Abstract
As the population ages, understanding how to maintain older adults' cognitive abilities is essential. Bilingualism has been linked to higher cognitive reserve, better performance in executive control, changes in brain structure and function relative to monolinguals, and delay in dementia onset. Learning a second language thus seems a promising avenue for cognitive enhancement in older adults. Our review aims to determine whether learning a foreign language in later life improves cognition and promotes neuroplasticity. We screened articles from the Pubmed, Scopus, and Science Direct databases to identify interventional studies using second language training in senior participants, including either cognition or neuroimaging as outcome measures. A total of nine articles were found, with only one neuroimaging study. Results from these studies are inconsistent, but tend to suggest that second language learning is associated with improvement in attentional switching, inhibition, working memory, and increased functional connectivity. We discuss the implications of these results, and suggest new directions and methodological recommendations for future research.
Keywords: older adults, second language learning (L2 learning), executive functioning, neuroplasticity, cognitive reserve
Introduction
With the population aging, incidence rates of dementia are on the rise. The World Health Organization's website predicts that by 2050, 152 million people could be living with dementia (World Health Organization, 2020). As effective drug treatments have yet to be developed, non-pharmacological interventions are currently the most viable option for preventing, or at least delaying, neurodegenerative diseases.
These interventions aim to boost or maintain cognitive and brain reserve (Stern, 2012). Overall, reserve refers to one's ability to better resist or cope with the accumulation of age-related or disease-related alterations. More specifically, the concept of cognitive reserve has been developed to account for interindividual variability in older adults' susceptibility to cognitive decline. For example, research has shown that some older adults are able to withstand greater brain atrophy before exhibiting cognitive deficits (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2006). Higher reserve would support this greater resilience. Yet, it should be noted that reserve is a complex and multicomponent construct, and there is currently no consensus on its specific definition (Cabeza et al., 2018; Stern et al., 2020).
Nevertheless, research on the subject of reserve is abundant. Cognitive reserve and successful aging have been associated with lifestyle factors, including education (Stern, 2012), participation in leisure and social activities (Scarmeas and Stern, 2003), or physical exercise (Cheng, 2016). Of importance, cognitive reserve is not a static capacity, and is considered to be modifiable throughout life (Tucker and Stern, 2011).
Bilingualism, or the capacity to speak two languages fluently, has also been shown to contribute to cognitive reserve (Schweizer et al., 2012). Relevantly, dual-language use has been associated with executive functioning benefits, which have been observed in bilingual children, adults, and elders, even when the second language is acquired after childhood (Costa and Sebastián-Gallés, 2014). However, these cognitive advantages are debated, as some studies have failed to show differences in cognition between monolingual and bilingual adults (Paap et al., 2014; von Bastian et al., 2016; Nichols et al., 2020). Interestingly, differences may be more visible in older adults, as young adults already function at their peak (Bialystok et al., 2005). Thus, research focusing on bilingual older adults may shed more light on the cognitive benefits associated with bilingualism. Notably, it has been shown that lifelong use of more than one language could lead to enhanced cognition in later life (Bialystok et al., 2004). A few studies conducted in elderly individuals have evidenced an advantage in episodic memory, letter fluency (Ljungberg et al., 2013), semantic verbal fluency (Rosselli et al., 2000), as well as higher general intelligence (Bak et al., 2014a) in bilingual seniors. Moreover, many studies have revealed superior executive functioning in older bilinguals, notably in auditory attention tests (Bak et al., 2014b), cognitive inhibition, and task switching (Bialystok et al., 2006; Goral et al., 2015; Blumenfeld et al., 2016). These benefits are found primarily in tests like the Stroop, Flanker, and Simon tasks, in which colors, shapes, or arrows must be processed and selected, and others ignored or suppressed. In a meta-analysis including 28 articles (Armstrong et al., 2019), bilingualism was found to have a significant effect on seniors' cognitive inhibition. As cognitive inhibition, among other executive functions, is usually weakened with age (Hejazi et al., 2019), this finding suggests that speaking more than one language has a modulating effect on age-related cognitive decline in bilinguals.
Neuroimaging studies have also reported differences in brain measures between monolingual and bilingual older adults, showing higher gray matter volume (GMV) in the anterior temporal lobe and in the left inferior temporal gyrus in bilinguals, which correlated to second language naming ability (Abutalebi et al., 2014), as well as greater GMV in the anterior cingulate cortex (Abutalebi et al., 2015a; Del Maschio et al., 2018), and in the caudate nucleus, pre-frontal cortex, and inferior frontal cortex (Del Maschio et al., 2018). Higher GMV in the left and right inferior parietal lobule was also observed in older bilinguals compared to monolinguals, and this was correlated with naming ability and language exposure respectively (Abutalebi et al., 2015b). Using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), greater axial diffusivity has also been detected in older bilinguals compared to monolinguals, specifically in the left superior longitudinal fasciculus (Anderson et al., 2018), along with greater fractional anisotropy in the corpus callosum, and the superior and inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Luk et al., 2011). Moreover, relative to older monolinguals, greater GMV and white matter volume in older bilinguals has been shown to correlate with more efficient executive functioning, as evidenced with superior inhibition and attentional performance, while no such correlation was found in monolingual groups (Olsen et al., 2015; Borsa et al., 2018).
Functional connectivity or activation differences between senior monolinguals and bilinguals have also been observed. With fMRI scans carried out during executive control tasks, greater functional connectivity in the frontoparietal control and default mode networks was found in older bilinguals relative to their monolingual peers (Grady et al., 2015). Moreover, older bilinguals switched faster than their monolingual peers during perceptual tasks, and fMRI imaging evidenced less activation in the left dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex, left ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex, and the anterior cingulate cortex, suggesting higher neural efficiency in bilingual seniors (Gold et al., 2013).
Altogether, cross-sectional studies suggest that bilingualism is likely to increase cognitive and brain reserve as it is associated with a beneficial effect on cognitive performance and cerebral integrity, notably in older adults. Despite the inherent discrepancies between lifelong dual-language use and later second language acquisition (SLA), bilingualism and SLA share important characteristics, notably considering the fact that learning a second language is a step toward bilingualism. Learning a second language at a later age may not lead to bilingualism, yet learning to use a foreign language could provide some cognitive benefits that may partly overlap with those associated with bilingualism. It has thus been hypothesized that SLA in older adults could be a promising avenue of cognitive training to promote healthy aging (Antoniou et al., 2013; Antoniou and Wright, 2017).
However, there is a common stereotype that SLA would be too challenging for older adults (Gómez, 2016). This perhaps stems from the widely cited critical period hypothesis, which postulates that there is a short window of time during childhood for successful language acquisition; but this hypothesis was developed in the context of first language acquisition, and does not necessarily apply to SLA (Singleton and Pfenninger, 2018). In fact, it has been shown that learning a new language, although more difficult after adolescence, is possible for older adults (Gómez, 2016; Kliesch et al., 2017; Hejazi et al., 2019). With age-related cognitive decline being gradual and varying greatly from person to person, more than a “critical” period, the notion of age-related changes affecting SLA could be better characterized as a “sensitive” period (Birdsong, 2018). Amongst the predictors of SLA ability in older adults, working memory capacity, over chronological age, has been shown to be a better predictor of second language success in seniors (Mackey and Sachs, 2012). What's more, implicit learning abilities, as opposed to those of explicit learning, have been shown to be more resistant to aging (Polony et al., 2016), and even improve with age (Ristin-Kaufmann and Gullberg, 2014), which could facilitate SLA in older adults.
