Abstract
Psychotic and autistic symptoms are related to social functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders (PD). The present study used a network approach to (1) evaluate the interactions between autistic symptoms, psychotic symptoms, and social functioning, and (2) investigate whether relations are similar in individuals with and without PD. We estimated an undirected network model in a sample of 504 PD, 572 familial risk for psychosis (FR), and 337 typical comparisons (TC), with a mean age of 34.9 years. Symptoms were assessed with the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; 5 nodes) and the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE; 9 nodes). Social functioning was measured with the Social Functioning Scale (SFS; 7 nodes). We identified statistically significant differences between the FR and PD samples in global strength (P < .001) and network structure (P < .001). Our results show autistic symptoms (social interaction nodes) are negatively and more closely related to social functioning (withdrawal, interpersonal behavior) than psychotic symptoms. More and stronger connections between nodes were observed for the PD network than for FR and TC networks, while the latter 2 were similar in density (P = .11) and network structure (P = .19). The most central items in strength for PD were bizarre experiences, social skills, and paranoia. In conclusion, specific autistic symptoms are negatively associated with social functioning across the psychosis spectrum, but in the PD network symptoms may reinforce each other more easily. These findings emphasize the need for increased clinical awareness of comorbid autistic symptoms in psychotic individuals.
Keywords: autism, functional outcome, psychosis, schizophrenia, network analysis, network models
Introduction
Psychotic disorders (PD) are typically accompanied by impairments in social functioning, which are often persistent and resistant to therapeutic interventions.1–3 For many people with a PD diagnosis and their relatives, improvement in the social domain is one of the most preferred outcomes of treatment.4 One factor that may contribute to poor social functioning in those with psychosis is the presence of autism spectrum conditions, which are characterized by social impairments.5–9 Symptoms within the psychosis and autism spectra commonly co-occur, and recent evidence suggests comorbid autistic symptoms may negatively impact social cognition and functioning in people with psychosis.4,10–16 Here, we argue that a better understanding of relations between psychosis, autism, and social functioning is essential to determine appropriate intervention targets.
Network models are relatively novel statistical tools in psychiatric research, which grew popular in the past decade, as they allow zooming into interactions at the symptom level (for clarity, the term “symptom(s)” is used here regardless of whether the underlying assessments are perceived as more indicative of traits or state-like symptoms). The appeal of the network approach is that it can overcome limitations of more traditional research strategies by conceptualizing psychopathology as mutually interacting elements of a complex network (eg, symptoms, biological components, environmental risk factors17–22). Network analysis can provide information on symptom relations, such as visual representation of the overall network structure, clustering, as well as the relative centrality of specific symptom clusters or “nodes” (ie, which nodes are connected to most other nodes, as well as which nodes connect specific clusters). In addition, network models have been argued to be especially useful when investigating links between different domains of interest (eg, comorbid features, risk factors, and symptomatology),19,23,24 as they may allow pinpointing specific bridging components (eg, nodes, links), which may provide useful information in identifying suitable treatment targets.
Although recent studies investigated symptom network structures in PD populations,25 as well as the impact of potential etiological20,26 and comorbid features,27 none included the relative impact of comorbid autistic features. Zhou et al28 conducted a meta-analysis and network analysis (n = 2469 college students) of relations between schizotypal and autistic symptoms. The outcome showed that negative symptoms were strongly correlated with autistic social/communicative symptoms, whereas positive symptoms were inversely correlated with autistic symptoms. However, it is unclear to what extent these findings in student populations translate to more heterogeneous nonclinical or clinical populations and whether the co-occurrence of symptoms can have an incremental impact on outcome.
The main aim of the present study was therefore to use a network approach to explore interactions between autistic symptoms, psychotic symptoms, and social functioning. In addition, we aimed to identify specific nodes within the autism and psychosis spectra that may play a pivotal role in determining social functioning in PD. As both psychosis and autism may represent broad phenotypes that can affect individuals at a subclinical level,29 we compared network characteristics between individuals with different levels of risk for psychosis: individuals with a familial risk for psychosis (FR), who share a genetic and environmental liability to psychosis due to their relatedness and physical proximity to an individual with a PD, and typical comparisons (TC) drawn from the general population with a default risk for psychosis. Building on the theory-driven work of Ziermans et al,5 who analyzed aggregated sumscores, we applied a data-driven network approach to establish which autistic symptom clusters would be more closely related to social functioning clusters than psychotic symptom clusters. Furthermore, aligned with previous research showing more densely connected networks for clinical populations27,30,31 we expected that relations within the PD network would be stronger than for FR and TC networks.
