Table 5.
Study | Year | n | Age | Sex | Region | PA Instrument | Wave | Correlation Coefficient/ Effect Size |
Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Barratt et al. [95] | 1997 | 101 | 26 | M | North America | Offending record and semi-structured interview |
P3 | na | P3 amplitudes differed between impulsive and non-impulsive offender groups, but the groups did not differ in clinically-rated impulsivity. |
Stanford et al. [96] | 2003 | 28 | 33 | M/F | North America | Offendingrecord | P3 | na | P3 amplitudes for the PA group took marginally longer to peak in response to auditory stimuli. |
Chen et al. [97] | 2015 | 24 | 30 | M | Asia | Offending record |
P3 | d = 1.33 | P3 amplitudes in the PA group showed less interference to sad cues. |
Helfritz-Sinville and Stanford [98] | 2015 | 58 | 19 | M | North America | LHAQ IPAS |
P3 | na | P3 amplitudes showed less efficient processing of threat cues for the PA group. |
Chen et al. [99] | 2019 | 38 | 17 | M | Asia | RPQ TAP |
N2 |
r = 0.52 r = 0.12 |
PA was associated with reduced N2 wave during the decision phase of the TAP. |
Statistical findings shown in bold are significant according to p-values < 0.05; r, Pearson correlation coefficient; d, Cohen’s d measure of effect size; na, not applicable; LHAQ, Lifetime History of Aggression Questionnaire [100]; IPAS, Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scales [101]; TAP, Taylor Aggression Paradigm [26].