Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Jan 31;17(1):e0262670. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262670

Towards an affect intensity regulation hypothesis: Systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between affective states and alcohol consumption

Anna Tovmasyan 1,2,*, Rebecca L Monk 1,2, Derek Heim 1,2
Editor: Jose M Moran3
PMCID: PMC8803173  PMID: 35100278

Abstract

While self-medication and positive and negative reinforcement models of alcohol use suggest that there is an association between daily affect and alcohol consumption, findings within the academic literature have been inconsistent. This pre-registered systematic review meta-analytically interrogated the results from studies amongst non-clinical populations that examine the relationship between daily affective states and alcohol consumption volume. PRISMA guided searches of PsychINFO, PsycARTICLES, Science Direct, PubMed, SCOPUS, and JSTOR databases were conducted. When both laboratory and field studies were included, meta-analyses with robust variance estimation yielded 53 eligible studies on negative affect (8355 participants, 127 effect sizes) and 35 studies for positive affect (6384 participants, 50 effect sizes). The significant pooled associations between intra-day affect and alcohol consumption were r = .09, [.03, .14] for negative affect, and r = .17, [.04, .30] for positive affect. A small-to-medium sized effect (d = .275, [.11, .44]) of negative affect on daily alcohol consumption volume was found in laboratory studies (14 studies, 1100 participants). While publication bias was suspected, P-curve analyses suggested that the results were unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone, and selection model analysis revealed no significant differences in results when publication bias was accounted for. For negative affect, using number of drinks as the measure of alcohol consumption was associated with lower effect sizes. For positive affect, the results demonstrated a decline of this observed effect over time. Overall, findings point towards the possibility of developing an affect intensity regulation theory of alcohol use. Conceptualizing the mood-alcohol nexus in terms of affect intensity regulation may afford a more parsimonious explanation of alcohol consumption rather than viewing the behavior as being shaped by either positive or negative affective states.

Introduction

In many Western societies the link between alcohol and mood is deeply rooted, and this cultural knowledge is aptly illustrated by Bukowski’s [1, p. 373] assertion that “If something bad happens you drink in an attempt to forget; if something good happens you drink in order to celebrate; and if nothing happens you drink to make something happen.” Not content with leaving the relationship between mood and alcohol consumption to the writers and philosophers, scientists have, for many years, sought to investigate this association. As a result, a large body of research and theory that seeks to shed light on the extent to which people drink because of their mood has accrued. However, research findings have been mixed and attempts to synthesise this literature meta-analytically have been limited to a study examining negative affect in laboratory settings [2]. This found a small-to-moderate effect of negative mood inductions on alcohol consumption in research that was conducted in contexts that differ markedly from those in which people typically drink. Therefore, the current systematic review and meta-analyses aimed to extend this formative work by focussing on positive as well as negative affect and examining methodically empirical findings garnered from studies carried out in both laboratory and in real world settings.

Both the self-medication hypothesis [3] and the negative reinforcement model of alcohol use [4] theoretically posit a direct association between diminished mental wellbeing and hazardous drinking [5], whereby alcohol is used as a means of improving low mood. While these theoretical models were initially developed to help understand heavy consumption in clinical populations [6,7], they have also been applied to explain excessive drug and alcohol use in non-clinical settings with a view of preventing the development of substance use disorders (e.g., [810]). In a similar vein, tension-reduction [11] and stress-response dampening theories [12] construe the experience of negative affect as a risk factor for problematic consumption. The decision to drink (or to exercise restraint), according to these theories, is a product of people’s affective experiences, and alcohol consumption is understood temporally [1214] as an outcome of their preceding (negative) mood.

Empirical findings examining the mood-alcohol nexus have, however, been varied. While some laboratory studies indicate that negative affect is associated with increased alcohol-related attentional bias [15,16], others suggest that it is not related to alcohol consumption [1719]. Similarly, some real-time studies find no association between negative affect and unplanned heavy drinking [20], while others suggest that negative affect is inversely related to drinking onset and further intoxication [2125]. The existing literature therefore provides mixed support for negative affect regulation theories of alcohol use.