Methods
Sample Characteristics
We used data from the Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) study, data release 6.0. GROUP is a multisite longitudinal observational study carried out between April 2004 and December 2013, investigating vulnerability and protective factors that may influence the onset and course of PD. The study was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht and subsequently by local review boards of each participating institute.32
Current analyses included a subset of the full data, from the third data collection time point (ie, 6-year follow-up) and from individuals who completed the measures described below: 504 PD, 572 FR (siblings only), and 337 TC participants, of which 53.9% were male (365, 254, and 153, respectively), with an age range between 21 and 63 years, a mean age of 34.9 years (SD = 8.65), and a mean IQ of 108 (abbreviated IQ based on Arithmetic, Block Design, Digit Symbol-Coding, and Information of the Dutch version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Third Edition33). The majority of participants were Caucasian (1244; 88.04%), followed by Mixed ethnicity (94; 6.65%), Surinamese (21; 1.49%), Moroccan (15; 1.06%), Turkish (10; 0.71%), Other (9; 0.64%), Asian (2; 0.14%), Antillean (1; 0.07%), while for 5 individuals (0.35% of the sample) it was unknown. For additional group characteristics see sTable 3 and table 1 in Ziermans et al.5
Materials
All individuals completed 3 measures: (1) the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), (2) the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE), and (3) the Social Functioning Scale (SFS), as well as demographic attributes (IQ and age), resulting in 23 variables included in the network. IQ was entered in the networks because it differentiated substantially between groups (see sTable3). All questionnaires are widely used, well-validated instruments.34–38
Autistic Symptoms
The AQ39 is a measure of autistic features in individuals. It is a brief, self-administered questionnaire consisting of 50 items (4-point scale: 1 = definitely agree to 4 = definitely disagree). These are divided into five 10-item subcategories: social skills, attention switching, attention to detail, communication, and imagination, scored in the traditional manner, ie, recoded into dichotomous scores with a range of 0–50 for the total score. A higher score indicates a higher level of autistic features in the specified area.
Psychotic Symptoms
The CAPE35 is a measure aimed at detecting psychotic-like experiences. It consists of 42 items (4-point scale: 1 = never to 4 = nearly always), with the reference period for reporting experiences being the past 3 years. The CAPE items correspond to 3 different symptom dimensions: (1) positive, (2) negative, and (3) depression, further subdivided into 9 clusters.40 These are positive symptoms: bizarre experiences (7 items), hallucinations (4 items), paranoia (5 items), grandiosity (2 items), and magical thinking (2 items); negative symptoms: social withdrawal (4 items), affective flattening (3 items), avolition (7 items), and a single cluster for depression (8 items). A higher score on any dimension indicates higher symptomatology.
Social Functioning
The SFS38 is an assessment of social functioning within the past 3 months. The SFS consists of 7 different dimensions: withdrawal (5 items), interaction (4 items), prosocial (22 items), independence performance (13 items), independence competence (13 items), recreation (15 items), and occupation (max. 8 [conditional] items). A score key is used to calculate raw scores, and a higher score indicates higher level of social functioning.
Network Analyses
We carried out all analyses in the R statistical software,41 using the packages bootnet42 v1.4.3, qgraph43 v1.6.9, and networktools44 v1.2.3. Missing data for individual items (under 1% for the 3 scales) were imputed using the package mice45 v3.11.0. The dimensions of the 3 scales, and age and estimated IQ as covariates, were included as nodes. An edge between 2 nodes represents a partial correlation between the nodes, while controlling for all other nodes in the network.46 Blue (red) edges were used to illustrate positive (negative) associations, and the wider and more saturated the edge, the stronger the association.
Network Estimation
To estimate the network models, we fitted Gaussian graphical models (undirected network structures) in each group separately. To ensure sparse and interpretable models, we used the least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO)47 statistical regularization technique. Specifically, we employed the graphical LASSO algorithm48 in combination with Extended Bayesian Information Criterion (EBIC) model selection, with a tuning hyperparameter set to .5, which has now been widely used when estimating network structures.26,49,50 Missing data for individual items were handled using pairwise estimation and Spearman correlations were used when estimating the networks, as recommended for nonnormality distributed data.51 The network layout was matched using the average of the Fruchterman and Reingold algorithm52 (ie, each network was first plotted individually and then the layout was averaged), which places the nodes with stronger connections closer to the center of the network.
Network Comparison
Network structures were constructed and compared using control, family, and PD data. To assess the differences between samples we used the Network Comparison Test (NCT).53 The NCT is a permutation test that investigates differences in the global strength of the networks, their structure, as well as differences in individual edges and centrality measures. We only investigated differences in edges when either the structure or global strength was identified as significantly different. Due to the conservative nature of the NCT, we report results both with and without a Bonferroni correction.
