Skip to main content
Scientific Reports logoLink to Scientific Reports
. 2022 Jan 19;12:1019. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05128-8

Early auditory responses to speech sounds in Parkinson’s disease: preliminary data

Fatemeh Mollaei 1,, Douglas M Shiller 2,4,5, Shari R Baum 2,3, Vincent L Gracco 2,3,6
PMCID: PMC8770631  PMID: 35046514

Abstract

Parkinson’s disease (PD), as a manifestation of basal ganglia dysfunction, is associated with a number of speech deficits, including reduced voice modulation and vocal output. Interestingly, previous work has shown that participants with PD show an increased feedback-driven motor response to unexpected fundamental frequency perturbations during speech production, and a heightened ability to detect differences in vocal pitch relative to control participants. Here, we explored one possible contributor to these enhanced responses. We recorded the frequency-following auditory brainstem response (FFR) to repetitions of the speech syllable [da] in PD and control participants. Participants with PD displayed a larger amplitude FFR related to the fundamental frequency of speech stimuli relative to the control group. The current preliminary results suggest the dysfunction of the basal ganglia in PD contributes to the early stage of auditory processing and may reflect one component of a broader sensorimotor processing impairment associated with the disease.

Subject terms: Sensorimotor processing, Diseases, Parkinson's disease, Basal ganglia, Auditory system

Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD), a manifestation of basal ganglia (BG) dysfunction, is associated with a number of speech production deficits in prosody, phonation and articulation, with phonation and laryngeal deficits the most prominent15. In addition to speech motor symptoms, auditory perceptual deficits ranging from self-monitoring to discrimination have been reported in PD57. Interestingly, it is during the monitoring of their own speech that PD participants often show the greatest differences from unimpaired speakers3,8,9. A common interpretation is that when individuals with PD are asked to produce speech with normal loudness (as judged by a speech-language pathologist), they perceive themselves as shouting or producing abnormally loud speech8. In addition, while listening at a given distance from a loudspeaker, individuals with PD estimated the loudness level to be significantly greater than that estimated by healthy control participants2.

These perceptual/sensory differences and their relationship with the speech motor output deficits of PD have been examined in several recent studies. In response to alterations in auditory feedback during speech, PD participants display an interesting characteristic. When faced with feedback consistent with a misplaced tongue position for a specific vowel (change in first formant frequency of vowel), PD participants exhibit reduced compensation compared to age-matched control participants10, consistent with a weaker motor response. In contrast, when faced with feedback consistent with a change in fundamental frequency (fo, voice pitch), PD participants exhibit increased compensation to the feedback shift1013. An increased response to voice pitch shifts has also been observed in participants with Alzheimer’s disease14,15 and cerebellar degeneration16. In a recent study, we demonstrated that the increased response to a change in voice pitch is accompanied also by increased sensitivity in detecting pitch alterations during auditory feedback monitoring in participants with PD4. Pitch feedback manipulations were presented under conditions of production and listening. In the production condition, participants’ vocal pitch was shifted, and participants judged whether their speech output had been manipulated in real-time; participants’ responses to pitch shift change were simultaneously recorded. During the listening task, participants judged whether paired tokens of their previously recorded speech samples were the same or different. Under the production condition, the ability of participants with PD to identify the pitch shift was greater than that of the controls, with a trend for better detection during the listening condition4. Interestingly, in a parallel experiment, detection accuracy of first formant shifts was reduced in individuals with PD only during the listening condition4.

At the neural level, electrophysiological recordings in response to voice pitch shifts yielded larger event related potentials in the inferior frontal gyrus, precentral gyrus, postcentral gyrus, and middle temporal gyrus in PD participants12. While the results suggest cortical involvement in the enhanced pitch shift response, there are also reports in the literature to suggest involvement at the level of the brainstem for auditory processing in PD participants1719. The brainstem, including the cochlear nuclei, the superior olivary complex, and the inferior colliculus of the midbrain comprise the auditory pathway to auditory cortical areas, with processing at each level20. The main focus of the current investigation was to evaluate the brainstem involvement in the processing of complex speech sounds in relation to the speech motor deficits in PD.