In a similar way, there are inconsistent findings with regards to the positive reinforcement theory of alcohol use [26], according to which people drink alcohol to enhance the positive emotions they are experiencing. As such, while real-time studies indicate that positive mood is associated with increased drinking likelihood and breath alcohol concentration later that day [21,2325,27], a study using questionnaire design found that having difficulties with regulating positive emotions was linked with drug but not alcohol misuse [28]. Studies conducted in the laboratory also produced mixed findings: while Stein et al. found that positive mood induction increases consumption [29], VandeVeen et al. found no such effect [30]. Given the variation in support for both negative and positive reinforcement theoretical models, formal interrogation of discrepant findings derived from both laboratory and field settings is required to shed light on the somewhat elusive association between affective states and alcohol consumption. It is therefore necessary to meta-analytically examine whether inconsistent findings may be due to power limitations of individual studies.

In addition to overcoming power concerns by combining effect sizes of studies, theoretical and methodological differences between investigations need to be examined systematically to help clarify inconsistencies in this body of work. The first reason for mixed findings may relate to how affect is conceptualised and measured. Theoretically, there are two perspectives on affective state: while some research operationalises affect as a singular concept and measures this on a continuum that is anchored between positive and negative affect (e.g., [31,32]), in other work current mood is treated as a unidimensional construct in which different affective states are unable to overlap simultaneously (e.g., [33,34]). This has methodological implications as the former conceptualisation of affect necessitates the use of measures that assess mood on a continuous scale, with scope for variability in valence and arousal (e.g., mood circumplex, [35]; UMACL, [36]; affect grid, [37]), while the latter perspective typically uses separate assessments of intensity of negative and positive affect (e.g., PANAS, [38]; VAMS, [39]). These theoretical and methodological considerations are important as they also have consequences for how study findings are interpreted. If affect is understood and measured as a continuous construct, evidence suggesting that positive affect increases consumption (in line with the positive reinforcement model) would contradict the negative reinforcement model. On the other hand, if positive and negative affect are assessed as discrete entities, then evidence for one theory would not necessarily contradict the other and it may be beneficial to combine these approaches into a more parsimonious model. When taking stock of this literature it is therefore essential to consider how measurement choices and theoretical conceptualisations of affect may impact results.

A second potential methodological reason for divergent findings in this research area centres on whether the studies examine distinct emotions or look at overall levels of affect. An important debate in the literature concerns whether emotions ought to be conceptualised along dimensions of valence and arousal [40,41] or as discrete entities [42,43]. In the alcohol literature, the first approach, where researchers measure mood scores (e.g., PANAS, [38]) as an average of various adjectives related to either negative or positive emotions (e.g., [27,4446]), has been more commonly utilised. Yet, an alternative approach, which adopts a discrete model of emotions, analyses each affect item individually. O’Donnell et al., for example, examined how stress and irritation impact consumption [47], while Dvorak and Simons [9] as well as Shadur et al. [33] looked at how anxiety and sadness affect drinking likelihood, and Rohsenow et al. [48] examined how anxiety, anger, and depression influence the number of alcohol units consumed. Considering that emotions differ in terms of arousal [35,49,50] and physiology [43], it is possible that the widely adopted methodology of averaging affective states may have inadvertently contributed to a homogenisation of different facets of emotions. It therefore needs to be examined whether the process of collapsing distinct emotions into positive and negative affect scores may have led researchers to miss the nuanced ways in which these emotions shape alcohol consumption differentially, or whether combining them into a single score for negative and positive affect is appropriate.

The third methodological variation between studies that may systematically impact results relates to their design. On the one hand, daily diary and ecological momentary assessment (EMA)/experience sampling methods (ESM) studies have enabled researchers to minimise retrospection bias [51,52] and to examine the behaviour in question in naturalistic settings. Here, participants are instructed to record in structured ways events/feelings that occurred during the day. However, such studies occur in uncontrolled environments, and it is therefore possible that findings may be impacted by extraneous factors that are not captured by the research methods used in these studies. Laboratory studies, on the other hand, typically utilise ad-libitum drinking paradigms, where participants can consume as much or as little alcohol as they wish. While having a controlled environment is advantageous, participants may feel obliged to drink alcohol [53,54] or may not be offered their typical beverage of choice (alcoholic or non-alcoholic, [55,56]) and therefore might not accurately reflect real-world drinking behaviours. The potential for study design to moderate the association between affect and alcohol consumption therefore needs to be considered meta-analytically.

