C., Mozley P.D., Mozley L.H., Saykin A.J., Shtasel D.L., Gur R.E. Cognitive functioning and neuroanatomic volume measures in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 1995;9:211–219. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.2.211. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Gur R.E., Cowell P., Turetsky B.I., Gallacher F., Cannon T., Bilker W., Gur R.C. A follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia. Relationship of neuroanatomical changes to clinical and neurobehavioral measures. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1998;55:145–152. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.55.2.145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Maher B.A., Manschreck T.C., Woods B.T., Yurgelun-Todd D.A., Tsuang M.T. Frontal brain volume and context effects in short-term recall in schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1995;37:144–150. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)00203-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Baaré W.F., Hulshoff Pol H.E., Hijman R., Mali W.P., Viergever M.A., Kahn R.S. Volumetric analysis of frontal lobe regions in schizophrenia: Relation to cognitive function and symptomatology. Biol. Psychiatry. 1999;45:1597–1605. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00266-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Szeszko P.R., Bilder R.M., Lencz T., Ashtari M., Goldman R.S., Reiter G., Wu H., Lieberman J.A. Reduced anterior cingulate gyrus volume correlates with executive dysfunction in men with first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2000;43:97–108. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00155-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Seidman L.J., Yurgelun-Todd D., Kremen W.S., Woods B.T., Goldstein J.M., Faraone S.V., Tsuang M.T. Relationship of prefrontal and temporal lobe MRI measures to neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia. Biol. Psychiatry. 1994;35:235–246. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(94)91254-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Salgado-Pineda P., Baeza I., Pérez-Gómez M., Vendrell P., Junqué C., Bargalló N., Bernardo M. Sustained attention impairment correlates to gray matter decreases in first episode neuroleptic-naive schizophrenic patients. Neuroimage. 2003;19:365–375. doi: 10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00094-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rüsch N., Spoletini I., Wilke M., Bria P., Di Paola M., Di Iulio F., Martinotti G., Caltagirone C., Spalletta G. Prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar gray matter networks and executive functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2007;93:79–89. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Guo X., Li J., Wang J., Fan X., Hu M., Shen Y., Chen H., Zhao J. Hippocampal and orbital inferior frontal gray matter volume abnormalities and cognitive deficit in treatment-naive, first-episode patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2014;152:339–343. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.12.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Cocchi L., Walterfang M., Testa R., Wood S.J., Seal M.L., Suckling J., Takahashi T., Proffitt T.-M., Brewer W.J., Adamson C., et al. Grey and white matter abnormalities are associated with impaired spatial working memory ability in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 2009;115:163–172. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Wojtalik J.A., Eack S.M., Pollock B.G., Keshavan M.S. Prefrontal gray matter morphology mediates the association between serum anticholinergicity and cognitive functioning in early course schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2012;204:61–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.04.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Zhang X., Yao J., Lv Y., Zhao X., Li Y., Sui Y., Zhiping D. An Association Study on the Cognitive Function and the Cerebral Grey Matter Volume of Patients with First-Episode Schizophrenia. Shanghai Arch. Psychiatry. 2018;30:154–167. doi: 10.11919/j.issn.1002-0829.217138. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Sanfilipo M., Lafargue T., Rusinek H., Arena L., Loneragan C., Lautin A., Rotrosen J., Wolkin A. Cognitive performance in schizophrenia: Relationship to regional brain volumes and psychiatric symptoms. Psychiatry Res. 2002;116:1–23. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(02)00046-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Goldberg T.E., Torrey E.F., Berman K.F., Weinberger D.R. Relations between neuropsychological performance and brain morphological and physiological measures in monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 1994;55:51–61. doi: 10.1016/0925-4927(94)90011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Vita A., Dieci M., Giobbio G.M., Caputo A., Ghiringhelli L., Comazzi M., Garbarini M., Mendini A.P., Morganti C., Tenconi F., et al. Language and thought disorder in schizophrenia: Brain morphological correlates. Schizophr. Res. 1995;15:243–251. doi: 10.1016/0920-9964(94)00050-I. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rigucci S., Rossi-Espagnet C., Ferracuti S., De Carolis A., Corigliano V., Carducci F., Mancinelli I., Cicone F., Tatarelli R., Bozzao A., et al. Anatomical substrates of cognitive and clinical dimensions in first episode schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2013;128:261–270. doi: 10.1111/acps.12051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Gur R.E., Turetsky B.I., Cowell P.E., Finkelman C., Maany V., Grossman R.I., Arnold S.E., Bilker W.B., Gur R.C. Temporolimbic volume reductions in schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2000;57:769–775. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.8.769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Szeszko P.R., Strous R.D., Goldman R.S., Ashtari M., Knuth K.H., Lieberman J.A., Bilder R.M. Neuropsychological correlates of hippocampal volumes in patients experiencing a first episode of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2002;159:217–226. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.2.217. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Minatogawa-Chang T.M., Schaufelberger M.S., Ayres A.M., Duran F.L.S., Gutt E.K., Murray R.M., Rushe T.M., McGuire P.K., Menezes P.R., Scazufca M., et al. Cognitive performance is related to cortical grey matter volumes in early stages of schizophrenia: A population-based study of first-episode psychosis. Schizophr. Res. 2009;113:200–209. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Dempster K., Norman R., Théberge J., Densmore M., Schaefer B., Williamson P. Cognitive performance is associated with gray matter decline in first-episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2017;264:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.04.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Antonova E., Kumari V., Morris R., Halari R., Anilkumar A., Mehrotra R., Sharma T. The relationship of structural alterations to cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: A voxel-based morphometry study. Biol. Psychiatry. 2005;58:457–467. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.04.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Castro-Fornieles J., Parellada M., Gonzalez-Pinto A., Moreno D., Graell M., Baeza I., Otero S., Soutullo C.A., Crespo-Facorro B., Ruiz-Sancho A., et al. The child and adolescent first-episode psychosis study (CAFEPS): Design and baseline results. Schizophr. Res. 2007;91:226–237. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Geller B., Zimerman B., Williams M., Bolhofner K., Craney J.L., DelBello M.P., Soutullo C. Reliability of the Washington University in St. Louis Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia (WASH-U-KSADS) mania and rapid cycling sections. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2001;40:450–455. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200104000-00014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Soutullo C. Traducción al Español de la Entrevista Diagnóstica: Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia, Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL, 1996) [(accessed on 30 June 2021)];2007 Available online: http://www.cun.es/la-clinica/departamentos-yservicios-medicos/psiquiatria-y-psicologia-medica/mas-sobreel-departamento/unidades/psiquiatria-infantil-y-adolescente.
- 59.Kay S.R., Fiszbein A., Opler L.A. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr. Bull. 1987;13:261–276. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Peralta Martín V., Cuesta Zorita M.J. Validation of positive and negative symptom scale (PANSS) in a sample of Spanish schizophrenic patients. Actas Luso-Esp. Neurol. Psiquiatr. Cienc. Afines. 1994;22:171–177. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Shaffer D., Gould M.S., Brasic J., Ambrosini P., Fisher P., Bird H., Aluwahlia S. A children’s global assessment scale (CGAS) Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1983;40:1228–1231. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790100074010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Fraguas D., Del Rey-Mejías A., Moreno C., Castro-Fornieles J., Graell M., Otero S., Gonzalez-Pinto A., Moreno D., Baeza I., Martínez-Cengotitabengoa M., et al. Duration of untreated psychosis predicts functional and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with first-episode psychosis: A 2-year longitudinal study. Schizophr. Res. 2014;152:130–138. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Rey M.J., Schulz P., Costa C., Dick P., Tissot R. Guidelines for the dosage of neuroleptics. I: Chlorpromazine equivalents of orally administered neuroleptics. Int. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1989;4:95–104. doi: 10.1097/00004850-198904000-00001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Woods S.W. Chlorpromazine equivalent doses for the newer atypical antipsychotics. J. Clin. Psychiatry. 