The auditory brainstem response (ABR) is an auditory evoked neural potential that provides a window into top–down and bottom–up processing of sensory information through the efferent and afferent projections of cortex, brainstem, and BG. Scalp-recorded auditory brainstem responses to complex sounds such as vowels include transient and sustained components that represent certain critical acoustic properties of speech stimuli21. Some of the acoustic properties of speech including pitch and formant frequencies are closely reflected within a component of the ABR (sustained response) known as the frequency-following response (FFR). The FFR reflects sustained electrical potentials that are precisely phase-locked to neuronal firing with an upper limit of about 1000 Hz in response to low to middle frequency periodic acoustic stimuli. Consequently, the FFR demonstrates a robust representation of time-varying fo and harmonics corresponding to the pitch and first formant frequency (F1) of the vowel (21,22 for a review refer to23). Measures that are derived from the FFR response, such as amplitude and latency, represent a mapping between the auditory stimulus and the neural activity, which may be modified due to changes associated with disease (e.g., autism spectrum disorders, and mild cognitive impairment24,25) or exposure to auditory stimulation (e.g., music, and bilingualism26,27). Specific regions in the BG and thalamic nuclei, including the subparafascicular thalamic nucleus, send dopaminergic projections to the main auditory midbrain nucleus and the inferior colliculus, consistent with a modulatory role of the BG in auditory processing2831. Based on these considerations, it is suggested that a dysfunctional dopaminergic system may contribute to differences in the processing of pitch and formant information for speech and ultimately to differences in speech motor output.

Here, we recorded FFR from the left mastoid during listening to a speech syllable. We compared patterns of phase-locking in individuals with PD and control participants on the assumption that it indexes—not solely, but to a large extent—subcortical interactions. On the basis of prior behavioral and perceptual results, we hypothesized that individuals with PD would show an increased FFR response only in the frequency range associated with fo compared to control participants, consistent with a brainstem contribution to their speech perceptual and production impairment.

Results

Electrophysiology

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was used to compare root mean square (RMS) magnitudes (pre-stimulus and response) and spectral response amplitudes of the FFR (fo and F1) between groups (PD vs. control). A waveform depicting the characteristics of the input stimulus [da], along with averaged FFR waveforms for the PD and control groups, is displayed in Fig. 1a and b. The Figure also includes a bar graph comparing the magnitudes of response RMS amplitudes during the pre-stimulus and stimulus periods, and it is displayed in Fig. 1c. In the time domain, no significant difference was observed between groups for the amplitude of the pre-stimulus region [F(1,28) = 0.24, p = 0.626]. In contrast, individuals with PD showed greater RMS amplitude for the frequency-following response [F(1,28) = 9.04, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.244].

Figure 1.

Figure 1

(a) Stimulus waveform. (b) Average waveform brainstem response to [da] in individuals with PD (red) and age- and gender-matched controls (black). The pre-stimulus period (− 10 to 0 ms) and the response period (0 to 60 ms) are shown. (c) Bar graph demonstrating between-group differences in RMS amplitude for the pre-stimulus and response periods. The error and shaded error bars represent the standard error. Note: *p < 0.01.

Figure 2 shows the averaged FFT (Fig. 2a) and bar graphs of the mean response amplitudes corresponding to fo and F1 (Fig. 2b) for the two groups. In the frequency domain, the amplitude of the frequency-following responses during the vowel portion of the stimulus (10–60 ms) for fo (80–120 Hz) and F1 (400–600 Hz) were assessed for group differences. Individuals with PD demonstrated larger amplitude FFR responses in the frequency range associated with fo compared to the control group [F(1,28) = 8.51, p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.233; PD group (Mean (M) = 0.146, Standard Deviation (SD) = 0.047; Range = 0.04–0.23); Control group (M = 0.100, SD = 0.056; Range = 0.04–0.25)]. However, there was no group difference for the frequency range associated with F1 [F(1,28) = 0.002, p = 0.966; PD group (M = 0.014, SD = 0.009; Range = 0.01–0.04); Control group (M = 0.014, SD = 0.006; Range = 0.01–0.03)].