Accordingly, the present pre-registered systematic review and meta-analyses aimed to synthesise findings on the impact of affective states on alcohol consumption in non-clinical populations by addressing these gaps, while accounting for potential sources of variability. Specifically, following the self-medication hypothesis and tension-reduction theory, which postulate that increases in negative affect predict substance use within a short timeframe [13,14], we examined the impact of affective states on same day consumption. In consideration of the suggestion that negative and positive affect may be distinct experiences [57,58] separate models were used in the analyses. Laboratory studies examining negative affect were analyzed separately prior to the main analysis (which examined both laboratory and field studies) to test for any causal association. For both negative and positive affect models, alcohol measure (e.g., number of drinks or units), affect conceptualization (i.e., whether studies treated negative and positive affective states as a continuum or separate entities and whether studies looked at distinct emotions or averaged them) and study design (i.e., laboratory or field research) were examined systematically as possible methodological moderators of the affect-alcohol relationship. A series of exploratory analyses of other variables (year of publication, country, study quality) was also undertaken. The results of two meta-analyses (on negative affect and on positive affect) were then compared to establish whether negative and positive affect are differentially associated with alcohol consumption volume.

Methods

Operational definitions

Alcohol consumption is defined as ingesting any beverage containing ethanol. Mood and emotions are distinct but related constructs in that the former tend to be more stable and ‘flat’, while the latter are construed as more vivid and quick [59]. However, studies sometimes use these terms interchangeably. While it is possible that mood and emotions have different effects on alcohol consumption volume over longer periods of time, the current focus was on the effects of within a shorter timeframe, where the distinction between mood and emotions is arguably less important. Therefore, to account for differences in the terminology, the terms ‘affect’, and ‘affective state’ are used in this review as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling. We use the term ‘field studies’ for real-time studies, diary studies or studies using telephone interviews.

Eligibility criteria

The literature search was primarily conducted by the lead author. To avoid missing data, the second author conducted a comparative title search using the same criteria to ensure the incorporation of any studies which may have been overlooked in the original review. Full-text papers of any titles and abstracts that were considered relevant were obtained where possible. The relevance of each study was assessed according to the following inclusion criteria (pre-registered on Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/8bngj): peer-reviewed papers; grey literature; focus on the general human population (non-clinical sample); looking at affective states on the day of and prior to consumption; looking at consumption volume during the day (e.g., amount consumed in millilitres, numbers of drinks); papers in English or Russian. The exclusion criteria were as follows: reviews, books, posters, and editorials; literature examining clinical samples (individuals with alcohol use disorders or any other clinical disorder).

Both studies that measured affect as a continuum (i.e., where positive and negative affect are at polar ends of the same assessment spectrum), or as separate entities were included in the review. Furthermore, studies that examined mean levels of affect (i.e., average negative or positive affect) as well as affect facets (i.e., specific emotions, e.g., stress, anger, or happiness) were included. To account for varied methods of assessment, both laboratory and field studies were included. Methodological differences (treating affect as continuum or separate entities, examining mean levels of affect or distinct emotions, alcohol measure used, laboratory or field studies) were included in analysis as moderators.

Literature review

A comprehensive search was conducted of the following databases: PsychINFO, PsycARTICLES, Science Direct, PubMed, SCOPUS, JSTOR using Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA, [60]) and American Psychological Association’s Meta-Analysis Reporting Standards (MARS; [61,62]) methodologies. The following commands were used for searching: ("alcohol " OR "drinking behavi*r”) AND ("mood" OR "emotions" OR “feelings” OR "affective states") NOT "disorders". The search was conducted on 2nd March 2020. For PsychINFO, after the filters ‘empirical study’ and ‘quantitative study’ were applied, the search yielded 8285 articles for screening. For PsychARTICLES, when the same filters were applied, the search yielded three articles. For Science Direct, as wildcards “*” were not supported, the search terms were ("alcohol " OR "drinking behavior” OR “drinking behaviour”) AND ("mood" OR "emotions" OR “feelings” OR "affective states") NOT "disorders". After the filter ‘research articles’ was applied, the search yielded 2327 articles. For PubMed (3189 citations), SCOPUS (1201 citations), and JSTOR (367 citations), no filters were applied. The citations were loaded to RefWorks software, and the duplicates were removed. Bibliographies from relevant reviews and book chapters, as well as articles that fit the inclusion criteria, were manually searched for additional citations. To ensure that all relevant literature published at the time was covered, a supplementary search was conducted on 29th January 2021, which yielded 3 additional references.