Parting from the postulate that SLA in older adults is indeed possible, and based on previous evidence indicating a beneficial effect of bilingualism on older adults' reserve and resilience, this review aims to address the influence of late natural language learning on cognitive and brain aging. Artificial language interventions, although valuable in their own right, will not be included in this review as they do not share the same socio-cultural components as those of natural language. Moreover, although artificial and second language learning processes are related (Friederici et al., 2002), some research suggests that they depend on different mechanisms (Robinson, 2010), and therefore may affect the brain differently.
Moreover, learning a natural language has practical and social implications. Beyond the cognitive benefits, learning a foreign language in later life could be particularly enriching from a psychological perspective, as it could provide an outlet for socialization (Pfenninger and Singleton, 2019), opportunities for travel (Antoniou et al., 2013), intercultural communication (Kuklewicz and King, 2018), as well as a means of building self-esteem (Pot et al., 2018), and well-being (Matsumoto, 2019), which could in turn have positive effects on cognition (Allerhand et al., 2014).
While studies are scarce, some research has evaluated the cognitive and neuroanatomic effects of SLA through interventional designs in those over 60; yet results seem inconsistent. Therefore, it remains unclear whether learning a new language in late adulthood fosters cognitive efficiency and neuroplasticity. Our systematic review thus aims to clarify the effects of SLA on older adults' cognitive and cerebral functioning, by reviewing available evidence and analyzing the methodological quality of longitudinal studies with SLA interventions for seniors. We will present the results of our systematic review and discuss their implications in the context of both cognitive improvement or maintenance, and neuroplasticity associated with SLA. We will then outline some methodological shortcomings that should be taken into account for future studies, and finally address future directions based on ongoing studies or research protocols targeting the cognitive and neurological effects of language learning in seniors.
Methods
Search Strategy
Following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) model, we conducted the literature search using the following key word combinations in the Pubmed, Scopus and Science Direct databases up until November 12th, 2020: “language learning”/“second language”/“foreign language”/“second language learning”/“foreign language learning”/and “older adults”/“seniors”/“elderly”/“third age.” In each of the databases searched, the filter for article type was used, excluding review articles, book chapters, conference papers, and abstracts. For Science Direct and Scopus, when using the keyword “seniors,” “high school,” “college,” and “university,” the AND NOT command was used in order to avoid studies with younger adults. Four additional articles were identified through ResearchGate. See Table 1 for details.
Table 1.
Search engine | Keywords |
---|---|
Pubmed | (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“language learning” OR “second language” OR “foreign language” OR “foreign language learning” OR “second language learning”))) |
Science direct | (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“second language”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“foreign language”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“foreign language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“second language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) |
Scopus | (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“second language”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“foreign language”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“foreign language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) (((“older adults” OR “elderly” OR “seniors” OR “third age”))) AND (((“second language learning”))) AND NOT (((“high school” OR “university” OR “college”))) |
Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We selected studies based on the following criteria: (1) peer-reviewed original research; (2) including healthy participants over 60; (3) comprising a longitudinal second language intervention; (4) providing at least one outcome measure of cognitive functioning or neuroimaging; (5) published before the 12th of November, 2020. Studies with clinical populations, or those including only participants under 60, non-natural language interventions, exclusively qualitative data, written in a language other than English, as well as reviews, research protocols, abstracts, and preprints were excluded. See Figure 1 for details.
Risk of Bias
In order to analyze the risk of bias (RoB) of the studies included in this review, we used the Study Quality Assessment Tools from the US Department of Health and Human Services (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools). Two independent raters (CW and SD) evaluated the studies with the Quality Assessment Tool for Before-After (Pre-Post) Studies with No Control Group or the Quality Assessment of Controlled Intervention Studies, depending on the study design. In case of inconsistencies between the two raters, results were further deliberated until agreeing upon a common score. RoB in the following part of this review will be discussed in terms of “methodological quality,” with high RoB corresponding to poor-quality studies and, reversely, low RoB corresponding to good quality studies.
For controlled intervention studies (randomized and non-randomized), there were 14 questions related to the study's randomization, blinding, baseline comparability of the experimental and control groups, drop-out and adherence to the assigned interventions, validation and pre-specification of outcome measures before analysis, sample size, and intention-to-treat analysis. A numerical scoring method was adopted, as previously determined by Cotelli et al. (2020), where the number of affirmative responses counted for one point and the sum of all responses was classified as follows: scores from 1 to 4 were rated as poor, 5–9 as fair, and 10–14 as good.
For interventional studies without a control group, there were 12 questions related to the clarity of the study's objectives, their selection criteria, the representability of their sample and its size, eligible persons' participation rate, the description and delivery of the intervention, the pre-specification and validation of outcome measures, assessors' blinding, drop-out percentage, pre-and post-statistical testing, and time-series design. The studies' RoB was scored using the same method, i.e., the sum of affirmative responses. The twelfth and final question of this scale concerned studies conducted on a group level such as a whole community or entire hospital patient population. Therefore, as this question did not apply to any of the included studies, we disregarded it, as did Cotelli et al. (2020). Thus, studies with scores from 1 to 4 were rated as poor, 5–8 as fair, and 9–11 as good. Full evaluations are provided in Supplementary Tables 1, 2.
Results
General Characteristics of Included Studies
After removing duplicates, we reviewed the pertinence of the 462 articles retrieved (Figure 1). After removing impertinent articles based on their titles, the abstracts and full texts of the remaining 54 articles were analyzed and studies were excluded if they were reviews (8), protocols (2), not written in English (1), as well as if they did not include cognitive outcome measures (24) or language interventions (3), only used artificial language training (5), did not include healthy older adults (1), or had the same participant sample and outcome cognitive measure of another included study (1). At the end of this selection process, only nine articles met our inclusion criteria.
Amongst the selected articles, eight included only cognitive outcome measures and one used both neuroimaging and cognitive outcome measures (Bubbico et al., 2019). A summary of the studies' participants and assessments is listed in Table 2, SLA and control interventions are detailed in Table 3, and the main outcomes and quality assessment scores are listed in Table 4.
Table 2.
