Table 1.
Authors and year | Research question | Primary outcome measured | Sample size* | Sample description* | Health condition or situation | Severity of health event specified? | Study of EC† terms? | Risk of bias assessment |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lichtenstein and Newman 1967 27 | To assess numerical estimates and symmetry of interpretation of “mirror image” pairs of terms (e.g., “quite likely”-“quite unlikely”) | Numerical estimates | 188 | Adult males | Not specified | No | No | A little concern |
Budescu et al. 1985 28 | To assess variability in the mapping of phrases to numbers | Numerical estimates | 32 | Faculty and graduate students of a university | Not specified | No | No | Moderate concern |
Reagan et al. 198929 | To map verbal probability words to numbers | Numerical estimates | 100 | Undergraduate students | Not specified | No | No | Moderate concern |
Shaw and Dear 199030 | To evaluate understanding of probability expressions and preference for receiving information | Numerical estimates, format preference | 100 | Adult female parents | Aspects of neonatal care | No | No | No concern |
Weber and Hilton 199031 | To examine the role of context in the interpretation of probability words | Numerical estimates | 85 | Undergraduate students | Varying disease types and side effects | Some specified as severe life-threatening events, others unspecified | No | Moderate concern |
Freeman et al. 19926 | To identify patients’ preferred risk language and physicians’ predictions about patient preferences | Format preference | 208 | Adult female patients with children from family practices | Vaccine risk | No | No | Moderate concern |
Woloshin et al. 199432 | To assess patients’ interpretation of probability terms | Numerical estimates; format preference | 307 | Adult patients from a family practice | Medication side effect or complication risk from procedure |
Minor vs major complications |
No | Moderate concern |
Hallowell et al. 1997 33 | To evaluate female patient preferences in formats used to present risk information during genetic counseling for breast and ovarian cancer | Format preference | 43 | Adult female patients presenting for genetic counseling in cancer clinic | Breast and ovarian cancer risks | No | No | A little concern |
Franic et al. 200034 | To evaluate format preference in patient medication package inserts | Format preference | 74 | Adult female patients from academic university | Adverse drug reactions | No | No | High concern |
Biehl et al. 200135 | To compare the interpretation of probability terms of adults with adolescents | Numerical estimates | 34 | Adults from a community center | Not specified | No | No | A little concern |
Kaplowitz et al. 200236 | To evaluate how patients want, request, and receive cancer prognosis information | Format preference | 352 |
Patients from the American Cancer Society (ACS) mailing list in Michigan, US |
Cancer prognosis information | No | No | A little concern |
Berry et al. 200237 | To assess the interpretation of verbal probability descriptors | Numerical estimates | 268 | Undergraduate and graduate students | Throat infection or ear infection; fictitious medication side effect | Mild vs severe side effects | Yes | Moderate concern |
Berry et al. 200338 | To compare the understanding of verbal and numerical descriptions of medication side effects | Numerical estimates | 360 | Adults from various public settings | Fictitious medication side effect | Mild vs severe side effects | Yes | A little concern |
Budescu et al. 200339 | To determine the directionality of probability phrases | Numerical estimates | 27 | Undergraduate students | Medical context; general medication administration | No | No | Moderate concern |
Davey et al. 200340 | To evaluate women’s understanding of diagnostic test results | Numerical estimates | 37 | Adult women who had previously participated in a population survey | Breast cancer risk | No | No | A little concern |
Lobb et al. 200341 | To evaluate how women wanted their risk of breast cancer to be described in consultation | Format preference | 193 | Adult women from cancer clinics | Breast cancer risk | No | No | Moderate concern |
Berry et al. 200442 | To evaluate people’s interpretation of EC verbal descriptors for medication side effect risks | Numerical estimates | 188 | Adults from various public places | Over-the-counter painkiller medication side effects | No | Yes | A little concern |
Berry et al. 200443 | To compare doctors’ and lay people’s interpretation of the EC verbal descriptors | Numerical estimates | 134 | Undergraduate and postgraduate students | Medication side effect | No | Yes | A little concern |