Figure 2.

Figure 2

(a): Fast Fourier transforms calculated for the vowel part of the average response of individuals with PD (red) and age- and gender-matched control (black) group. (b) Bar graphs demonstrating between-group differences in response amplitude corresponding to fo (80–120 Hz) and F1 (400–600 Hz) representation of the speech stimuli. The error and shaded error bars represent the standard error. Note: *p < 0.01.

Correlation analysis

To further investigate the relationship between the neurophysiological response to the auditory speech stimuli and the severity of behavioural speech disorders (dysarthria) of PD, we correlated the FFR amplitudes corresponding to fo and F1 with the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS) and perceptual dysarthria ratings for each participant with PD. No significant correlation was observed between the amplitudes corresponding to fo or F1 and the UPDRS (fo and MDS-UPDRS: r(13) = 0.088, p = 0.754; F1 and MDS-UPDRS: r(13) = 0.153, p = 0.587) or the perceptual dysarthria scores (fo and dysarthria score: r(13) = − 0.368, p = 0.177; F1 and dysarthria score: r(13) = − 0.428, p = 0.111).

Discussion

Our preliminary results are consistent with an enhanced encoding of vocal pitch, evidenced by increased FFR amplitude in the frequency range of 80–120 Hz, in individuals with PD compared to age- and gender-matched control participants. In contrast, we did not find evidence of enhanced encoding within the range of the first formant (400–600 Hz). The increased amplitude of the FFR response is consistent with a selective modulation of the fundamental frequency of the speech stimuli at the level of the brainstem. The brainstem components of the FFR are modulated by the BG through inhibitory and disinhibitory projections to auditory relay areas including the inferior colliculus (IC30), with the IC one of the largest generators of the FFR response3234.

Increased encoding of vocal pitch has been observed at the cortical level evidenced by larger auditory evoked potentials (P2 responses) in PD compared to non-PD participants12. Since the brainstem FFR can be modulated from the cortex35, the specific source of the enhancement (brainstem or cortex) can't be determined from the present results. However, it appears that the increased FFR response in PD participants reflects a reduction in the inhibitory (or tuning) function of the auditory brainstem pathway which enhances the frequency response within the vocal pitch range. In our previous study, PD participants were better able to detect and compensate for pitch shifts compared to the non-PD participants4. The current results suggest that the difference in FFR amplitude in the pitch range enhances the salience of the acoustic signal enabling better detection of differences in pitch during listening. In addition, the enhanced feedback signal interacts with the motor output during voice production to produce a stronger response to pitch shifts.

BG damage and its influence on the auditory signal on vocal output during production may be accompanied by an opposite change to the sensitivity of the somatosensory system. Voice production generates auditory as well as somatosensory feedback resulting from the laryngeal vibration, and feedback from both sensory systems contribute to the perception of pitch and loudness36,37. For individuals with no history of neurodegenerative disease, vocal fold mucous anesthetization yields a greater compensatory response to auditory feedback alterations in pitch compared to the pitch response without anesthesia38, suggesting a trade-off in sensitivity or gain between the two sources of sensory input (somatosensory vs. auditory). Similarly, masking of laryngeal somatosensory feedback by applying low-pass filtered stochastic vibrations to the neck enhances the Lombard response (increased loudness)37. In individuals with PD, using air-puff stimulation, it was found that thresholds for detecting laryngeal somatosensory input are increased, consistent with reduced sensitivity of mechanoreceptors in the laryngeal area39. As a result, BG damage appears to influence both sensory modalities (auditory and somatosensory) in opposing ways resulting in an increase in sensitivity to pitch-related auditory feedback and a decrease in sensitivity to pitch-related somatosensory feedback.