Study | Country | Sample N | Groups (N and group) | Sex M/F | Age | Cognitive tests | SLA Test |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Bak et al. (2016) | Scotland | 67 | 33 SL 16 active control 18 passive control |
NR | 18–78 | TEA and its 3 subtests. | LBQ |
Berggren et al. (2020) | Sweden | 160 | 90 SL 70 control | 60/100 | 65–75 | Raven's matrices, the WASI-II, verbal intelligence, working and long-term associative memory tasks. N-back and numerical working memory test. | Vocabulary test |
Bubbico et al. (2019) | Italy | 26 | 14 SL 12 control | 7/19 | 59–79 | MMSE, VFT, TMTa and b, TMTab, the BMT, and FAB. | None |
Klimova et al. (2020) | Czech Republic | 42 | 20 SL 22 control | 6/36 | 55–77 | MoCA | Lex-tale |
Long et al. (2019) | Scotland | 105 | NA | 38/65 | 21–85 | TEA and its 3 subtests. | LBQ |
Pfenninger and Polz (2018) | Austria | 12 | NA | 4/8 | 63–90 | Stroop, A-K-T | C-test |
Ramos et al. (2017) | Spain | 43 | 26 SL 17 control | 22/21 | 60–80 | Switching paradigm test | Lex-tale |
Ware et al. (2017) | France | 14 | NA | 5/9 | 63–90 | MoCA | None |
Wong et al. (2019) | China | 153 | 53 SL 51 games 49 music |
23/130 | 60–85 | ADAS-Cog, the auditory reading span, and the backward digit span tests, ANT, forward digit span, and Simon Task. | None |
SL, second language; NR, not reported; NA, non-applicable; TEA, Test of Everyday Attention; LBQ, Language Background Questionnaire; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; ADAS-Cog, Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale96Cognitive Subscale; ANT, Attention Network Test; BMT, Babcock Memory Test; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; VFT, Verbal Fluency Test; A-K-T, Geriatric Concentration Test.
Table 3.
Study | Control group(s) | Description of SLA intervention | Duration, frequency, and total hours |
---|---|---|---|
Bak et al. (2016) | Other intensive university courses Passive group |
Summer intensive Gaelic course and additional evening entertainment (films, concerts, and conversation) at the National Center for Gaelic Language and Culture. | 1 week, 14 h total. |
Berggren et al. (2020) | Relaxation | Customized Italian course at an adult senior center, using a traditional text book (Olsson and Braconi, 2005) with grammar and verbal exercises, along with weekly vocabulary lists for memorization. | 11 weeks, 2 weekly classes, 5 h/week, 55 h total. |
Bubbico et al. (2019) | Passive | English language program with native teacher focusing on basic vocabulary skills and grammar, speaking and writing skills, including American and British culture, through classroom, homework, and team projects. | 4 months, 1 90 min weekly class with 30 min homework, 32 h total. |
Klimova et al. (2020) | Passive | Customized language course employing both traditional (vocabulary drilling) and non-traditional methods (problem solving and mind-mapping). | 12 weeks, 3 weekly 45 min lessons, 27 h total. |
Long et al. (2019) | NA | Summer intensive Gaelic course and additional and evening entertainment (films, concerts, and conversation) at the National Center for Gaelic Language and Culture. | 1 week, 14 h total. |
Pfenninger and Polz (2018) | NA | Intensive English course using the Headway A1 (Soars and Soars, 2007) textbook, focusing on the understanding and use of everyday expressions, as well as vocabulary training. | 4 weeks, 3 weekly 2-h sessions, 24 h total. |
Ramos et al. (2017) | Passive | Basque at a Center for continuing education with native Basque-Spanish instructors, specialized in teaching adult learners. | 8 months, 3 sessions per week, 176 h total. |
Ware et al. (2017) | NA | Customized English class with media, and tablet exercises, focusing on oral comprehension and translation. | 4 months, 2-h weekly sessions, 32 h total. |
Wong et al. (2019) | Games (active) Music (passive) |
English with Rosetta Stone Version 4, at a community center, along with bi-monthly group activities with English practice in a social setting. | 6 months, 5 h per week, 120 h total. |
Table 4.
Study | Study design | Time points measured | Outcome | RoB |
---|---|---|---|---|
Bak et al. (2016) | CT | Baseline, a week later, and 9 months follow-up | Significant improvement in attentional switching (subtest 3) after just 1 week of SL training compared to passive controls, and maintenance of scores if Gaelic was practiced more than 5 h per week at follow-up 9 months later. All Gaelic learners showed significant improvement, regardless of age group. | 7/14 Fair |
Berggren et al. (2020) | RCT | Baseline and 11 weeks later | No significant differences were found between the SL group and the relaxation group, even though the SL group did demonstrate success in learning Italian vocabulary. | 8/14 Fair |
Bubbico et al. (2019) | RCT | Baseline and 4 months later | The MMS scores of the SL group remained stable, but the control group's scores significantly decreased. No other significant differences in cognition were found, although the SL group's functional connectivity increased in the right inferior and superior frontal gyrus, as well as in the left superior parietal lobule. | 7/14 Fair |
Klimova et al. (2020) | RCT | Baseline and 12 weeks later | Increases in MoCA scores were revealed in some of the participants in the SL group, yet they did not meet significance. Yet, decreases in scores in the SL group were also revealed, as well as increases in scores of the control group. | 6/14 Fair |
Long et al. (2019) | No CG | Baseline, and a week later | The TEA tests were found to be significantly correlated to Gaelic level. After the course, the beginner group evidenced the most improvement in TEA scores. Comparisons were made for different age groups, and all groups improved significantly in attentional switching and inhibition subtests 2 and 3. | 5/11 Fair |
Pfenninger and Polz (2018) | No CG | Baseline and 4 weeks later | Significantly improved Stroop and A-K-T scores for monolinguals, and significantly less errors on the language proficiency C-test. Qualitative results showed a positive effect on social life, improved memory, and boosted well-being. | 8/11 Fair |
Ramos et al. (2017) | CT | Baseline and 8 months later | No significant increases in switching ability among the intervention group were found in their test designed for the study. | 3/14 Poor |
Ware et al. (2017) | No CG | Baseline and 4 months later | No significant differences in MoCa and UCLA between pre-and post-intervention. | 6/11 Fair |
Wong et al. (2019) | RCT | Baseline and 6 months later and 3-month follow-up | Significant improvement was found for the active intervention groups, with the English group significantly improving in the ADAS-Cog, the auditory reading span, and the backward digit span tests. Yet the ANT, the forward digit span test, and the Simon task scores did not reach significance. | 8/14 Fair |
For the quality assessment scores, it should be noted that the studies without control groups were scored over 11 points, while those with control groups were assessed with a 14-point scale. CT, Controlled trial; RCT, randomized controlled trial; CG, control group; SL, second language; MMS, Mini Mental State; TEA, Test of Everyday Attention; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; ADAS-Cog, Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale -Cognitive Subscale; ANT, Attention Network Test; BMT, Babcock Memory Test; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; VFT, Verbal Fluency Test; A-K-T, Geriatric Concentration Test; UCLA, University of California Loneliness Assessment.
The nine studies included in this review have a total of 622 participants, aged 18–90. Two studies included a large sample of adults 18 and over, including subgroups of older participants over 60 (Bak et al., 2016; Long et al., 2019), while the other studies only recruited older adults. Six studies had a control group, four of which were randomized. In these studies, the control groups included either passive controls that did not change their habits (Ramos et al., 2017; Bubbico et al., 2019; Berggren et al., 2020; Klimova et al., 2020) or active controls involved either in other university courses (Bak et al., 2016) or online games like Sudoku and crossword puzzles (Wong et al., 2019). Of note, these two last studies had both active and passive control groups, although in the former they were not assigned randomly, as participants chose their intervention group. The remaining three studies did not have control groups (Ware et al., 2017; Pfenninger and Polz, 2018; Long et al., 2019).
Six studies used English with Chinese, Czech, Austrian, French, and Italian participants, two used Gaelic with Scottish participants, one used Basque with Spanish participants, and another used Italian with Swedish participants. The teaching methods varied from online classes to traditional classroom settings (see Table 3 for details).