Knapp et al. 200444 | To explore whether the EC verbal descriptors effectively convey the risk of side effects | Numerical estimates | 120 | Adults from cardiac rehabilitation clinics following a recent admission | Medication side effects for cardiac medication | No | Yes | A little concern |
Berry and Hochhauser 200645 | To assess how verbal descriptors affect people’s perceptions of clinical trial participation risks | Numerical estimates | 96 | Adults from a train station | Fictional serious skin condition | No | No | A little concern |
Hubal and Day 200646 | To evaluate the understanding of verbal probability terms and effects of alternative formats | Numerical estimates | 222 | Undergraduate students | Medication side effect | No | No | Moderate concern |
Young and Oppenheimer 200647 | To assess how different formats of risk information influence medication compliance | Numerical estimates | 120 | Adult students from a university | Medication side effect | No | Yes | Moderate concern |
France et al. 200848 | To compare the understanding of frequency of side effects when expressed in percentages or descriptive language | Numerical estimates, % correctly identified | 50 | Patients in the chest pain unit of an urban emergency department who had one or more ischemic heart disease factors | Risks of treatment for acute myocardial infarction | Severe vs less severe side effects | No | No concern |
Graham et al. 200949 | To identify women’s preference and interpretation of language for description of the size of treatment complication risks | Format preference‡ | 262 | Adult female patients undergoing routine follow-up visits for breast cancer | Breast cancer risk | No | No | A little concern |
Knapp et al. 200950 | To assess the effectiveness of presenting side effect risk information in different formats | Numerical estimates | 148 | Adult users of an online cancer information website | Medication side effect | No | No | No concern |
Nagle et al. 200951 | To evaluate female patients’ preference on risk of disease | Format preference | 294 | Adult female patients from a maternity unit | Down syndrome risk | No | No | No concern |
Cheung et al. 201052 | To compare patients’ preference for risk presentation in medications | Format preference | 240 | Adult patients from arthritis clinics in a hospital and outpatient practice | Pain relief medication | No | Yes | A little concern |
Vahabi 201053 | To evaluate whether format preference influences comprehension | Format preference | 180 | Adult female patients from various community settings | Breast cancer risk | No | No | High concern |
Peters et al. 201454 | To measure risk comprehension and willingness to use a medication when presented with different formats | Numerical estimates | 905 | Adult participants from a paid online questionnaire | Cholesterol medication | No | Yes | No concern |
Knapp et al. 201455 | To evaluate recommendations on communicating frequency information on side effect risk | Numerical estimates | 339 | Adult users of an online cancer information website | Medication side effects | No | No | A little concern |
Webster et al. 201756 | To assess how people interpret the EC verbal descriptors | Numerical estimates, % correctly identified | 1003 | Adult users of an online survey conducted by a market research company | Medication side effects | Mild vs severe side effects | Yes | A little concern |
Carey et al. 2018 57 | To assess patients’ interpretation of verbal descriptor chance of remission and preferences for format of risk communication | Numerical estimates, format preference | 210 | Adult medical oncology outpatients with a diagnosis of cancer | Cancer long-term side effects and chances of remission | No | No | A little concern |
Wiles et al. 202058 | To determine the perceived risk of surgical complication risk using verbal probability terms | Numerical estimates | 290 | Adult patients attending a pre-operative assessment in a clinic | Major adverse postoperative complication | No | No | No concern |
*Several studies contained both subsamples that met our inclusion criteria (adult laypeople) and other subsamples that did not (physicians, adolescents). As described in the “METHODS” section, these studies were included if the results for the eligible subsample were reported separately. For these studies, we report the sample size and sample description of the subgroup that met our inclusion criteria
†EC = European Community
‡Graham et al. 2009 49 required respondents to choose from ordinal categories ranging from 1/100 to 1/10 000. The modal interpretation of “sometimes” was 1/100 (36% of women), “uncommon” 1/1000 (35%), “very uncommon” 1/10 000 (40%), “rare” 1/10 000 (58%) and “very rare” 1/10 000 (51%). Because of the categorical assessment and the fact that no larger numbers were provided to choose from, we did not average these results into the findings in Table 2.