One consequence of the opposing changes in feedback from the two sensory systems is an imbalance in their contributions to speech production. The addition of the pitch shift would combine with normal feedback to increase the perception of pitch and loudness, while the change in threshold for somatosensory input would result in smaller movements due to the reduced reafferent input. Interestingly, one therapy for remediation of the hypokinetic features in the speech of individuals with PD involves vocal exercises focused on the production of loud speech (Lee Silverman Voice Treatment—LSVT9,4043). The approach generates greater somatosensory feedback from the articulatory and laryngeal systems compared to normal speech. One of the effects of the LSVT is smaller vocal pitch compensations following treatment suggesting a down regulation of the enhanced auditory response44. Hence, it is reasonable to suggest that by increasing loudness, the LSVT treatment may act to reset the balance between the feedback systems providing the conditions for more normal speech production and perception.

In our previous work, we also found a reduction in compensation to manipulations of F1 auditory feedback10, and a reduction in F1 error detection during listening in our PD participants4. In the present study there was no evidence of an enhancement or a reduction in the FFR at frequencies in the range of the first formant. The lack of difference in FFR amplitude in the first formant frequency range suggests that the motor and sensory deficits related to F1 may occur at a higher level than the brainstem. Because formant information is associated with phonetic processing, it more likely involves primary and association levels of the processing stream, which rely on integration of both formant and harmonic information to extract relevant linguistic information. As a result, the influence of BG dysfunction on speech has differential effects related to the processing levels for different components of the speech production (and presumably) the perceptual process.

This study has some shortcomings that should be mentioned. First, most of the individuals with PD in the current study showed mild to moderate severity. It would be of interest to study a greater range of severity in future studies to have a more complete picture of the effect of PD on speech auditory brainstem processing. It is also important to consider other concomitant factors along with PD on the FFR responses, such as amount of musical training or multilingualism (in the case of tonal languages as the second language), as there are reports of the effects of both on FFR responses26,27. In order to recruit as many participants as possible, we did not screen for the amount of musical training or multilingualism in the current study, these factors merit attention in future investigations. In addition, based on recent work on different speech subtypes of PD45,46, it would be beneficial to assess the relationship of the sensory deficits and different speech subtypes. In the current work, we did not classify different speech subtypes of individuals with PD due to the relatively small sample size. In the future, one can, for example, investigate whether pitch and loudness sensory differences are more frequent in the prosodic subtype of speech deficits in PD as opposed to the phonatory-prosodic or articulatory-prosodic subtypes.

In conclusion, in these preliminary data, we found increased frequency-following neural responses related to fo during the perception of speech in individuals with PD compared to age- and gender-matched control participants. These findings provide a neural basis for the sensory processing deficits of vocal pitch and loudness at the brainstem level in this population10,11,13. Impaired modulation of sensory information at the BG may be one possible factor in the manifestations of speech deficits in individuals with PD.

Materials and methods

Ethics statement

This study was approved by the McGill Faculty of Medicine Institutional Review Board, in accordance with principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent was obtained from participants prior to their involvement in the research project.

Participants

Fifteen patients with Parkinson’s disease (6 female, 9 male; mean age: 65.87 years) and fifteen age- and gender-matched control participants (6 female, 9 male; mean age: 63.13 years) were recruited for this study (same group as our previous study4,10). The severity of PD motor symptoms, assessed using the Movement Disorder Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS; Part III Motor Examination47), ranged from mild (a score of 13) to moderate (a score of 48; mean [M] ± standard deviation [SD] score, 24.79 ± 9.19). Cognitive functioning was assessed using the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA48) and was in the normal range for all individuals with PD (scores > 26). All patients were taking L-dopa in addition to other medication, including dopaminergic and/or anticholinergic drugs. Participants were tested off medication for 12 h. Two participants reported a history of speech therapy focused on increasing speech loudness and intelligibility.