The interventions' duration was very heterogeneous, ranging from 1 week to 8 months, and the intensity of training programs varied from 14 to 176 h, with an average of ~55 h.
Only two studies used standardized second language proficiency tests before and after the intervention (Ramos et al., 2017; Pfenninger and Polz, 2018). One study used a vocabulary test, but only after the intervention (Berggren et al., 2020), while the other studies did not evaluate the second language proficiency attained at posttest (Bak et al., 2016; Ware et al., 2017; Bubbico et al., 2019; Long et al., 2019; Wong et al., 2019; Klimova et al., 2020).
All of the studies had at least one cognitive outcome measure; the long-term cognitive effects of language learning were evaluated in only two studies with follow-up measures after their second language interventions either 3 months (Wong et al., 2019), or 9 months later (Bak et al., 2016).
Methodological Quality of the Studies
The nine studies fell into three different categories: randomized controlled intervention studies (n = 4), non-randomized controlled intervention studies (n = 3), and before-after (pre-post) intervention studies with no control group (n = 2); see the Methods section for details on the quality (RoB) assessment. Detailed evaluation can be consulted in Supplementary Tables 1, 2.
All studies scored as fair, except one that had a poor-quality score, or high RoB (Ramos et al., 2017). Although none of the studies had high enough scores to be considered good quality, based on the scoring guidelines of Cotelli et al. (2020), five studies scored higher than the average six points (scores ≥7), and we therefore refer to them here as the higher quality studies (Bak et al., 2016; Pfenninger and Polz, 2018; Bubbico et al., 2019; Wong et al., 2019; Berggren et al., 2020).
Cognitive Outcomes of Language Learning
Focusing specifically on the five higher quality studies with executive functioning measures, mixed results have been shown for cognitive inhibition, attentional switching, and working memory (Figure 2A); with three of the studies evidencing improvement (Bak et al., 2016; Pfenninger and Polz, 2018; Wong et al., 2019), while the other two did not (Bubbico et al., 2019; Berggren et al., 2020). Yet, it should be noted that there are many methodological differences between these studies, with variation in intervention length and intensity, as well as diversity in terms of cognitive outcome tests. Longer (Wong et al., 2019), or more intensive interventions (Bak et al., 2016; Pfenninger and Polz, 2018) yielded positive results, while shorter, less intensive interventions did not (Bubbico et al., 2019; Berggren et al., 2020). Episodic memory (including associative, verbal and non-verbal episodic memory tests) was assessed in two higher quality studies and was not found to be significantly affected by second language interventions (Bubbico et al., 2019; Berggren et al., 2020). Additionally, verbal and spatial intelligence were not shown to improve either (Berggren et al., 2020). Yet, no other study in this review tested those specific intelligence and memory capacities (see Tables 2, 4). Finally, global functioning was found to improve after the second language intervention in one study using the ADAS-Cog (Wong et al., 2019). In another study using the MMSE, performances were preserved in the second language intervention group while they declined in the control group (Bubbico et al., 2019). In fact, the control and experimental groups' MMSE scores significantly differed at baseline, with the control group's scores significantly higher than those of the experimental group. However, with the decrease of the control group's scores and the maintenance of the intervention group's scores over time, there was no longer a significant group difference of scores at post-intervention.
When examining the lower quality studies (Figure 2A), one study showed significant improvement in cognitive scores, specifically in cognitive inhibition and attentional switching (Long et al., 2019), while the other three studies did not evidence cognitive improvement. In the lowest quality study, an original paradigm of switching, which was designed for the study, did not reveal changes in this cognitive function (Ramos et al., 2017). Additionally, some lower quality studies used brief tests of global functioning, like the MoCA (Ware et al., 2017; Klimova et al., 2020) and found no effect of the intervention. While these tests are validated and pertinent measures for patient populations, they may not be sensitive enough to capture subtle changes in cognition for healthy participants; these may be measured with more sensitive tests of global cognition such as the ADAS-Cog (see above).
Language Learning and Functional Brain Connectivity
Only one study used neuroimaging outcome measures (Bubbico et al., 2019; Figure 2B). This higher quality study evidenced increased functional connectivity at post-intervention, both when compared to baseline connectivity and to controls, in the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG), the right superior frontal gyrus (rSFG), and the left superior parietal lobule (lSPL), while maintaining MMSE scores. Moreover, the increased functional connectivity in the rSFG was associated with increased global cognitive functioning scores, in the experimental group only.
Discussion
Summary of Main Results
Overall, our systematic review highlights mixed results regarding the cognitive effects of second language training in older adults (Figure 2A). As there are very few studies published on the subject to date, firm conclusions cannot yet be drawn. Nonetheless, four of the five studies with the highest quality scores (i.e., with the lowest RoB) did show significant increases in attentional switching (Bak et al., 2016), cognitive inhibition (Pfenninger and Polz, 2018), working memory (Wong et al., 2019), or functional connectivity (Bubbico et al., 2019), therefore providing some moderate evidence for increases in cognitive and cerebral functioning after short-term second language training in seniors. Nevertheless, in terms of methodological quality, none of the studies scored high enough to be considered good quality; therefore, the results should be interpreted with caution.
Cognitive Effects of Second Language Learning in Seniors
When tallying the number of articles that show a positive effect of language learning interventions on cognition, the evidence for increases in scores of attentional switching and cognitive inhibition is the strongest, improvement in working memory and general cognitive functioning is moderate, while no indication of an effect on intelligence, verbal episodic memory or verbal fluency has been shown. These results can be interpreted in the context of bilingualism research as discussed in what follows.
Improvement in cognitive inhibition after second language learning is consistent with dual language processing. Cognitive control advantages associated with bilingualism are hypothesized to spring from having to juggle representations of two different languages in one's mind. The dual activation theory posits that while a representation of one language is activated, its correlate in the other language is activated at the same time. Thus, a bilingual would constantly have to inhibit one language while using the other (Green, 1998). Interference inhibition is thus essential for bilingual language use (Antoniou and Wright, 2017). This could explain the increases in cognitive inhibition scores in two studies (Pfenninger and Polz, 2018; Long et al., 2019), as learning a new language requires the inhibition of one's mother tongue (Long et al., 2019).
Likewise, the lifelong use of more than one language has also been shown to affect switching capacity (López Zunini et al., 2019), and this was also found to improve after language learning in two studies (Bak et al., 2016; Long et al., 2019). Yet, it could be argued that as the language learning interventions of these studies are quite short, practice effects could be responsible for the improvement at post-test. Nonetheless, after 9 months, the scores of attentional switching were maintained only for participants who practiced the second language more than 5 h per week (Bak et al., 2016), therefore suggesting that the results were not due to test-retest effects but to the amount of second language practice.
The lack of increases in verbal skills could also be interpreted in the context of bilingualism. Bilinguals have been shown to suffer from more tip-of-the-tongue states (Bialystok et al., 2008; Pyers et al., 2009), and to have smaller vocabularies in each of their tongues, as they simply have less time to master new vocabulary in each language (Gollan et al., 2009). Thus, verbal memory, verbal fluency, and verbal intelligence may not greatly increase through second language training.