To obtain grey literature, Google Scholar and Open Science Framework were searched. We also contacted the labs that conduct studies on the topics of affect and alcohol consumption. However, only one grey literature study (a study from our own lab) was included in the review, as other available studies did not fit the inclusion criteria (e.g., did not examine same day affect and alcohol consumption).

Quality assessment and data extraction

Study quality was assessed using standard criteria [63], with papers screened by two independent reviewers (Cohen’s Kappa = .71). Each paper was rated on the following criteria (each criterion assessed on a scale from 0 to 2): justification of research question, justification of study deigns, appropriate method of study selection, robustness of the measures, sample size justification, appropriateness of analytic methods, estimation of variance, control for confounds, results being reported in sufficient details, and conclusion being supported by results, with a maximum quality score of 22. Scores of 1–10 were considered to be poor quality, those that scored 11–15 were deemed to be of moderate quality, and studies with scores of 16–20 were classified as being of good quality, with manuscripts scoring 21–22 bring considered to be excellent quality. None of the studies were judged to be of poor quality (and hence none were excluded based on this), while there were 20 studies of moderate quality, 36 that were good quality, and two that were deemed to be of excellent quality.

Following the quality assessment, relevant data were extracted from each study (see Table 1 for full summary). For subset of laboratory studies on negative affect, Cohen’s d statistics was extracted (by calculating the mean difference between the two groups, and then dividing results by pooled standard deviation, or converted from F value, [64]), along with corresponding standard error. Correlation coefficients (r) were extracted either from correlation tables (N = 26 for negative affect, N = 19 for positive affect), obtained from the authors (N = 2 for negative affect, N = 2 for positive affect), or converted from available statistics (N = 24 for negative affect, N = 13 for positive affect), such as standardised beta weights (using formula provided by Peterson and Brown [65]), converted from d obtained from unstandardised beta weight and pooled standard deviation [66] or from sample size, means, and F-test [67]. When necessary, we changed the direction of correlation coefficient to ensure that each effect size reflected the relation between higher levels of affect and higher consumption volume. The standard error for each effect size was calculated using the following formula: SE(r) = √1-r2/N-2 (following [68]), while the variance was obtained by squaring the standard error.

Table 1. Characteristics of included studies.