Centrality Estimation
Centrality indices54 were computed to investigate which nodes display the highest connectivity and play a central role in bridging different dimensions. Specifically, we computed strength (ie, a measure of how well connected a node is) and bridge strength (ie, a measure of how well a node connects to other clusters). These measures were chosen as they were well aligned with our research aims, while still expected to be robust.42 Predictability and expected influence centrality estimates were further computed and included in Appendix 1 in the Supplementary Online Content, for the interested reader. Of note, expected influence disregards the presence of negative edges. As for the SFS assessment a higher score indicates better social functioning, we considered negative edges to be of interest here and thus focused our main analysis on strength and bridge strength centrality metrics.
Robustness Analysis
Finally, we conducted a robustness analysis to assess the stability and accuracy of our results,42 available in Supplement, Appendix 2.
Results
Network Analysis and Comparison
sTable 1 in the Supplementary Online Content presents the means and SDs of all individual items. No statistically significant differences were identified between the FR and the TC sample in global strength (P = .11) and network structure (P = .19), and therefore we focus our results on the comparison between the FR and the PD sample, of which sample sizes are higher and more comparable.
Figure 1 presents the network structures (1) for the FR sample, (2) for the PD sample, (3) highlighting edge (presence and strength) and centrality differences between the 2 networks as identified by the NCT. Figure 1c includes 2 network structures, as to allow pinpointing where specifically the differences lie (eg, in the presence/absence of edges, as would be the case with the edge between IQ and D, which is present in the PD sample only, or in the strength of the edges, as would be the case with for instance the edge between N2 and N3, which is present in both networks). Overall, within both networks, negative symptoms and depression were more strongly linked to positive psychotic symptoms than autistic symptoms, while autistic symptoms were more closely related to social functioning than psychotic symptoms. The items social skills (A1), attention switching (A2), and communication skills (A4) were negatively linked to withdrawal (S1) and interpersonal behavior (S2). For both networks, the item attention to detail (A3) did not cluster well with the remaining autism nodes, and instead was more closely related to the CAPE positive psychotic symptoms and depression. Within both networks, communication skills (A4) were linked to paranoia (P3) (this link is less visible for the FR network, but nonetheless present and not identified as different in edge strength according to the NCT), while attention switching (A2) was linked to bizarre experiences (P1) and depression (D).
Of note, we identified statistically significant differences between the FR and PD samples both in global strength (P < .001) and in network structure (P < .001). Furthermore, we identified several statistically significant differences between edges (see table 1) and centrality measures of the 2 networks (see table 2). Especially within the same domain, relations were more connected for the PD network, as more and stronger connections between nodes were observed than for the FR network. All links between autism spectrum items were stronger for the PD network, as well as between the social functioning items and psychotic experiences items. Within the PD network, the item paranoia (P3) was positively linked to recreational activities (S6), while the item grandiosity (P4) was negatively linked to attention switching (A2) and imagination (A5).
Table 1.
Variable 1 | Variable 2 | P Value |
---|---|---|
Social skills | Attention switching | .007 |
IQ | Imagination | .044 |
Social skills | Imagination | .047 |
Bizarre experiences | Hallucinations | <.001* |
Attention switching | Grandiosity | .011 |
Imagination | Grandiosity | .008 |
Paranoia | Grandiosity | .006 |
Bizarre experiences | Magical thinking | .001 |
Grandiosity | Magical thinking | .001 |
Bizarre experiences | Social withdrawal | .026 |
Communication skills | Affective flattening | .039 |
Affective flattening | Avolition | .011 |
IQ | Depression | .008 |
Attention to detail | Depression | .048 |
Avolition | Prosocial activities | .007 |
Withdrawal | Prosocial activities | .013 |
Imagination | Independence performance | .036 |
IQ | Independence competence | .049 |
Independence performance | Independence competence | <.001* |
Paranoia | Recreational activities | .003 |
Prosocial activities | Recreational activities | .008 |
Imagination | Occupation employment | .002* |
Withdrawal | Occupation employment | .002 |
Prosocial activities | Occupation employment | .024 |
Independence competence | Occupation employment | <.001* |
Note: PD, psychotic disorders.
*Remained significant after Bonferroni correction.
Table 2.
Variable | P Value |
---|---|
IQ | <.001* |
Social skills | .002* |
Attention detail | <.001* |
Imagination | <.001* |
Bizarre experiences | <.001* |
Hallucinations | <.001* |
Paranoia | <.001* |
Grandiosity | <.001* |
Magical thinking | <.001* |
Depression | .033 |
Interpersonal behavior | .008 |
Prosocial activities | .028 |
Independence performance | <.001* |
Independence competence | <.001* |
Recreational activities | .049 |
Occupation employment | <.001* |
Note: PD, psychotic disorders.
*Remained significant after Bonferroni correction.
Centrality Analysis
Figure 2 illustrates the node strength for each node in the network, both for the FR and the PD samples. The 3 most central items in terms of strength for the FR sample were avolition (N3), social withdrawal (N1), and depression (D), while for the PD sample these were bizarre experiences (P1), social skills (A1), and paranoia (P3) (see sFigures 11–14 for bootstrapped difference tests).
Figure 3 illustrates the bridge strength for each node included in the network, both for the FR and the PD samples. The 3 most central items in terms of bridge strength for the FR sample were withdrawal (S1), social skills (A1), and attention switching (A2), while for the PD sample these were IQ, imagination (A5), and social skills (A1).
Robustness Analysis
The results of the robustness analyses are presented and described in Supplementary Online Content, Appendix 2. Generally, the results show high stability both for the FR and the PD network structures Of note, the CS coefficient for bridge strength for the PD network was below the preferred 0.5 cutoff score, but above the recommended 0.25. We therefore advise caution when interpreting this measure.
Discussion
The current study presents a bird’s-eye view of relations between autistic and psychotic symptom clusters, and social functioning in individuals with PD, FR, as well as TC. Our network analyses revealed the following: (1) across all group networks, all autistic nodes, except attention to detail, were more closely related to nodes reflecting frequency of social interactions than psychotic symptom nodes; (2) the FR an TC networks were highly similar in terms of global strength and network structure, prompting further focus on PD and FR network comparisons; (3) the PD network was generally characterized by stronger and more connections than the FR network, though only few individual edges differed significantly after Bonferroni correction; and (4) centrality measures indicated that the relative importance of single nodes may differ between the PD and FR networks. The implications of these findings are discussed further in the paragraphs below.
Importantly, network analysis as a statistical technique is best thought of as an exploratory technique that identifies patterns of conditional independence that may be interpreted as empirical phenomena. These phenomena can hold clues that facilitate a better understanding of the causal structure and dynamics that characterizes disorders.55,56 By using a purely data-driven approach that creates an actual image of how symptom clusters may interact with each other, we were able to show that for both the FR and PD networks, autistic symptom clusters were more directly related to social functioning nodes than psychotic symptom clusters. In particular, the autistic nodes “Social skills,” “Attention switching,” and “Communication skills” were negatively linked to SFS “Withdrawal” and “Interpersonal behavior.” While commonly clumped together into a single “social interaction” factor,36 our results indicate that using standardized assessment of specific autistic symptoms instead—which tend to be relatively stable phenotype—may help better identify social vulnerabilities and resilience in PD, in line with previous findings.8,57,58 Further, our results show both “Paranoia” and “Social skills” to have a negative edge with “Withdrawal.” Therefore, this could be an area within the network where one might expect an interaction effect. Specifically, experiencing both increased paranoia and poor (autistic-like) social skills could be related to severe social withdrawal. In addition, focusing on withdrawal (which was also identified to have high bridging properties in both populations) may in turn facilitate better outcomes.
One of the general objectives of the GROUP project32 is to investigate vulnerability factors contributing to the expression of psychosis. However, the current FR network structure was highly similar to the TC network. This could suggest that interrelations between autistic, psychotic, and social functioning features are either a poor indicator of latent manifestations of PD, or they may only become clinically relevant for psychosis onset in high-risk individuals if other (mediating) factors are entered into the equation. There is some evidence in favor of the latter, as a recent network study based on data collected with the experience sampling method showed that inclusion of a “stress” node resulted in a higher number of network connections, with stress having a central position in the network structure across TC, FR, and PD groups, and also showing direct connections with subsequent psychotic experiences.59 Future studies including high-risk samples should aim to replicate current findings and are encouraged to incorporate known risk factors, such as distress and childhood trauma26,60 into network structures.
Given its global similarity with the TC network and the larger and more comparable sample size, the FR network was subsequently used for network comparisons with PD. In line with our expectation, the PD network was denser than the FR network, with more connections identified especially between nodes of the same domain. This included the connection between “Hallucinations” and “Bizarre experiences,” one of the main links surviving Bonferroni correction (table 1) and indicating the strong presence of feedback loops between the psychotic symptoms. Denser network structures have been previously reported for PD,59 and more so for nonremission compared to remission,27 but also for other clinical populations.30,31 Presumably, and in line with network theory,18 this could reflect the capacity of symptoms to reinforce each other much more steadily, resulting in a higher level of vulnerability,30 eg, the occurrence of high frequent hallucinations may more easily trigger an increase of other positive symptoms such as thought interference (as indicated by the “Bizarre experiences” node, in some studies referred to as “Delusions” 61). However, denser networks may also reflect a greater potential for more (transfer) effect of treatments. In line with this, it was found that treatment-responsive individuals diagnosed with PD had more densely connected symptom networks after antipsychotic treatment than did treatment-responsive individuals at baseline,62 which could indicate that in the presence of a more densely connected network structure (ie, even under greater severity), treatment may be more efficient. Identifying how these specific mechanisms act may be an important next step in the field of network modeling. Of note, research in other domains—especially depression—also identified less dense network structures in clinical populations.63,64 These findings could indicate that symptom reactivity may be different across various mental disorders, and raises the question of whether network topology differs across distinct mental health conditions.