Participants' previous language experience can influence the benefits of second language interventions, as suggested by the greater improvement of attentional switching in beginners compared to intermediate and advanced second language learners, evidenced in Long et al. (2019). This is in line with the “adaptive control hypothesis,” which posits that to adapt to more complex language contexts, control processes are required and these higher demands on the executive control system will lead, eventually, to improvement in executive functioning (Green and Abutalebi, 2013). Therefore, the pre-training language level, as well as the intensity of second language training, are important factors to consider.
Moreover, participants' language status, in terms of being mono-, bi-, or multi-lingual, is also of consequential significance, as those who speak more than one language may already benefit from the cognitive advantages associated with speaking multiple tongues. Learning a new language in later life may be most beneficial for those with no second language experience at all. This is consistent with the absence of increases in cognitive scores in studies with Swedish participants who demonstrated working knowledge of English (Berggren et al., 2020), or with bilingual participants (Pfenninger and Polz, 2018).
Overall, there is some variability in the effects of SLA interventions on cognition. However, studies with the highest quality tend to demonstrate a significant improvement in some executive functions.
Nonetheless, it should be noted that second language learning in older adults is not equivalent to lifelong bilingualism, and the cognitive advantages associated with later language learning may not be as pronounced in those who are only just starting to learn a new language than in those who have juggled different tongues from a much earlier age. Further research is needed to establish whether SLA in late life could actually contribute to cognitive reserve, as it has been suggested for lifelong bilingualism.
Functional Brain Correlates of Second Language Learning
Whereas, behavioral measures do not always show significant improvement after SLA interventions, the only neuroimaging study conducted on older adults suggests that the effects of learning a new language can be detectable in the brain. After second language learning, connectivity has been shown to increase in the rIFG, rSFG, and lSPL (Bubbico et al., 2019). Interestingly, these regions have been shown to be involved in both the executive control and language networks (Tops and Boksem, 2011; Shomstein, 2012; Hu et al., 2016). Moreover, the connectivity increases were correlated with higher scores of global cognition (i.e., MMSE) in the intervention group, perhaps revealing cognitive maintenance. In fact, the maintenance of scores after the intervention, in comparison with passive controls, could be encouraging as learning may have a beneficial influence, not only through brain growth and improvements in cognitive performance, but also by reducing age-related brain alterations and cognitive decline (Nyberg et al., 2012).
These results align with previous studies in younger adults that suggest that language learning influences brain plasticity. For instance, language learning in younger adults has been shown to affect functional connectivity (Ghazi Saidi et al., 2013), GMV and cortical thickness (Legault et al., 2019). Promisingly, studies in young adults have evidenced increases in hippocampus volume after second language training (Mårtensson et al., 2012; Bellander et al., 2016). As the hippocampus plays an important role in episodic memory, and hippocampal atrophy is widely recognized as a biomarker of Alzheimer's disease (De Flores et al., 2015), increases in its volume, as a function of SLA, could be of significance in the face of age-related atrophy and cognitive decline. Furthermore, older bilinguals have been shown to have greater left hippocampal GMV than their monolingual counterparts (Li et al., 2017).
While these studies on SLA in younger adults seem encouraging, older adults might not rely on the same neural mechanisms when learning a new language, and therefore the same brain regions may not be affected in the same manner. For example, learning second language vocabulary, as in the memorization of foreign words, has been shown to affect the brain differentially as a function of age, with older adults evidencing greater activation in the left IFG, left lingual gyrus and cuneus, and younger adults showing greater activation in the left cingulate gyrus and the left caudate nucleus (Marcotte and Ansaldo, 2014). Relative to their younger counterparts, these different mechanisms may induce different patterns of brain plasticity in seniors.
Limits
Altogether, although this review highlights some evidence for increases in executive functioning with language learning, it also points to inconsistencies, probably due to methodological differences across the different trials.
Some of the studies' methodological shortcomings should be emphasized: the lack of pertinent, sensitive, or validated cognitive measures, small sample sizes, lack of randomized active and passive control groups, absence of second language proficiency tests, as well as short or low intensity language training programs.
First of all, most of the lower quality studies scored below average due to small sample sizes or a lack of randomization. The inclusion of randomized active and passive control groups is very important for a study's credibility. This is particularly true when studying the elderly, who are more likely to decline over time. Indeed, the absence of modifications can either indicate an absence of a beneficial effect, or a maintenance of scores, which could be in fact a positive result; only the inclusion of a control group can help to unravel such results.
Secondly, it is of note that higher quality studies tended to use more than one cognitive outcome test and generally found a positive effect of language learning. In contrast, studies that were rated as lower quality used only one cognitive outcome measure, such as the MoCA, a brief test of global cognitive functioning, which greatly limited the possibility of capturing subtle cognitive changes. Another point is the choice of cognitive measures. Berggren et al.'s (2020) study did not show a significant effect on cognition, but they did not use tests that measure cognitive inhibition and switching, which have previously been shown to be affected by dual language use.
Thirdly, across the studies, a variety of tests were used to measure the same cognitive functions. Yet, tests that supposedly measure the same executive function, do not always yield similar results. For instance, the Simon, Stroop, and Flanker tasks, although sometimes used interchangeably, measure different subcomponents of attention, and therefore can produce different results (Dash et al., 2019). Only two of the studies in our review used the same executive functioning test (i.e., TEA), and both showed increases in attentional switching scores (Bak et al., 2016; Long et al., 2019). Most of the other studies used different tests, which complicates their direct comparison and may account for some of the discrepancies in results.
Fourthly, the inclusion of validated language proficiency measures is perhaps as equally important as the inclusion of appropriate cognitive measures; yet some of the studies had none at all. This is a major flaw in methodology if researchers aim to provide solid evidence of effects that are induced by actual language learning.
The differing intensities of the studies' language interventions could further account for some of the discrepancies in results. For instance, working memory improved in one higher quality study with a 6-month long intervention (Wong et al., 2019), and did not in another, also of higher quality, but proposing a shorter intervention of under 4 months (Berggren et al., 2020).
Finally, another point that merits consideration is how a foreign language is taught, given that social learning impacts the brain differently than traditional methods in SLA (Li and Jeong, 2020). For instance, it has been revealed in younger adults learning a foreign language that a larger impact on the brain is induced through social interaction than through media (Yusa et al., 2017).
Participants' language and cultural background is also of significance. Study protocols should include language background and demographic questionnaires, and recruit participants who share similar cultural and linguistic experiences, as the neurocognitive effects of learning might vary as a function of similarity to participants' mother tongues. The choice of target language is thus significant. In this respect, artificial language learning could also be of interest as it allows for the control of language similarity and exposure (Folia et al., 2010).
Further, motivation is a very important factor in SLA research (Ushioda, 2009), as it is strongly correlated with language learning success (Gardner and Lambert, 1972; Hernandez, 2006), as well as commitment to the interventions. Therefore, measures of motivation should be included in research protocols.
Ultimately, in terms of brain imaging measures, only one study to date investigated functional changes associated with SLA interventions, with resting-state functional MRI. Future studies should provide a greater overview on SLA functional substrates, but also on structural correlates of second language learning in older adults, by including pre-and post-intervention multimodal neuroimaging. While more costly than simple neuropsychological evaluations, only brain imaging will help to better understand the mechanisms by which SLA interventions can influence older adults' brain health.