Each participant read aloud the Rainbow Passage (a standard speech perceptual passage assessment) in order to carry out a perceptual analysis of dysarthric speech characteristics. A licensed Speech-Language Pathologist rated the speech of participants on 43 perceptual dimensions that span the speech subsystems, including phonatory and articulatory subsystems, using a 7-point scale1. Overall, the severity of participants with PD was rated as moderate (2 participants), mild-to-moderate (4 participants), mild (6 participants), and within normal limits (3 participants). Inter-rater agreement was tested between the first rater and a second listener using intraclass correlation (ICC) in order to assess consistency in the ratings of speech perceptual characteristics in individuals with PD. The resulting ICC was in the excellent range, ICC = 0.9049, indicating that the raters had a high degree of agreement. We used the perceptual and MDS-UPDRS scores to evaluate any relationship between severity of speech and motor symptoms and the magnitude of the FFR responses.

All participants underwent an audiometric screening and were found to have binaural pure tone hearing thresholds of 40 dB HL or less at 250, 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000 Hz. None of the participants used hearing aids. All participants were native speakers of North-American English. Participants in the control group were healthy with no history of neurological condition.

Stimulus and recording

A 40 ms (ms) speech syllable, [da], was synthesized at a 20 kHz sampling rate using a Klatt synthesizer50. After a 5 ms stop burst, voicing remained constant with a fundamental frequency of 100 Hz, and the first formant frequency of 500 Hz. The [da] stimulus was chosen because it combines transient ([d], the first 10 ms with a 5 ms voice onset time) and periodic ([a]) segments51), two acoustic features that have been extensively studied in speech ABR52. For each participant, the [da] stimulus was presented 12,000 times with a 50 ms interstimulus-interval. Stimuli were presented in alternating stimulus polarities (i.e., compression and decompression of air molecules of periodic sound waves: positive and negative) to both ears at 80 dB SPL through electromagnetically-shielded insert earphones (Etymotic ER-2) to reduce stimulus and noise artifacts, using a TDT stimulus presentation system (Tucker-Davis Technologies, TDT Inc., Alachua, FL). A vertical montage of four electrodes (left mastoid active, two on the forehead as grounds, and a hairline reference) was used, with all impedances kept under 5 kΩ. Continuous responses were recorded (20 kHz sampling frequency) with ActiABR200 software (BioSemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands). During the recording session (lasting approximately 18 min), participants sat in a comfortable chair in a sound attenuated room.

Data processing

Electrophysiological responses were band-pass filtered offline in EEGLAB53 between 70 and 2000 Hz to maximize signal-to-noise ratio and detection of peaks within the phase-locking limits of the brainstem. The root mean square (RMS) amplitude of the neural responses was used to quantify the overall magnitude of response and pre-stimulus activity. RMS amplitudes were computed for the pre-stimulus period (− 10 to 0 ms) and the response period (0 to 60 ms). A fast Fourier transform (FFT) was performed on a signal window between 10 and 60 ms, corresponding to the voiced portion of the stimulus (Brainstem toolbox52). Mean amplitude across frequency ranges corresponding to fo (80 to 120 Hz) and F1 (400 to 600 Hz) were calculated for each trial and then averaged across participants for each group. Responses were then averaged over a − 10 to 60 ms window, with stimulus onset occurring at time zero. Any trial with an amplitude greater than 40 µV was considered an artifact and rejected before averaging. We included 11,400 trials of response averages (5700 trials in each polarity) in the analysis after artifact rejection across participants. There were no differences in the number of rejected trials between the two groups [t(28) = 0.971, p = 2.048]. Responses (0 to 60 ms) were then amplitude baseline-corrected in the pre-stimulus period (− 10 to 0 ms). In a final step, responses from the two stimulus polarities were averaged to minimize the influence of cochlear microphonic and stimulus artifact on the measured response54.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) with root mean square (RMS) magnitudes (pre-stimulus and response) and spectral responses (fo and F1) as within-subject factors and group (PD vs. control) as between-subject factor. Factor and simple effect sizes were quantified using ηp2 to assess any statistically significant effects, defined as small (0.2–0.3), medium (0.5), and large (> 0.855). Greenhouse–Geisser corrections for unequal variances were applied when necessary. In addition, separate Pearson correlation analyses were performed between FFR amplitudes of fo or F1 and perceptual or MDS-UPDRS clinical scores in individuals with PD. This resulted in four correlation analyses. A Bonferroni-adjusted α rate of 0.012 was used.