Centrality analyses revealed many statistically significant differences between the FR and PD sample (see figure 2 and table 2), with most surviving the Bonferroni correction. The strongest nodes for FR were negative and depressive symptoms and for PD positive and autistic symptoms, suggesting that the relative importance of single nodes differs between populations. For the PD group, such findings emphasize that an increase in positive and autistic symptoms may quickly impact neighboring nodes and thus lead the network structure faster into a disorder state, while for the FR group a focus on affective symptoms may be warranted. While at this stage this remains hypothesis generating, longitudinal research can further investigate these timescales in future studies, preferably with adjusted questionnaires that allow for more variation to capture differences in trait and state-like symptoms.
In terms of bridge centrality (figure 3), ie, how well individual nodes connect to other node clusters, the overall profile was rather different for FR and PD. For brevity, we highlight only 2 individual nodes here, fully acknowledging that other findings might merit further discussion as well. First, although IQ was virtually irrelevant within the FR network, and showing marginal strength in the PD network, it does show widespread connections to individual nodes in the PD network structure. This illustrates that lower IQ is as much a part of the clinical phenotype of PD as any other node in the network and should not automatically be “controlled” for in experimental group comparisons. Second, “Attention to detail,” unlike any other node, does not cluster within its expected domain (ie, AQ node cluster) and is relatively unrelated to other nodes. This is strikingly similar to the network results in a nonclinical student population,28 as well as with previous research that identified, in 2 clinical samples, attention to detail to be the least central node and only weakly connected to other nodes.65 This finding further fits with different factor models of the AQ, in which attention to detail comprises its own separate factor. We therefore suggest attention to detail is subject to further scrutiny with regard to its construct validity.
The current study should be considered in light of several limitations. First, our analyses are exploratory in nature and our findings are hypothesis generating. In light of this, we chose to use the EBICglasso algorithm in the estimation of the network, erring on the side of discovery. Of note, however, is that this may be more prone to specificity concerns51,66 and as such we recommend caution in interpreting smaller edges and replication of results. Second, current results are based on cross-sectional data and thus causality cannot be inferred. Third, our study did not identify statistically significant differences between the TC and FR sample in global strength and structure. This may be a real effect, or may result from limited power of the NCT to identify effects (ie, the control sample is considered small for network analysis). Further, scientific conclusions and policy decisions should not be based only on whether a P value passes a specific threshold.67 It is thus essential for future research with larger sample sizes to attempt to replicate current results. Given these findings, as well as our aim to retain comparable sample sizes while comparing the networks using the NCT (ie, as to avoid effects due to sample size), we chose not to collapse TC and FR samples, but keep these independent. Of note, while means (and thus severity) should not play a role in the network structure and the NCT, the variance could affect the results if floor or ceiling effects are present. Here, we did not identify such pronounced effects. Fourth, despite their psychometric strengths and common use, self-report questionnaires have inherent limitations to capture psychological phenomena (eg, cultural bias, debatable factor structures). In addition, some of the subdomains of AQ and SFS could be argued to be conceptually related constructs. Nonetheless, overlap in measurement can usually be seen through very strong connections between 2 items in a network, which was not the case here (ie, not even for the 2 withdrawal items). Correlations of AQ and CAPE were modest and response types for all questionnaires were quite different and as such we do not expect current results and conclusions to be explained away by measurement overlap. Finally, denser network structures could also be an indication of the presence of a common cause (eg, a central dysfunction in the brain), but this seems unlikely as even constructs within the same domain (eg, positive vs negative psychotic symptoms) are generally considered to derive from different etiological pathways.
Conclusions
To conclude, the present study was the first to use a data-driven network approach in an aim to explore interactions between autistic symptoms, psychotic symptoms, and social functioning across different levels of risk for psychosis. While the presence of autistic symptoms has a general negative effect on social functioning, in the PD network autistic and psychotic symptoms may reinforce each other more strongly than for unaffected individuals, and thereby affect social functioning more steadily. On the one hand, this suggests an increased vulnerability for bispectral symptomatology, on the other hand a greater potential for more (transfer) effect of treatments. Consequently, these findings emphasize the need for increased clinical awareness and default assessment of comorbid autistic symptoms in individuals with a PD to help enrich their daily social environments.
Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) Investigators
GROUP investigators are: van Amelsvoort Therese7, Bartels-Velthuis Agna A.8, Simons Claudia J.P.7,9, van Os Jim10,11
7Maastricht University Medical Center, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastricht, The Netherlands
8University of Groningen, University Medical Center Groningen, University Center for Psychiatry, Rob Giel Research center, Groningen, The Netherlands
9GGzE Institute for Mental Health Care, Eindhoven, The Netherlands
10University Medical Center Utrecht, Department of Psychiatry, Brain Centre Rudolf Magnus, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands
11King’s College London, King’s Health Partners, Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, London, United Kingdom
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
We are grateful for the generosity of time and effort by all included individuals. Furthermore, we would like to thank all research personnel involved in the GROUP project, in particular: Joyce van Baaren, Erwin Veermans, Ger Driessen, Truda Driesen, and Erna van ’t Hag. We would also like to thank Veronika Kovacs, who carried out preliminary analyses for this project as part of her master thesis at the University of Amsterdam. The authors have declared that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study.
Contributor Information
GROUP Investigators:
Therese van Amelsvoort, Agna A Bartels-Velthuis, Claudia J P Simons, and Jim van Os
Funding
The infrastructure for the GROUP study is funded through the Geestkracht programme of the Dutch Health Research Council (Zon-Mw, grant number 10-000-1001), and matching funds from participating pharmaceutical companies (Lundbeck, AstraZeneca, Eli Lilly, Janssen Cilag) and universities and mental health care organizations (Amsterdam: Academic Psychiatric Centre of the Academic Medical Center and the mental health institutions: GGZ Ingeest, Arkin, Dijk en Duin, GGZ Rivierduinen, Erasmus Medical Centre, GGZ Noord Holland Noord. Groningen: University Medical Center Groningen and the mental health institutions: Lentis, GGZ Friesland, GGZ Drenthe, Dimence, Mediant, GGNet Warnsveld, Yulius Dordrecht and Parnassia psycho-medical center, The Hague. Maastricht: Maastricht University Medical Centre and the mental health institutions: GGzE, GGZ Breburg, GGZ Oost-Brabant, Vincent van Gogh voor Geestelijke Gezondheid, Mondriaan, Virenze riagg, Zuyderland GGZ, MET ggz, Universitair Centrum Sint-Jozef Kortenberg, CAPRI University of Antwerp, PC Ziekeren Sint-Truiden, PZ Sancta Maria Sint-Truiden, GGZ Overpelt, OPZ Rekem. Utrecht: University Medical Center Utrecht and the mental health institutions: Altrecht, GGZ Centraal, and Delta). A.-M.I. was supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), grant number 406.16.516. D.B. was supported by the European Research Council (ERC) Consolidator Grant, grant number 647209. This work was further supported by the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation (NARSAD Young Investigator grant, number 25500 to T.Z.).
References
- 1. Oorschot M, Lataster T, Thewissen V, et al. . Symptomatic remission in psychosis and real-life functioning. Br J Psychiatry. 2012;201(3):215–220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Robinson DG, Woerner MG, McMeniman M, Mendelowitz A, Bilder RM. Symptomatic and functional recovery from a first episode of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):473–479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Wykes T, Steel C, Everitt B, Tarrier N. Cognitive behavior therapy for schizophrenia: effect sizes, clinical models, and methodological rigor. Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):523–537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. de Crescenzo F, Postorino V, Siracusano M, et al. . Autistic symptoms in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry. 2019;10(78):1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Ziermans TB, Schirmbeck F, Oosterwijk F, Geurts HM, De Haan L. Autistic traits in psychotic disorders: prevalence, familial risk, and impact on social functioning. Psychol Med. 2020:1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Deste G, Vita A, Penn DL, Pinkham AE, Nibbio G, Harvey PD. Autistic symptoms predict social cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. 2020;215:113–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Bechi M, Agostoni G, Buonocore M, et al. . The influence of premorbid adjustment and autistic traits on social cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2020;26(3):276–285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Vaskinn A, Abu-Akel A. The interactive effect of autism and psychosis severity on theory of mind and functioning in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2019;33(2):195–202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9. Howes OD, Murray RM. Schizophrenia: an integrated sociodevelopmental-cognitive model. Lancet. 2014;383(9929):1366–1387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Lugnegård T, Hallerbäck MU, Gillberg C. Asperger syndrome and schizophrenia: overlap of self-reported autistic traits using the Autism-spectrum Quotient (AQ). Nord J Psychiatry. 2015;69(4):268–274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Deste G, Barlati S, Gregorelli M, et al. . Looking through autistic features in schizophrenia using the PANSS Autism Severity Score (PAUSS). Psychiatry Res. 2018;270:764–768. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Chandrasekhar T, Copeland JN, Spanos M, Sikich L. Autism, psychosis, or both? Unraveling complex patient presentations. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2020;29(1):103–113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Kincaid DL, Doris M, Shannon C, Mulholland C. What is the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder and ASD traits in psychosis? A systematic review. Psychiatry Res. 2017;250:99–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Sullivan S, Rai D, Golding J, Zammit S, Steer C. The association between autism spectrum disorder and psychotic experiences in the Avon longitudinal study of parents and children (ALSPAC) birth cohort. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;52(8):806–814.e2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Chisholm K, Lin A, Abu-Akel A, Wood SJ. The association between autism and schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a review of eight alternate models of co-occurrence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;55:173–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Barlati S, Deste G, Gregorelli M, Vita A. Autistic traits in a sample of adult patients with schizophrenia: prevalence and correlates. Psychol Med. 2019;49(1):140–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Borsboom D, Cramer AO. Network analysis: an integrative approach to the structure of psychopathology. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:91–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Borsboom D. A network theory of mental disorders. World Psychiatry. 