Future Directions and Recommendations
Based on the limitations of the studies reviewed, we recommend considering the following points in future research: Inclusion of randomized active and passive control groups, cognitive test batteries with sensitive evaluations that measure subtle changes in the executive functioning domains previously shown to be affected by second language use, language proficiency tests carried out at pre-and post-intervention, linguistic and demographic questionnaires, measures of motivation, and perhaps most importantly, pre-and post-intervention multimodal neuroimaging measures. Finally, teaching methodologies should be outlined carefully, with course duration and intensity justified within the context of previous SLA research. The influence of SLA interventions on cognition and brain health has been relatively neglected and the few existing studies provide inconsistent results, stressing the need for further investigation. The above methodological considerations should be taken into account in these future studies. Fortunately, three ongoing research protocols on the effects of second language training in seniors have been identified, and the quality of their methodologies appear to be superior to those included in our review.
A study currently being conducted in France entitled Age-well, part of the Silver Santé Study European Project, assesses healthy senior participants (>65 years old), randomly assigned to either a meditation, English training, or a passive control group for 18 months (Poisnel et al., 2018). This study, with its large sample size (137 healthy participants), is unprecedented as it is randomized with an active and passive control group, the former being an 18-month English intervention taught at the University of Caen, with a complete battery of cognitive tests, multimodal neuroimaging, psychological questionnaires evaluating emotions and quality of life, blood biomarkers, as well as sleep quality measures, including actigraphy and polysomnography. If we were to preliminarily calculate its RoB score, it would be rated as a high-quality study considering its randomization, inclusion of two active interventions (meditation vs. English training), and a passive control group, blinding of assessors, statistical power, and intention-to-treat analysis.
Another study, entitled ENGAGE, will examine the effects of a leisure-based Spanish or music intervention on 144 Canadian healthy seniors with memory complaints. Participants will be randomly assigned to an experimental condition involving cognitive training through either musical or language training, or an active control group involving low stimulating activities such as discussing documentaries and playing casual video games (Belleville et al., 2019). Their interventions will last 4 months, yet participants will be followed for 24 months and undergo episodic memory and attention tests, as well as psychological assessments, and structural and fMRI scans. In addition, cognitive reserve proxies such as apolipoprotein, brain-derived neurotrophic factor, catechol-O-methyltransferase, and scores on lifestyle questionnaires will be analyzed. Despite the absence of passive controls, a preliminary RoB rating for this study would also suggest a high quality given their outlined randomization and blinding procedures, as well as their large sample size.
Finally, another Canadian protocol called “Boosting Cognitive Reserve Through Adult Second Language Acquisition with Duolingo” is recruiting around 90 older adults for a 4-month randomized controlled trial in which participants will either (1) learn Spanish through Duolingo, (2) train with a computerized cognitive stimulation program, or (3) be part of a passive control group for 16 weeks. Their main outcome measures are tests of executive functioning, including the n-back and the Simon tasks. Among other secondary outcome measures, Spanish proficiency will be evaluated with the WebCAPE Online Spanish test before and after the Duolingo intervention. Although the protocol has not yet been published in a scientific journal, this study seems to be of high quality with the inclusion of an active and passive control group, fairly large sample size, and pertinent cognitive measures, as well as a second language proficiency test as an outcome measure.
Conclusion
Although inquiry on the subject of SLA interventions in older adulthood is still in its infancy, there are some indications of cognitive benefits associated with foreign language learning in later life, especially for executive functions. Yet, the quality of the reviewed studies is fair at best, and research on the subject is sparse. The only neuroimaging study to date showed improvement in functional connectivity, yet without any other references, conclusions cannot yet be drawn. Further research with complete validated test batteries that include standardized language proficiency tests, randomization with passive and active control groups, and longer interventions with larger sample sizes should be carried out to expand upon these promising findings. The three identified ongoing clinical trials with the aforementioned methodological strengths should notably allow for significant advancements in the field.
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.
Author Contributions
CW, JG, and GC formulated the research questions. CW conducted the literature search. CW and SD assessed the quality and risk of bias of the included studies. All authors had an active role in drafting and revising the manuscript, and all authors approved the final draft for publication.
Funding
This research was supported by the French Institute of Health and Medical Research (INSERM), Region Normandie.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnagi.2021.706672/full#supplementary-material
References
- Abutalebi J., Canini M., Della Rosa P. A., Green D. W., Weekes B. S. (2015b). The neuroprotective effects of bilingualism upon the inferior parietal lobule: a structural neuroimaging study in aging Chinese bilinguals. J. Neurolinguist. 33, 3–13. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2014.09.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Abutalebi J., Canini M., Della Rosa P. A., Sheung L. P., Green D. W., Weekes B. S. (2014). Bilingualism protects anterior temporal lobe integrity in aging. Neurobiol. Aging 35, 2126–2133. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Abutalebi J., Guidi L., Borsa V., Canini M., Della Rosa A., Parris B. A., et al. (2015a). Bilingualism provides a neural reserve for aging populations. Neuropsychology 69, 201–210. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Allerhand M., Gale C., Deary I. J. (2014). The dynamic relationship between cognitive function and positive well-being in older people: a prospective study using the English longitudinal study of aging. Psychol. Aging 29, 306–318. 10.1037/a0036551 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson J. A. E., Grundy J. G., De Frutos J., Barker R. M., Grady C., Bialystok E. (2018). Effects of bilingualism on white matter integrity in older adults. NeuroImage 167, 143–150. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoniou M., Gunasekera G. M., Wong P. C. M. (2013). Foreign language training as cognitive therapy for age-related cognitive decline: a hypothesis for future research. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2689–2698. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Antoniou M., Wright S. M. (2017). Uncovering the mechanisms responsible for why language learning may promote healthy cognitive aging. Front. Psychol. 8:2217. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02217 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Armstrong B. A., Ein N., Wong B. I., Gallant S. N., Li L. (2019). The Effect of bilingualism on older adults' inhibitory control: a meta-analysis. Gerontologist 61, 102–117. 10.1093/geront/gnz086 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bak T. H., Long M. R., Vega-Mendoza M., Sorace A. (2016). Novelty, challenge, and practice: the impact of intensive language learning on attentional functions. PLoS ONE 11:e153485. 10.1371/journal.pone.0153485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bak T. H., Nissan J. J., Allerhand M. M., Deary I. J. (2014a). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Biling. Aging. Ann Neurol. 75, 959–963. 10.1002/ana.24158 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bak T. H., Vega-Mendoza M., Sorace A. (2014b). Never too late? An advantage on tests of auditory attention extends to late bilinguals. Front. Psychol. 5:485. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00485 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellander M., Berggren R., Mårtensson J., Brehmer Y., Wenger E., Li T.-Q., et al. (2016). Behavioral correlates of changes in hippocampal gray matter structure during acquisition of foreign vocabulary. Neuroimage 131, 205–213. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belleville S., Moussard A., Ansaldo A. I., Belchior P., Bherer L., Bier N., et al. (2019). Rationale and protocol of the ENGAGE study: a double-blind randomized controlled preference trial using a comprehensive cohort design to measure the effect of a cognitive and leisure-based intervention in older adults with a memory complaint. Trials 20:282. 