2003;64:663–667. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v64n0607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Narbona J.C.N. Evaluación neuropsicológica del niño. In: Peña-Casanova J., editor. Manual de Logopedia. Elsevier; Barcelona, Spain: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Zimmerman M.E., Brickman A.M., Paul R.H., Grieve S.M., Tate D.F., Gunstad J., Cohen R.A., Aloia M.S., Williams L.M., Clark C.R., et al. The relationship between frontal gray matter volume and cognition varies across the healthy adult lifespan. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2006;14:823–833. doi: 10.1097/01.JGP.0000238502.40963.ac. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Ramsden S., Richardson F.M., Josse G., Thomas M.S., Ellis C., Shakeshaft C., Seghier M.L., Price C.J. Verbal and non-verbal intelligence changes in the teenage brain. Nature. 2011;479:113–116. doi: 10.1038/nature10514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Antonova E., Sharma T., Morris R., Kumari V. The relationship between brain structure and neurocognition in schizophrenia: A selective review. Schizophr. Res. 2004;70:117–145. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2003.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Kubota M., van Haren N.E., Haijma S.V., Schnack H.G., Cahn W., Hulshoff Pol H.E., Kahn R.S. Association of IQ Changes and Progressive Brain Changes in Patients With Schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:803–812. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0712. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Vidal C.N., Rapoport J.L., Hayashi K.M., Geaga J.A., Sui Y., McLemore L.E., Alaghband Y., Giedd J.N., Gochman P., Blumenthal J., et al. Dynamically spreading frontal and cingulate deficits mapped in adolescents with schizophrenia. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 2006;63:25–34. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.1.25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Yildiz M., Borgwardt S.J., Berger G.E. Parietal lobes in schizophrenia: Do they matter? Schizophr. Res. Treat. 2011;2011:581686. doi: 10.1155/2011/581686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Thompson P.M., Vidal C., Giedd J.N., Gochman P., Blumenthal J., Nicolson R., Toga A.W., Rapoport J.L. Mapping adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2001;98:11650–11655. doi: 10.1073/pnas.201243998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Reitan R.M., Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery. Neuropsychology Press; Tuscon, AZ, USA: 1985. [Google Scholar]
- 74.Golden C.J. STROOP: Test de Colores y Palabras. TEA Ediciones S.A; Madrid, Spain: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 75.Conners C.K. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II: Computer Program for Windows Technical Guide and Software Manual 2000. Multi-Health Systems; North Tonawanda, NY, USA: 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX, USA: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Heaton R.K., Chelune G.J., Talley J.L., Kay G.G., Custiss G.C. Test de Clasificación de Tarjetas de Wisconsin. TEA Ediciones S.A; Madrid, Spain: 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 78.Benton A.L., deS. Hamsher K., Sivan A.B. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates; Lowa City, IA, USA: 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Reig S., Sánchez-González J., Arango C., Castro J., González-Pinto A., Ortuño F., Crespo-Facorro B., Bargalló N., Desco M. Assessment of the increase in variability when combining volumetric data from different scanners. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:355–368. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Reig S., Moreno C., Moreno D., Burdalo M., Janssen J., Parellada M., Zabala A., Desco M., Arango C. Progression of brain volume changes in adolescent-onset psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2009;35:233–243. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Andreasen N.C., Rajarethinam R., Cizadlo T., Arndt S., Swayze V.W., 2nd, Flashman L.A., O’Leary D.S., Ehrhardt J.C., Yuh W.T. Automatic atlas-based volume estimation of human brain regions from MR images. J. Comput. Assist. Tomogr. 1996;20:98–106. doi: 10.1097/00004728-199601000-00018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Kates W.R., Warsofsky I.S., Patwardhan A., Abrams M.T., Liu A.M., Naidu S., Kaufmann W.E., Reiss A.L. Automated Talairach atlas-based parcellation and measurement of cerebral lobes in children. Psychiatry Res. 1999;91:11–30. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(99)00011-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Ashburner J., Friston K. Multimodal image coregistration and partitioning—A unified framework. Neuroimage. 1997;6:209–217. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1997.0290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Desco M., Pascau J., Reig S., Gispert J.D., Santos A., Benito C., Molina V., Garcia-Barreno P. Multimodality image quantification using Talairach grid. In: Hanson K.M., Sonka M., editors. Proceedings of the SPIE—The International Society for Optical Engineering. Society of Photo-optical Instrumentation Engineers; San Diego, CA, USA: 2001. pp. 18–22. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Talairach J., Tournoux P. Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme Medical Publishers; New York, NY, USA: 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Reig S., Parellada M., Castro-Fornieles J., Janssen J., Moreno D., Baeza I., Bargalló N., González-Pinto A., Graell M., Ortuño F., et al. Multicenter study of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescent-onset psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2011;37:1270–1280. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.James A.C., Javaloyes A., James S., Smith D.M. Evidence for non-progressive changes in adolescent-onset schizophrenia: Follow-up magnetic resonance imaging study. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2002;180:339–344. doi: 10.1192/bjp.180.4.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.James A.C., James S., Smith D.M., Javaloyes A. Cerebellar, prefrontal cortex, and thalamic volumes over two time points in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2004;161:1023–1029. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Bledowski C., Rahm B., Rowe J.B. What “works” in working memory? Separate systems for selection and updating of critical information. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:13735–13741. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2547-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Culham J.C., Kanwisher N.G. Neuroimaging of cognitive functions in human parietal cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001;11:157–163. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00191-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Shomstein S. Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: Top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2012;6:38. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2012.00038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Jódar-Vicente M. Cognitive functions of the frontal lobe. Rev. Neurol. 2004;39:178–182. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Fuster J.M. Frontal lobe and cognitive development. J. Neurocytol. 2002;31:373–385. doi: 10.1023/A:1024190429920. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Clarke J.M. Neuropsychology. 2nd ed. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2013. Neuroanatomy: Brain Structure and Function. [Google Scholar]
- 95.Lezak M.D., Howieson D.B., Bigler E.D., Tranel D. Neuropsychological Assessment. 5th ed. Oxford University; Oxford, UK: 2012. p. 1161. [Google Scholar]
- 96.SStuss D.T., Knight R.T. Oxford Medicine. Oxford University Press; New York, NY, USA: 2013. Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. [Google Scholar]
- 97.Stuss D.T., Alexander M.P. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychol. Res. 2000;63:289–298. doi: 10.1007/s004269900007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Stuss D.T., Gallup G.G., Jr., Alexander M.P. The frontal lobes are necessary for ‘theory of mind’. Brain. 2001;124:279–286. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.2.279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Stuss D.T., Murphy K.J., Binns M.A., Alexander M.P. Staying on the job: The frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain. 2003;126:2363–2380. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Honey G.D., Fletcher P.C. Investigating principles of human brain function underlying working memory: What insights from schizophrenia? Neuroscience. 2006;139:59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.05.036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Gutiérrez-Galve L., Wheeler-Kingshott C.A., Altmann D.R., Price G., Chu E.M., Leeson V.C., Lobo A., Barker G.J., Barnes T.R., Joyce E.M., et al. Changes in the Frontotemporal Cortex and Cognitive Correlates in First-Episode Psychosis. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;68:51–60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Haring L., Müürsepp A., Mõttus R., Ilves P., Koch K., Uppin K., Tarnovskaja J., Maron E., Zharkovsky A., Vasar E. Cortical thickness and surface area correlates with cognitive dysfunction among first-episode psychosis patients. Psychol. Med. 2016;46:2145–2155. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716000684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Ehrlich S., Brauns S., Yendiki A., Ho B.-C., Calhoun V., Schulz S.C., Gollub R.L., Sponheim S.R. Associations of cortical thickness and cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophr. Bull. 2012;38:1050–1062. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Crespo-Facorro B., Roiz-Santiáñez R., Pérez-Iglesias R., Rodriguez-Sanchez J., Mata I., Tordesillas-Gutierrez D., Sanchez E., Tabarés-Seisdedos R., Andreasen N., Magnotta V. Global and regional cortical thinning in first-episode psychosis patients: Relationships with clinical and cognitive features. Psychol. Med. 2011;41:1449. doi: 10.1017/S003329171000200X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Hatton S.N., Lagopoulos J., Hermens D.F., Scott E., Hickie I.B., Bennett M.R. Cortical thinning in young psychosis and bipolar patients correlate with common neurocognitive deficits. Int. J. Bipolar Disord. 2013;1:1–13. doi: 10.1186/2194-7511-1-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Case R. Intellectual Development: Birth to Adulthood. Academic Press; London, UK: 1985. p. 480. [Google Scholar]
- 107.Conway A.R., Kane M.J., Engle R.W. Working memory capacity and its relation to general intelligence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2003;7:547–552. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Gathercole S.E., Lamont E., Alloway T.P. Working Memory and Education. Academic Press; Cambridge, MA, USA: 2006. Working memory in the classroom; pp. 219–240. [Google Scholar]
- 109.Spencer J.P. The development of working memory. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2020;29:545–553. doi: 10.1177/0963721420959835. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Cowan N. Working Memory Maturation: Can We Get at the Essence of Cognitive Growth? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2016;11:239–264. doi: 10.1177/1745691615621279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Kail R. Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psychol. Bull. 1991;109:490–501. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.3.490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Edin F., Klingberg T., Johansson P., McNab F., Tegnér J., Compte A. Mechanism for top-down control of working memory capacity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2009;106:6802–6807. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0901894106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Solé-Padullés C., Castro-Fornieles J., de la Serna E., Calvo R., Baeza I., Moya J., Lázaro L., Rosa M., Bargalló N., Sugranyes G. Intrinsic connectivity networks from childhood to late adolescence: Effects of age and sex. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016;17:35–44. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Alemán-Gómez Y., Janssen J., Schnack H., Balaban E., Pina-Camacho L., Alfaro-Almagro F., Castro-Fornieles J., Otero S., Baeza I., Moreno D., et al. The human cerebral cortex flattens during adolescence. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:15004–15010. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1459-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Pina-Camacho L., Del Rey-Mejías Á., Janssen J., Bioque M., González-Pinto A., Arango C., Lobo A., Sarró S., Desco M., Sanjuan J., et al. Age at First Episode Modulates Diagnosis-Related Structural Brain Abnormalities in Psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2016;42:344–357. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Goldberg T.E., Keefe R.S., Goldman R.S., Robinson D.G., Harvey P.D. Circumstances under which practice does not make perfect: A review of the practice effect literature in schizophrenia and its relevance to clinical treatment studies. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1053–1062. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Rothman K.J. No adjustments are needed for multiple comparisons. Epidemiology. 1990;1:43–46. doi: 10.1097/00001648-199001000-00010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Freise R.J. Do multiple outcome measures require p-value adjustment? BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2002;2:8. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-2-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Althouse A.D. Adjust for Multiple Comparisons? It’s Not That Simple. Ann. Thorac. Surg. 2016;101:1644–1645. doi: 10.1016/j.athoracsur.2015.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Armstrong R.A. When to use the Bonferroni correction. Ophthalmic Physiol. Opt. 2014;34:502–508. doi: 10.1111/opo.12131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Perneger T.V. What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments. BMJ. 1998;316:1236. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Rothman K.J., Greenland S., Lash T.L. Modern Epidemiology Third, Mid-Cycle Revision Edition. Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins; Philadelphia, PA, USA: 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Vinogradov S., Fisher M., de Villers-Sidani E. Cognitive training for impaired neural systems in neuropsychiatric illness. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:43–76. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.251. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.