2017;16(1):5–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Isvoranu A-M, Boyette L-L, Guloksuz S, Borsboom D. Symptom network models of psychosis. In: Tamminga CA, Ivleva EI, Reininghaus U, van Os J, eds. Psychotic Disorders: Comprehensive Conceptualization and Treatments. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 20. Isvoranu AM, Guloksuz S, Epskamp S, van Os J, Borsboom D; GROUP Investigators . Toward incorporating genetic risk scores into symptom networks of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2020;50(4):636–643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Borsboom D, Cramer AOJ, Kalis A. Brain disorders? Not really: why network structures block reductionism in psychopathology research. Behav Brain Sci. 2019;42(e2):1–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Fonseca-Pedrero E, Ortuño J, Debbané M, et al. . The network structure of schizotypal personality traits. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(suppl 2):S468–S479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, van der Maas HLJ, Borsboom D. Comorbidity: a network perspective. Behav Brain Sci. 2010;33(2–3):137–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Boschloo L, van Borkulo CD, Rhemtulla M, Keyes KM, Borsboom D, Schoevers RA. The network structure of symptoms of the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. PLoS One. 2015;10(9):e0137621. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. van Rooijen G, Isvoranu AM, Meijer CJ, van Borkulo CD, Ruhé HG, de Haan L; GROUP investigators . A symptom network structure of the psychosis spectrum. Schizophr Res. 2017;189:75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Isvoranu AM, van Borkulo CD, Boyette LL, Wigman JT, Vinkers CH, Borsboom D; Group Investigators . A network approach to psychosis: pathways between childhood trauma and psychotic symptoms. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43(1):187–196. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. van Rooijen G, Isvoranu AM, Kruijt OH, et al. ; GROUP investigators . A state-independent network of depressive, negative and positive symptoms in male patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophr Res. 2018;193:232–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Zhou HY, Yang HX, Gong JB, et al. . Revisiting the overlap between autistic and schizotypal traits in the non-clinical population using meta-analysis and network analysis. Schizophr Res. 2019;212:6–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Guloksuz S, van Os J. The slow death of the concept of schizophrenia and the painful birth of the psychosis spectrum. Psychol Med. 2018;48(2):229–244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. van Borkulo C, Boschloo L, Borsboom D, Penninx BW, Waldorp LJ, Schoevers RA. Association of symptom network structure with the course of [corrected] depression. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(12):1219–1226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Wigman JT, van Os J, Borsboom D, et al. ; MERGE . Exploring the underlying structure of mental disorders: cross-diagnostic differences and similarities from a network perspective using both a top-down and a bottom-up approach. Psychol Med. 2015;45(11):2375–2387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Korver N, Quee PJ, Boos HB, Simons CJ, de Haan L; GROUP investigators . Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP), a multi-site longitudinal cohort study focused on gene-environment interaction: objectives, sample characteristics, recruitment and assessment methods. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2012;21(3):205–221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. van der Heijden P, van den Bos P, Mol B, Kessels RP. Structural validity of the Dutch-language version of the WAIS-III in a psychiatric sample. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2013;20(1):41–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Chan EHC, Kong SDX, Park SH, et al. . Validation of the social functioning scale: comparison and evaluation in early psychosis, autism spectrum disorder and social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2019;276:45–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Konings M, Bak M, Hanssen M, van Os J, Krabbendam L. Validity and reliability of the CAPE: a self-report instrument for the measurement of psychotic experiences in the general population. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2006;114(1):55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Cath DC, Boomsma DI. Factor structure, reliability and criterion validity of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): a study in Dutch population and patient groups. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(8):1555–1566. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Lundqvist LO, Lindner H. Is the Autism-Spectrum Quotient a valid measure of traits associated with the autism spectrum? A Rasch validation in adults with and without autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2017;47(7):2080–2091. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Birchwood M, Smith J, Cochrane R, Wetton S, Copestake S. The Social Functioning Scale. The development and validation of a new scale of social adjustment for use in family intervention programmes with schizophrenic patients. Br J Psychiatry. 1990;157:853–859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Wüsten C, Schlier B, Jaya ES, et al. ; Genetic Risk and Outcome of Psychosis (GROUP) Investigators . Psychotic experiences and related distress: a cross-national comparison and network analysis based on 7141 participants from 13 countries. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(6):1185–1194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Version 4.0.2. 2020. https://www.r-project.org/
- 42. Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Fried EI. Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: a tutorial paper. Behav Res Methods. 2017;50(1):195–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Epskamp S, Cramer AOJ, Waldorp LJ, Schmittmann VD, Borsboom D. qgraph: network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. J Stat Softw. 2012;48(4):1–18. [Google Scholar]
- 44. Jones PJ. networktools: Assorted Tools for Identifying Important Nodes in Networks. R Package Version 1.0.0. CRAN. 2017. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/networktools/index.html
- 45. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. mice: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Stat Softw. 2011;45(3):1–67. [Google Scholar]
- 46. Epskamp S, Waldorp LJ, Mõttus R, Borsboom D. The Gaussian graphical model in cross-sectional and time-series data. Multivariate Behav Res. 2018;53(4):453–480. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47. Tibshirani R. Regression selection and shrinkage via the lasso. J R Stat Soc B. 1994;58(1):267–288. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics. 2008;9(3):432–441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Abacioglu CS, Isvoranu AM, Verkuyten M, Thijs J, Epskamp S. Exploring multicultural classroom dynamics: a network analysis. J Sch Psychol. 2019;74:90–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Birkeland MS, Greene T, Spiller TR. The network approach to posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review. Eur J Psychotraumatol. 2020;11(1):1700614. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Isvoranu A-M, Epskamp S. Continuous and ordered categorical data in network psychometrics: which estimation method to choose? Deriving guidelines for applied researchers, PsyArXiv, doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mbycn, 2021, preprint: not peer reviewed. https://psyarxiv.com/mbycn/ [DOI] [PubMed]
- 52. Fruchterman TMJ, Reingold EM. Graph drawing by force-directed placement. Softw Pract Exp. 1991;21(11):1129–1164. [Google Scholar]
- 53. van Borkulo, C., Epskamp, S., Jones, P., Haslbeck, J., & Millner, A. NetworkComparisonTest. R Package Version 2.2.1. CRAN. 2016. (2016). Package ‘NetworkComparisonTest’. https://cran.microsoft.com/web/packages/NetworkComparisonTest/NetworkComparisonTest.pdf [Google Scholar]
- 54. Opsahl T, Agneessens F, Skvoretz J. Node centrality in weighted networks: generalizing degree and shortest paths. Soc Networks. 2010;32(3):245–251. [Google Scholar]
- 55. Robinaugh DJ, Hoekstra RHA, Toner ER, Borsboom D. The network approach to psychopathology: a review of the literature 2008–2018 and an agenda for future research. Psychol Med. 2020;50(3):353–366. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Fried EI, Cramer AOJ. Moving forward: challenges and directions for psychopathological network theory and methodology. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2017;12(6):999–1020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Deste G, Vita A, Nibbio G, Penn DL, Pinkham AE, Harvey PD. Autistic symptoms and social cognition predict real-world outcomes in patients with schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Harvey PD, Deckler E, Jones MT, Jarskog LF, Penn DL, Pinkham AE. Autism symptoms, depression, and active social avoidance in schizophrenia: association with self-reports and informant assessments of everyday functioning. J Psychiatr Res. 2019;115:36–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Klippel A, Viechtbauer W, Reininghaus U, et al. ; MERGE . The cascade of stress: a network approach to explore differential dynamics in populations varying in risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2018;44(2):328–337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Isvoranu AM, Borsboom D, van Os J, Guloksuz S. A network approach to environmental impact in psychotic disorder: brief theoretical framework. Schizophr Bull. 2016;42(4):870–873. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Therman S, Ziermans TB. Confirmatory factor analysis of psychotic-like experiences in a general population sample. Psychiatry Res. 2016;235:197–199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62. Peralta V, Gil-Berrozpe GJ, Librero J, Sánchez-Torres A, Cuesta MJ. The symptom and domain structure of psychotic disorders: a network analysis approach. Schizophr Bull Open. 2020;1(1):sgaa008. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Fried EI, van Borkulo CD, Epskamp S, Schoevers RA, Tuerlinckx F, Borsboom D. Measuring depression over time. Or not? Lack of unidimensionality and longitudinal measurement invariance in four common rating scales of depression. Psychol Assess. 2016;28(11):1354–1367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Beard C, Millner AJ, Forgeard MJ, et al. . Network analysis of depression and anxiety symptom relationships in a psychiatric sample. Psychol Med. 2016;46(16):3359–3369. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. van Heijst BF, Deserno MK, Rhebergen D, Geurts HM. Autism and depression are connected: a report of two complimentary network studies. Autism. 2020;24(3):680–692. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Williams DR, Rast P. Back to the basics: rethinking partial correlation network methodology. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2020;73(2):187–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Hurlbert SH, Levine RA, Utts J. Coup de grâce for a tough old bull: “statistically significant” expires. Am Stat. 2019;73(suppl 1):352–357. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.