10.1186/s13063-019-3250-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berggren R., Nilsson J., Brehmer Y., Schmiedek F., Lövdén M. (2020). Foreign language learning in older age does not improve memory or intelligence: evidence from a randomized controlled study. Psychol. Aging 35, 212–219. 10.1037/pag0000439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E., Craik F., Luk G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 34, 859–873. 10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E., Craik F. I. M., Klein R., Viswanathan M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the simon task. Psychol. Aging 19, 290–303. 10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E., Craik F. I. M., Ryan J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: effects of aging and bilingualism. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cognit. 32, 1341–1354. 10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1341 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok E., Martin M. M., Viswanathan M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: the rise and fall of inhibitory control. Int. J. Biling. 9, 103–119. 10.1177/13670069050090010701 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Birdsong D. (2018). Plasticity, variability and age in second language acquisition and bilingualism. Front. Psychol. 9:81. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blumenfeld H. K., Schroeder S. R., Bobb S. C., Freeman M. R., Marian V. (2016). Auditory word recognition across the lifespan: links between linguistic and non-linguistic inhibitory control in bilinguals and monolinguals. Linguist. Approaches Biling. 6, 119–146. 10.1075/lab.14030.blu [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Borsa V. M., Perani D., Della Rosa P. A., Videsott G., Guidi L., Weekes B. S., et al. (2018). Bilingualism and healthy aging: aging effects and neural maintenance. Neuropsychologia 111, 51–61. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bubbico G., Chiacchiaretta P., Parenti M., di Marco M., Panara V., Sepede G., et al. (2019). Effects of second language learning on the plastic aging brain: functional connectivity, cognitive decline, and reorganization. Front. Neurosci. 13:423. 10.3389/fnins.2019.00423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cabeza R., Albert M., Belleville S., Craik F. I. M., Duarte A., Grady C. L., et al. (2018). Maintenance, reserve and compensation: the cognitive neuroscience of healthy ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 19, 701–710. 10.1038/s41583-018-0068-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng S.-T. (2016). Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: the role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 18:85. 10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Costa A., Sebastián-Gallés N. (2014). How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 336–345. 10.1038/nrn3709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cotelli M., Manenti R., Ferrari C., Gobbi E., Macis A., Cappa S. F. (2020). Effectiveness of language training and non-invasive brain stimulation on oral and written naming performance in primary progressive aphasia: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 108, 498–525. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dash T., Berroir P., Joanette Y., Ansaldo A. I. (2019). Alerting, orienting, and executive control: the effect of bilingualism and age on the subcomponents of attention. Front. Neurol. 10:1122. 10.3389/fneur.2019.01122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Flores R., La Joie R., Chételat G. (2015). Structural imaging of hippocampal subfields in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease. Neuroscience 309, 29–50. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Del Maschio N., Sulpizio S., Gallo F., Fedeli D., Weekes B. S., Abutalebi J. (2018). Neuroplasticity across the lifespan and aging effects in bilinguals and monolinguals. Brain Cogn. 125, 118–126. 10.1016/j.bandc.2018.06.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folia V., Udden J., de Vries M., Forkstam C., Petersson K. M. (2010). Artificial language learning in adults and children. Lang. Learn. 60, 188–220. 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00606.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Friederici A. D., Steinhauer K., Pfeifer E. (2002). Brain signatures of artificial language processing: evidence challenging the critical period hypothesis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 529–534. 10.1073/pnas.012611199 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner R. C., Lambert W. E. (1972). Attitudes and Motivation in Second-Language Learning. Boston, MA: Newbury House Publisher. [Google Scholar]
- Ghazi Saidi L., Perlbarg V., Marrelec G., Pélégrini-Issac M., Benali H., Ansaldo A.-I. (2013). Functional connectivity changes in second language vocabulary learning. Brain Lang. 124, 56–65. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.11.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gold B. T., Kim C., Johnson N. F., Kryscio R. J., Smith C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. J. Neurosci. 33, 387–396. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gollan T. H., Montoya R. I., Cera C., Sandoval T. C. (2009). More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. J. Mem. Lang. 42, 787–814. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gómez D. R. (2016). Critical geragogy and foreign language learning: an exploratory application. Educ. Gerontol. 42, 136–143. 10.1080/03601277.2015.1083388 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goral M., Campanelli L., Spiro A. (2015). Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism 18, 79–89. 10.1017/S1366728913000126 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grady C. L., Luk G., Craik F. I. M., Bialystok E. (2015). Brain network activity in monolingual and bilingual older adults. Neuropsychologia 66, 170–181. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.10.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Green D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism 1, 67–81. 10.1017/S1366728998000133 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Green D. W., Abutalebi J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: the adaptive control hypothesis. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 515–530. 10.1080/20445911.2013.796377 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hejazi Z., Kim J., Pisano T. S., Ouchikh Y., Lerman A., Obler L. K. (2019). “Brain-based challenges of second language learning in older adulthood,” in The Handjournal of the Neuroscience of Multilingualism, eds J. W. Schwieter, and M. Paradis (Chichester: John Wiley and Sons, Ltd; ), 408–426. 10.1002/9781119387725.ch20 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hernandez T. (2006). Integrative motivation as a predictor of success in the intermediate foreign classroom. Lang. Learn. 39, 605–617. 10.1111/j.1944-9720.2006.tb02279.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hu S., Ide J. S., Zhang S., Li C. R. (2016). The right superior frontal gyrus and individual variation in proactive control of impulsive response. J. Neurosci. 36, 12688–12696. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1175-16.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kliesch M., Giroud N., Pfenninger S. E., Meyer M. (2017). “Research on second language acquisition in old adulthood: what we have and what we need,” in Third Age Learners of Foreign Languages. Multilingual Matters, ed D. Gabryś-Barker (Bristol: Blue Ridge Summit; ), 48–75. 10.21832/9781783099412-006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Klimova B., Slaninova G., Pavel Prazak P., Kacetl J., Valis M. (2020). Enhancing cognitive performance of healthy czech seniors through non-native language learning— a mixed-methods pilot study. Brain Sci. 9:573. 10.3390/brainsci10090573 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuklewicz A., King J. (2018). “It's never too late”: a narrative inquiry of older polish adults' English language learning experiences. TESL-EJ 22, 1–22. [Google Scholar]
- Legault J., Grant A., Fang S.-Y., Li P. (2019). A longitudinal investigation of structural brain changes during second language learning. Brain Lang. 197:104661. 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104661 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li L., Abutalebi J., Emmorey K., Gong G., Yan X., Feng X., et al. (2017). How bilingualism protects the brain from aging: insights from bimodal bilinguals: bimodal bilingualism prevents brain aging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4109–4124. 10.1002/hbm.23652 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li P., Jeong H. (2020). The social brain of language: grounding second language learning in social interaction. NPJ Sci. Learn. 5:8. 10.1038/s41539-020-0068-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ljungberg J. K., Hansson P., Andrés P., Josefsson M., Nilsson L.-G. (2013). A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PLoS ONE 8:e73029. 10.1371/journal.pone.0073029 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long M. R., Vega-Mendoza M., Rohde H., Sorace A., Bak T. H. (2019). Understudied factors contributing to variability in cognitive performance related to language learning. Bilingualism 23, 801–811. 10.1017/S1366728919000749 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- López Zunini R. A., Morrison C., Kousaie S., Taler V. (2019). Task switching and bilingualism in young and older adults: a behavioral and electrophysiological investigation. Neuropsychologia 133:107186. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107186 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luk G., Bialystok E., Craik F. I. M., Grady C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. J. Neurosci. 31, 16808–16813. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mårtensson J., Eriksson J., Bodammer N. C., Lindgren M., Johansson M., Nyberg L., et al. (2012). Growth of language-related brain areas after foreign language learning. NeuroImage 63, 240–244. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey A., Sachs R. (2012). Older learners in SLA research: a first look at working memory, feedback, and L2 development: older learners in SLA Res. Lang. Learn. 62, 704–740. 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00649.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Marcotte K., Ansaldo A. I. (2014). Age-related behavioural and neurofunctional patterns of second language word learning: different ways of being successful. Brain Lang. 135, 9–19. 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto D. (2019). Exploring third-age foreign language learning from the well-being perspective: work in progress. SISAL 10, 111–116. 10.37237/100107 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Nichols E. S., Wild C. J., Stojanoski B., Battista M. E., Owen A. M. (2020). Bilingualism affords no general cognitive advantages: a population study of executive function in 11,000 people Psychol. Sci. 31, 548–567. 10.1177/0956797620903113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nyberg L., Lövdén M., Riklund K., Lindenberger U., Bäckman L. (2012). Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cogn. Sci. 16, 292–305. 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsen R. K., Pangelinan M. M., Bogulski C., Chakravarty M. M., Luk G., Grady C. L., et al. (2015). The effect of lifelong bilingualism on regional grey and white matter volume. Brain Res. 1612, 128–139. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.02.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsson M. R., Braconi P. E. (2005). Buon viaggio! Stockholm: Bilda Förlag. [Google Scholar]
- Paap K. R., Johnson H. A., Sawi O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? J. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 615–639. 10.1080/20445911.2014.94491431555050 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pfenninger S. E., Polz S. (2018). Foreign language learning in the third age: a pilot feasibility study on cognitive, socio-affective and linguistic drivers and benefits in relation to previous bilingualism of the learner. JESLA 2:1. 10.22599/jesla.36 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pfenninger S. E., Singleton D. (2019). A critical review of research relating to the learning, use and effects of additional and multiple languages in later life. Lang. Teach. 52, 419–449. 10.1017/S0261444819000235 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Poisnel G., Arenaza-Urquijo E., Collette F., Klimecki O. M., Marchant N. L., Wirth M., et al. (2018). The Age-well randomized controlled trial of the medit-ageing european project: effect of meditation or foreign language training on brain and mental health in older adults. Transl. Res. Clin. Interv. 4, 714–723. 10.1016/j.trci.2018.10.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polony T. E., Gnand M., Abari K., Nagy A., Santha J. (2016). The animal farm task: implicit and explicit language learning in young and elderly adults. Cogn. Brain Behav. Interdiscip. J. 20, 395–409. [Google Scholar]
- Pot A., Keijzer M., de Bot K. (2018). Intensity of multilingual language use predicts cognitive performance in some multilingual older adults. Brain Sci. 8:92. 10.3390/brainsci8050092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pyers J. E., Gollan T. H., Emmorey K. (2009). Bimodal bilinguals reveal the source of tip-of-the-tongue states. Cognition 112, 323–329. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.04.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramos S., Fernández García Y., Antón E., Casaponsa A., Duñabeitia J. A. (2017). Does learning a language in the elderly enhance switching ability? J. Neuroling. 43, 39–48. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2016.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ristin-Kaufmann N., Gullberg M. (2014). The effects of first exposure to an unknown language at different ages. Bull. VALS-ASLA. 99, 17–29. [Google Scholar]
- Robinson P. (2010). Implicit artificial and incidental natural second language learning: how comparable are they? Lang. Learn. 60, 245–263. 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00608.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rosselli M., Ardila A., Araujo K., Weekes V. A., Caracciolo V., Padilla M., et al. (2000). Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish-English bilinguals. Appl. Neuropsychol. 7, 17–24. 10.1207/S15324826AN0701_3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scarmeas N., Stern Y. (2003). Cognitive reserve and lifestyle. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 25, 625–633. 10.1076/jcen.25.5.625.14576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schweizer T. A., Ware J., Fischer C. E., Craik F. I. M., Bialystok E. (2012). Bilingualism as a contributor to cognitive reserve: evidence from brain atrophy in Alzheimer's disease. Cortex 48, 991–996. 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shomstein S. (2012). Cognitive functions of the posterior parietal cortex: top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 6:38. 10.3389/fnint.2012.00038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singleton D., Pfenninger S. E. (2018). L2 acquisition in childhood, adulthood and old age: misreported and under-researched dimensions of the age factor. JSLS 1, 254–276. 10.1075/jsls.00003.sin [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Soars J., Soars L. (2007). New Headway: Pre-Intermediate Third Edition: Student's Journal A: Student's Journal A Pre-intermediate lev (Headway ELT). Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Stern Y. (2012). Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. 11, 1006–1012. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stern Y., Arenaza-Urquijo E. M., Bartrés-Faz D., Belleville S., Cantilon M., Chetelat G., et al. (2020). Whitepaper: defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer's Dement. 16. 1305–1311. 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tops M., Boksem M. A. S. (2011). A potential role of the inferior frontal gyrus and anterior insula in cognitive control, brain rhythms, and event-related potentials. Front. Psychol. 2:330. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00330 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tucker A. M., Stern Y. (2011). Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr. Alzheimer's Res. 4, 354–360. 10.2174/156720511795745320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ushioda E. (2009). “A person-in-context relational view of emergent motivation, self, and identity,” in Motivation, Language Identity, and the L2 Self, eds Z. Dörnyei, and E. Ushioda (Bristol: Multilingual Matters; ), 215–228. 10.21832/9781847691293-012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Valenzuela M., Sachdev P. (2006). Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol. Med. 36, 441–454. 10.1017/S0033291705006264 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- von Bastian C. C., Souza A. S., Gade M. (2016). No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: a test of four hypotheses. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 145, 246–258. 10.1037/xge0000120 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ware C., Damnee S., Djabelkhir L., Cristancho V., Wu Y.-H., Benovici J., et al. (2017). Maintaining cognitive functioning in healthy seniors with a technology-based foreign language program: a pilot feasibility study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:42. 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong P. C. M., Ou J., Pang C. W. Y., Zhang L., Tse C. S., Lam L. C. W., et al. (2019). Language training leads to global cognitive improvement in older adults: a preliminary study. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 62, 2411–2424. 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-L-18-0321 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization (2020). Dementia. Available online at: https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/dementia#:~:text=Worldwide%2C%20around%2050%20million%20people%20have%20dementia%2C%20with%20nearly%2060,%2D%20and%20middle%2Dincome%20countries.andtext=The%20total%20number%20of%20people,2030%20and%20152%20in%202050 (accessed October 11, 2020).
- Yusa N., Kim J., Koizumi M., Sugiura M., Kawashima R. (2017). Social interaction affects neural outcomes of sign language learning as a foreign language in adults. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:115. 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.