Number Authors Year Country Method Sample size(s) Sample type Sample gender(s) Sample age(s) Positive or negative Affect measure Looked at distinct emotions (Yes/No) Alcohol measure Effect size (r) Relevant key findings
1 Austin, Notebaert, Wiers, Salemink, & MacLeod [80] 2020 Netherlands Experimental, ad libitum taste test 39 General population 24 women M = 24.82, SD = 7.49, range = 18–59 Negative VAMS [39] No Ad libitum (grams of beverage consumed) r = .72 Beer consumption was higher in the negative affect induction condition.
2 Cyders, Zapolski, Combs, Settles, Fillmore, & Smith [81] 2010 USA Experimental study, ad libitum taste test 33 Undergraduate psychology students 14 women M = 22.27, SD = 2.36, range not reported Positive PANAS [38] No Ad libitum (millilitres of beverage consumed) r = .40 Participants drank more in the positive mood than in the neutral mood condition.
3 de Castro [82] 1990 USA Diary study 96 General population 63 women M = 32.9, SD not reported, range = 21–54 Both, as a continuum 7-point Likert scale (elated—depressed, anxious—calm scales) No Record everything you drink r (elation-depression scale) = .33; r (anxious-calm scale) = .17 The degree of elation, not depression, was related to the amount of alcohol ingested. On the calm-anxiety scale, people were mostly drinking when they felt ’neutral’.
4 Dinc & Cooper [83] 2015 UK Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 106 Undergraduate psychology students 60 women M = 23.915, SD = 6.73, range not reported Positive UMACL [36] No Ad libitum (millilitres of beverage consumed) r = .43 People consumed more alcohol when in a positive mood compared to a neutral mood.
5 Duif, Thewissen, Wouters, Lechner, & Jacobs [44] 2019 Netherlands Experience Sampling Method 162 General population 109 women M = 36.07, SD = 9.27, range = 20–50 Both PANAS [38] No Number of drinks Within persons: r (daily negative affect) = .01; r (momentary negative affect) = —.03; r (daily positive affect) = .03; r (momentary positive affect) = .04. Between persons: r (daily negative affect) = .01, r (momentary negative affect) = .03; r (daily positive affect) = .01; r (momentary positive affect) = .01. Negative affect was not associated with the amount consumed. Higher levels of positive affect were associated with more consumption.
6 Gabel, Noel, Keane & Lisman [84] 1980 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test 18 Undergraduate psychology students Men only M & SD not reported, range = 18–22 Both Self-report; basal skin conductance; heart rate Yes: sexual arousal, fear, neutral Ad libitum (millilitres of beverage consumed) r (negative) = .11; r (positive) = .65 Participants in the sexual arousal condition drank more than in fear or neutral conditions.
7 Gautreau, Sherry, Battista, Goldstein, & Stewart [85] 2015 Canada Experience Sampling Method 143 Frequent drinkers 105 women M = 20.78, SD = 3.36, range not reported Positive PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (low arousal) = .03; r (high arousal) = .26 High-arousal positive mood was associated with higher number of drinks than low-arousal positive mood.
8 Liu, Wang, Zhan, & Shi [87] 2009 China Telephone Interviews 37 General population 5 women M = 31, SD = 8.01, range not reported Negative Work stress checklist [88] Yes—stress Number of units r = .16 Stress was associated with increased alcohol consumption.
9 Mohr, Arpin & McCabe [89] 2015 USA Experience Sampling Method 47 Moderate-to-heavy drinkers 23 women M = 36, SD = 16.98, range not reported Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .01; r (positive) = .04 Affect variability, not mean levels of affect, was associated with elevated consumption.
10 Mohr, Brannan, Wendt, Jacobs, Wright, & Wang [90] 2013 USA Experience Sampling Method 49 Moderate-to-heavy drinkers 24 women M = 36, SD = 17.32, range not reported Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (positive) = -.16 Participants drank more following increases in both positive and negative mood.
11 Monk, Qureshi, & Heim [32] 2020 UK Experience Sampling Method 69 General population 42 women M = 21.47, SD = 4.47, range = 18–36 Both, as a continuum How would you rate your current mood (on a scale from 0 to 5)? No Number of drinks r (negative) = -.31; r (positive) = .31 Feeling unhappy prior to the commencement of drinking was a significant predictor of drinking larger quantities of alcohol in the following drinking session.
12 O’Donnel et al. [47] 2019 Australia Experience Sampling Method 83 General population 63 women M = 21.42, SD = 3.09, range = 18–30 Both (happy, relaxed, irritated, stressed) Happy, relaxed, irritated, stressed, on a 6-point scale Yes (happy, relaxed, irritated, stressed) Number of drinks r (happy) = .09; r (relaxed) = .10; r (stressed) = -.09; r (irritated) = -.04 Affect was not related to levels of consumption.
13 Peacock, Cash, Bruno, & Ferguson [91] 2015 Australia Experience Sampling Method 53 General population 22 women M = 28.2, SD = 11.2, range = 18–60 Both, as a continuum Visual-analogue Mood Scales [35] No Number of drinks r (negative) = -.19; r (positive) = .19 Higher positive affect was associated with increased alcohol consumption.
14 Pihl & Yankofsky [92] 1979 Canada Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 40 General population Men only M = 20.05, SD not reported, range = 18–27 Negative MAACL [93] Yes (depression and anxiety) Ad libitum (total amount of pure alcohol consumed) r = .29 Less alcohol was consumed by participants who experienced higher depression and anxiety prior to consumption.
15 Richardson, Hoene, & Rigatti [45] 2020 USA Diary study 222 University students 149 women M = 20.12, SD = 2.58, range not reported Both PANAS [38] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .11; r (positive) = .17 At low levels of positive affect, individuals higher in self- critical perfectionism reported higher levels of drinking to cope than those lower in self-critical perfectionism. Individuals were also more likely to drink to cope with high negative affect compared to low negative affect.
16 Rohsenow [94] 1982 USA Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 60 Undergraduate students Men only M = 23, SD not reported, range = 21–32 Negative MAACL [93] Yes (anxiety) Ad libitum (amount consumed in mls, average sip size, total number of sips) r = .31 Those feeling more anxious took fewer sips of alcohol.
17 Simons, Gaher, Oliver, Bush, & Palmer [95] 2005 USA Experience Sampling Method 56 Moderate drinkers 30 women M = 21.5, SD = .57, range = 21–23 Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .03; r (positive) = .16 Both negative and positive affect were associated with greater consumption volume.
18 Simons, Wills, & Neal [96] 2014 USA Experience Sampling Method 274 Moderate-to-heavy drinkers 153 women M = 19.88, SD = 1.37, range = 18–27 Both PANAS-X [97] and mood circumplex model [86] No Number of drinks + transdermal alcohol monitoring r (negative) = .11; r (positive) = -.13 Daily negative affect was directly associated with higher consumption on drinking days.
19 Stasiewicz & Lisman [98] 1989 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 32 Men in risk for future child abuse Men only M = 20.6, SD & range not reported Negative Blood pressure and heart rate Yes (aversion, arousal, distress) Ad libitum (millilitres of beverage consumed) r = .50 Higher aversion, arousal, and distress were associated with higher consumption.
20 Stevenson, Dvorak, Kramer, Peterson, Dunn, Leary, & Pinto [99] 2019 USA Experience Sampling Method 101 College students 66 women M = 20.93, SD = 2.89, range = 18–29 Both PANAS [38] Yes (depression, anxiety) Number of drinks r (anxious) = .08; r (depressed) = -.01; r (positive) = .02 Intending to drink to enhance one’s mood was associated with increased consumption volume.
21 Sutker, Libet, Allain, & Randall [100] 1983 USA Diary study 32 General population 21 women Not reported Negative MAACL [93] Yes (anxiety, depression, hostility) Number of drinks r (anxiety) = .07, r (depression) = -.10; r (hostility) = -.02 Negative affect was not associated with consumption.
22 Swendsen, Tennen, Carney, Affleck, Willard, & Hromi [14] 2000 France Experience Sampling Method 100 Frequent drinkers 55 women M = 22.9, SD = 4.6, range not reported Both Mood Circumplex [86] Yes (active, peppy, happy, relaxed, quiet, bored, sad, nervous) Type & Quantity of beverage r (active) = .11; r (peppy) = —.05; r (happy) = .21; r (relaxed) = .12; r (quiet) = .32; r (bored) = .06; r (sad) = .01; r (nervous) = .30 Happy and nervous affective states were associated with increased consumption, feeling quiet was associated with decreased consumption.
23 Wardell, Read, Curtin, & Merrill [101] 2012 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 146 College students 67 women M = 21.45, SD = .73, range = 21–24 Both, as a continuum Affect Grid [37] No Ad libitum (oz of beverage consumed, blood alcohol concentration) r (negative) = -.07; r (positive) = .07 Mood was not associated with consumption.
24 Mohr et al. [102] 2005 USA Experience Sampling Method 122 Undergraduate students 69 women M = 18.9, SD = 1.16, range not reported Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number and volume of drinks r (negative) = .08; r (positive) = .07 Both positive and negative mood were positively associated with consumption volume.
25 Hussong, Galloway, & Feagans [103] 2005 USA Experience Sampling Method 72 College students 36 women M = 18.10, SD not reported, range not reported Both PANAS [38] Yes (fear, hostility, attentiveness, sadness, shyness) Number of drinks r (fear) = -.08; r (hostility) = .17; r (attentiveness) = -.10; r (sadness) = .18; r (shyness) = -.05 Affect interacted with drinking motives to predict consumption: those high in drinking-to-cope motives drank less on days in which they experienced greater sadness. When experiencing moderate to high levels of fear and shyness, individuals high in drinking-to-cope were more likely to drink. For those low in coping motivations, fear and shyness did not predict daily drinking.
26 Todd, Armeli, Tennen, Carney, & Affleck [104] 2003 USA Diary study 83 Community sample 44 women M = 37.2, SD = 6.65, range not reported Negative Perceived stress scale [105] and mood circumplex [86] Yes (anger, boredom, loneliness, nervousness, sadness) Number, size, and proof of drinks Study 1. r (angry) = .05; r (bored) = -.03; r (lonely) = -.10; r (nervous) = -.06; r (sad) = -.05. Study 2. r (angry) = 0; r (bored) = .02; r (lonely) = .05; r (nervous) = -.06; r (sad) = -.06 Associations between stress/negative affect and drinking outcome variable tend to be near zero for individuals with high drinking-to-cope scores and negative for individuals with low drinking-to-cope scores.
27 Carney, Armeli, Tennen, Affleck, & O’Neil [106] 2000 USA Diary study 83 Community sample 44 women M = 37.15, SD = 6.65, range = 26.01–50.76 Negative Perceived Stress Scale [105] Yes (stress) Number, size, and proof of drinks r = .27 Perceived stress was associated with increased consumption.
28 Armeli, Carney, Tennen, Affleck, & O’Neil [107] 2000 USA Diary study 88 Moderate drinkers 48 women M = 37.81, SD = 6.92, range not reported Negative Modified version of the Assessment of Daily Experience [108] Yes (stress) Number of drinks r = -.23 Men who more strongly anticipated positive outcomes or a sense of carelessness from drinking drank relatively more on stressful days compared with low-stress days. Men who anticipated greater impairment from drinking drank relatively less on stressful days. These effects did not hold for women.
29 Dvorak, Pearson, & Day [109] 2014 USA Experience Sampling Method 74 University students 43 women M = 21.30, SD = 2.07, range = 18–29 Both How ___ are you feeling right now (on a scale from 1 to 11)? No Number of drinks r (negative) = .17; r (positive) = -.14 Negative daytime mood was associated with increased consumption, positive daytime mood was not associated with consumption.
30 Thomas, Merrill, von Hofe, & Magid [110] 2014 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 112 Frequent drinkers 52 women M = 27.0, SD = 5.16, range not reported Negative Subjective units of distress scale; heart rate; mean arterial pressure; and salivary cortisol. Yes (stress) Ad libitum (millilitres of beverage consumed,latency to first sip of beer, average sip size, median latency between sips) r = .10 The stressor did not result in greater consumption of alcohol.
31 Grant, Stewart, & Mohr [111] 2009 Canada Experience Sampling Method 146 College students 113 women M, SD, & range not reported Negative How ___ did you feel today (on a scale from 0 to 4)? Yes (depression and anxiety) Number of drinks r (depressed) = -.07; r (anxious) = -.03 Daily depressed mood did not trigger subsequent evening alcohol consumption and daily anxious mood was protective against subsequent evening drinking.
32 Steptoe & Wardle [112] 1999 UK Diary study 79 Nurses and teachers 45 women M = 39.75, SD = 9.95, range not reported Both POMS [113] Yes (anxiety) Number of units r (negative) = .58; r (positive) = .58 Consumption of alcohol tended to be greater on days on which participants reported more positive and less anxious mood.
33 Hull & Young [114] 1983 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 120 Frequent drinkers Men only Over 21, M, SD & range not reported Negative MAACL [93] Yes (anxiety, hostility, depression) Ad libitum (ounces of beverage consumed) In high self-conscious subjects: r (anxious) = .44; r (hostile) = .10, r (depressed) = .09. In low self-conscious subjects r (anxious) = .09, r (hostile) = .10, r (depressed) = .09 Negative mood was related to consumption volume in high self-conscious but not low self-conscious participants.
34 O’Hara, Armeli, & Tennen [115] 2014 USA Diary study 1636 College students 867 women M = 19.2, SD & range not reported Both Items from Mood Circumplex [86] and PANAS-X [97] Yes (sadness, anxiety) Number of drinks r (anxiety) = -.06; r (anger) = .00; r (sadness) = -.20; r (positive mood) = .02 Anxiety, anger, and positive mood were positively related to the number of drinks consumed.
35 Todd et al. [116] 2005 USA Experience Sampling Method 98 Community sample 49 women M = 43.5, SD & range not reported Both Single-item mood measure: How ___ did you feel (on a scale from 0 to 4)? Yes (peppy, happy, relaxed, bored, sad, nervous, angry, lonely, disappointed) Number, size, and proof of drinks r (peppy) = .01; r (happy) = .05; r (relaxed) = .14; r (positive mood) = .09; r (bored) = .13; r (sad) = .07; r (nervous) = -.01; r (angry) = .05; r (lonely) = .06; r (disappointed) = .00; r (negative moods) = .05 There was no significant association between mood and alcohol consumption.
36 Todd, Armeli, & Tennen [117] 2009 USA Experience Sampling Method 97 Community sample 48 women M = 43.5, SD & range not reported Both Mood Circumplex [86] Yes (angry, bored, disappointed, lonely, nervous, sad) Number, size, and proof of drinks r (angry) = .02; r (bored) = .11; r (disappointed) = -.01; r (lonely) = .04; r (nervous) = -.02; r (sad) = .07; r (negative mood) = .04; r (positive mood) = .08 Affective state was not associated with consumption.
37 Collins et al. [118] 1998 USA Experience Sampling Method 37 Heavy drinkers 15 women M = 35.92, SD & range not reported Both Mood Circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .97; r (positive) = .97 Positive but not negative mood predicted excessive drinking.
38 Ehrenberg, Armeli, Howland, & Tennen [119] 2016 USA Experience Sampling Method 722 College students 391 women M = 19.24, SD & range not reported Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .06; r (positive) = .01 Consumption level was unrelated to negative affect and positively related to positive affect.
39 Higgins & Marlatt [120] 1975 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test 64 Undergraduate psychology students Men only M & SD not reported, range = 18–26 Negative MAACL [93] Yes (fear of evaluation) Ad libitum (ounces of beverage consumed and amount of pure alcohol consumed) r = .35 Participants expecting to be evaluated drank significantly more alcohol than low-fear control participants.
40 Holroyd [121] 1978 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test 60 Undergraduate students Men only Over 18, M, SD, & range not reported Negative State Anxiety Scale Yes (social anxiety) Ad libitum (numbers of bottles of beer opened, blood alcohol concentration) r = .21 Socially anxious participants and those who received negative social evaluation drank less alcohol.
41 Dvorak, Pearson, Sargent, Stevenson, & Mfon [122] 2016 USA Experience Sampling Method 74 University students 43 women M = 21.30, SD & range not reported Both PANAS-X [97] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .10; r (positive) = .09 Higher positive mood and mood instability were associated with increased consumption.
42 Armeli et al. [123] 2007 USA Experience Sampling Method 98 Heavy drinkers 49 women M = 43.5, SD & reported Both PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] Yes (stress) Number of drinks r (negative) = .06; r (positive) = .06 Higher levels of stress and negative affect interacted with individual differences factors to predict increased consumption.
43 Corcoran & Parker [124] 1991 USA Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 69 Undergraduate students 25 women M, SD, & range not reported Negative Personal Evaluation Form Yes (stress) Ad libitum (amount of beverage consumed in ounces) r = .17 Stress was not associated with consumption.
44 Kidorf & Lang [125] 1999 USA Experimental, ad libitum taste test; mood induction; within-subject 84 Undergraduate students 42 women M = 22.5, SD & range not reported Negative MAACL [93] Yes (stress) Ad libitum (amount of pure alcohol consumed) r = .08 Stress was positively related to consumption.
45 Tucker, Vuchinich, Sobell, & Maisto [126] 1980 USA Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 43 Heavy social drinkers Men only M & SD not reported, range = 18–26 Negative How anxious did you feel (on a scale from 1 to 7)? Yes (stress) Ad libitum (milliliters of alcohol consumed) r = —.70 Stress was positively related to consumption.
46 McGrath, