Skip to main content
. 2021 Aug 6;36(12):3820–3829. doi: 10.1007/s11606-021-07050-7

Table 3.

Numbers and Proportions Preferring Verbal or Numeric Probabilities

Study * Sample size n (%)
Preferred verbal Preferred numeric Preferred combination No preference
Woloshin et al. 198425 307 91 (29.6) 135 (43.9) 81 (26.3) NA
Shaw and Dear 199023 81 43 (53.1) 30 (37.0) NA 8 (9.9)
Freeman and Bass 19926 208 89 (42.7) 119 (57.2) NA NA
Hallowell et al. 1997 33 43 3 (7) 9 (21) 22 (52) 8 (19)
Franic and Pathak 200026 74 4 (5.4) 70 (94.6) NA NA
Lobb et al. 2003 41 109 (unaffected by condition) 24 (22.1) 55 (50) 20 (18.3) 10 (9.6)
84 (affected by condition) 15 (17.9) 16 (19.2) 45 (53.8) 8 (9)
Graham et al. 2009 49 262 136 (52) 125 (47.7) NA 1 (0.3)
Nagle et al. 200938 294 85 (28.9) 132 (44.9) 76 (25.8) NA
Cheung et al. 201039 240 60 (25.0) 180 (75.0) NA NA
Vahabi 201040 180 61 (33.9) 119 (66.1) NA NA
Carey et al. 2018 57 210 59 (28) 33 (16) 79 (38) 39 (18)

NA indicates that this option was not presented to respondents

*Kaplowitz et al. (2002)36 also evaluated format preference among cancer patients and survivors in a hypothetical choice between a verbal probability and a quantitative estimate of survival. However, the findings are not integrated into this table because the options were not mutually exclusive, and the authors do not clarify how many patients chose both. (Table 1 of that paper shows that 80% endorsed verbal probabilities and 53% endorsed quantitative information, suggesting that some subset must have chosen both)