Acknowledgements

We thank Francois-Xavier Brajot, PhD, CCC-SLP and Suzanne Lalonde, SLP(C) for their help in the evaluations of speech samples. In addition, we are grateful for all the participants who took part in this study.

Author contributions

F.M. designed the study, implemented the experiments, tested participants, analysed the data, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. D.M.S. contributed to the design, analysis, and editing of the manuscript. S.R.B. contributed to the analysis, interpretation of the data, and writing of the manuscript. V.L.G. contributed to the conceptualisation, design, statistical analysis, and writing of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by Grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) awarded to Dr. Baum and Dr. Gracco.

Data availability

Anonymized datasets recorded and analyzed during this study are available from the corresponding author within the limits of participants’ consent.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Duffy JR. Motor Speech Disorders: Substrates, Differential Diagnosis, and Management. Elsevier Mosby; 2013. pp. 198–217. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Ho AK, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R, Alfredson R. Speech volume regulation in Parkinson’s disease: Effects of implicit cues and explicit instructions. Neuropsychologia. 1999;37(13):1453–1460. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(99)00067-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Ho AK, Bradshaw JL, Iansek R. Volume perception in parkinsonian speech. Mov. Disord. 2000;15(6):1125–1131. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200011)15:6<1125::aid-mds1010>3.0.co;2-r. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Mollaei F, Shiller DM, Baum SR, Gracco VL. The relationship between speech perceptual discrimination and speech production in Parkinson’s disease. J Speech. Lang. Hear. Res. 2019;62(12):4256–4268. doi: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-18-0425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Brajot FX, Shiller DM, Gracco VL. Autophonic loudness perception in Parkinson's disease. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2016;139(3):1364–1371. doi: 10.1121/1.4944569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Abur D, Lester-Smith RA, Daliri A, Lupiani AA, Guenther FH, Stepp CE. Sensorimotor adaptation of voice fundamental frequency in Parkinson's disease. PLoS ONE. 2018;13(1):e0191839. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0191839. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Troche J, Troche MS, Berkowitz R, Grossman M, Reilly J. Tone discrimination as a window into acoustic perceptual deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2012;21:258–263. doi: 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Fox CM, Ramig LO. Vocal sound pressure level and self-perception of speech and voice in men and women with idiopathic Parkinson disease. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 1997;6(2):85–94. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Fox CM, Morrison CE, Ramig LO, Sapir S. Current perspectives on the Lee Silverman Voice Treatment (LSVT) for individuals with idiopathic Parkinson disease. Am. J. Speech-Lang. Pathol. 2002;11(2):111–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mollaei F, Shiller DM, Baum SR, Gracco VL. Sensorimotor control of vocal pitch and formant frequencies in Parkinson's disease. Brain Res. 2016;1646:269–277. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Chen X, Zhu X, Wang EQ, Chen L, Li W, Chen Z, Liu H. Sensorimotor control of vocal pitch production in Parkinson's disease. Brain Res. 2013;1527:99–107. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.06.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Huang X, Chen X, Yan N, Jones JA, Wang EQ, Chen L, Guo Z, Li W, Liu P, Liu H. The impact of Parkinson's disease on the cortical mechanisms that support auditory–motor integration for voice control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37(12):4248–4261. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23306. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Liu H, Wang EQ, Metman LV, Larson CR. Vocal responses to perturbations in voice auditory feedback in individuals with Parkinson's disease. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(3):e33629. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0033629. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Ranasinghe KG, Gill JS, Kothare H, Beagle AJ, Mizuiri D, Honma SM, Gorno-Tempini ML, Miller BL, Vossel KA, Nagarajan SS, Houde JF. Abnormal vocal behavior predicts executive and memory deficits in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol. Aging. 2017;52:71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.12.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Ranasinghe KG, Kothare H, Kort N, Hinkley LB, Beagle AJ, Mizuiri D, Honma SM, Lee R, Miller BL, Gorno-Tempini ML, Vossel KA, Nagarajan SS. Neural correlates of abnormal auditory feedback processing during speech production in Alzheimer’s disease. Sci. Rep. 2019;9(1):1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-41794-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Parrell B, Agnew Z, Nagarajan S, Houde J, Ivry RB. Impaired feedforward control and enhanced feedback control of speech in patients with cerebellar degeneration. J. Neurosci. 2017;37(38):9249–9258. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3363-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Gawel MJ, Das P, Vincent S, Rose FC. Visual and auditory evoked responses in patients with Parkinson's disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 1981;44(3):227–232. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.44.3.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Tachibana H, Takeda M, Sugita M. Short-latency somatosensory and brainstem auditory evoked potentials in patients with Parkinson's disease. Int. J. Neurosci. 1989;44(3–4):321–326. doi: 10.3109/00207458908986210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Teo C, Rasco L, Al-Mefty K, Skinner RD, Boop FA, Garcia-Rill E. Decreased habituation of midlatency auditory evoked responses in Parkinson's disease. Mov. Disord. 1997;12(5):655–664. doi: 10.1002/mds.870120506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Krishnan A, Gandour JT. The role of the auditory brainstem in processing linguistically-relevant pitch patterns. Brain Lang. 2009;110(3):135–148. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Russo N, Nicol T, Musacchia G, Kraus N. Brainstem responses to speech syllables. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2004;115(9):2021–2030. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.04.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Swaminathan J, Krishnan A, Gandour JT. Pitch encoding in speech and nonspeech contexts in the human auditory brainstem. NeuroReport. 2008;19(11):1163. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283088d31. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Du Y, Kong L, Wang Q, Wu X, Li L. Auditory frequency-following response: A neurophysiological measure for studying the “cocktail-party problem”. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2011;35(10):2046–2057. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.05.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Otto-Meyer S, Krizman J, White-Schwoch T, Kraus N. Children with autism spectrum disorder have unstable neural responses to sound. Exp. Brain Res. 2018;236(3):733–743. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5164-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Bidelman GM, Lowther JE, Tak SH, Alain C. Mild cognitive impairment is characterized by deficient brainstem and cortical representations of speech. J. Neurosci. 2017;37(13):3610–3620. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3700-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bidelman GM, Gandour JT, Krishnan A. Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain Cogn. 2011;77(1):1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Bidelman GM, Gandour JT, Krishnan A. Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;23(2):425–434. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nevue AA, Elde CJ, Perkel DJ, Portfors CV. Dopaminergic input to the inferior colliculus in mice. Front. Neuroanat. 2016;9:168. doi: 10.3389/fnana.2015.00168. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Nevue AA, Felix RA, II, Portfors CV. Dopaminergic projections of the subparafascicular thalamic nucleus to the auditory brainstem. Hear. Res. 2016;341:202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2016.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Winer JA, Chernock ML, Larue DT, Cheung SW. Descending projections to the inferior colliculus from the posterior thalamus and the auditory cortex in rat, cat, and monkey. Hear. Res. 2002;168(1–2):181–195. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(02)00489-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Graybiel AM. Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators in the basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13(7):244–254. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90104-i. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bidelman GM. Subcortical sources dominate the neuroelectric auditory frequency-following response to speech. Neuroimage. 2018;175:56–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.DeLong MR. Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin. Trends Neurosci. 1990;13:281–285. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90110-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Stocco A, Lebiere C, Anderson JR. Conditional routing of information to the cortex: A model of the basal ganglia’s role in cognitive coordination. Psychol. Rev. 2010;117(2):541. doi: 10.1037/a0019077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Coffey EB, Nicol T, White-Schwoch T, Chandrasekaran B, Krizman J, Skoe E, Zatorre RJ, Kraus N. Evolving perspectives on the sources of the frequency-following response. Nat. Commun. 2019;10(1):1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-13003-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fairbanks G. Systematic research in experimental phonetics: 1. A theory of the speech mechanism as a servosystem. J. Speech Hear. Disord. 1954;19:133–139. doi: 10.1044/jshd.1902.133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Brajot FX, Nguyen D, DiGiovanni J, Gracco VL. The impact of perilaryngeal vibration on the self-perception of loudness and the Lombard effect. Exp. Brain Res. 2018;236(6):1713–1723. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5248-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Larson CR, Altman KW, Liu H, Hain TC. Interactions between auditory and somatosensory feedback for voice F0 control. Exp. Brain Res. 2008;187(4):613–621. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1330-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Hammer MJ, Barlow SM. Laryngeal somatosensory deficits in Parkinson’s disease: Implications for speech respiratory and phonatory control. Exp. Brain Res. 2010;201(3):401–409. doi: 10.1007/s00221-009-2048-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Fox, C. M., Ramig, L. O., Ciucci, M. R., Sapir, S., McFarland, D. H. & Farley, B. G. The science and practice of LSVT/LOUD: Neural plasticity-principled approach to treating individuals with Parkinson disease and other neurological disorders In Seminars in Speech and Language 283–299 (Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc, 2006). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 41.Ramig LO, Sapir S, Fox C, Countryman S. Changes in vocal loudness following intensive voice treatment (LSVT®) in individuals with Parkinson's disease: A comparison with untreated patients and normal age-matched controls. Mov. Disord. 2001;16(1):79–83. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(200101)16:1<79::aid-mds1013>3.0.co;2-h. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ramig LO, Fox CN, Sapir S. Speech and voice disorders in Parkinson’s disease. In: Olanow CW, Stocchi F, Lang AE, editors. Parkinson’s Disease: Non-motor and Non-dopaminergic Features. Blackwell Publishing Ltd; 2011. pp. 348–362. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ramig L, Halpern A, Spielman J, Fox C, Freeman K. Speech treatment in Parkinson's disease: Randomized controlled trial (RCT) Mov. Disord. 2018;33(11):1777–1791. doi: 10.1002/mds.27460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Li Y, Tan M, Fan H, Wang EQ, Chen L, Li J, Chen X, Liu H. Neurobehavioral effects of LSVT® LOUD on auditory-vocal integration in Parkinson’s disease: A preliminary study. Front. Neurosci. 2021;15:181. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2021.624801. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rusz J, Tykalova T, Novotny M, Zogala D, Sonka K, Ruzicka E, Dusek P. Defining speech subtypes in de novo Parkinson disease: Response to long-term levodopa therapy. J. Neurol. 2021;97(21):e2124–e2135. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000012878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Rusz J, Tykalová T, Novotný M, Růžička E, Dušek P. Distinct patterns of speech disorder in early-onset and late-onset de-novo Parkinson’s disease. NPJ Parkinson’s Dis. 2021;7(1):1–8. doi: 10.1038/s41531-021-00243-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Goetz CG. Movement disorder society-unified Parkinson's disease rating scale (MDS-UPDRS): A new scale for the evaluation of Parkinson's disease. Rev. Neurol. 2009;166(1):1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2009.09.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, Cummings JL, Chertkow H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cicchetti DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol. Assess. 1994;6(4):284. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Klatt DH. Software for a cascade/parallel formant synthesizer. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1980;67(3):971–995. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kraus N, McGee TJ, Carrell TD, Zecker SG, Nicol TG, Koch DB. Auditory neurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children with learning problems. Science. 1996;273(5277):971–973. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5277.971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Skoe E, Kraus N. Auditory brainstem response to complex sounds: A tutorial. Ear Hear. 2010;31(3):302–324. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181cdb272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2004;134(1):9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Campbell T, Kerlin JR, Bishop CW, Miller LM. Methods to eliminate stimulus transduction artifact from insert earphones during electroencephalography. Ear Hear. 2012;33(1):144. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182280353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Witte R, Witte J. Statistics. Wiley; 2010. [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Anonymized datasets recorded and analyzed during this study are available from the corresponding author within the limits of participants’ consent.


Articles from Scientific Reports are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES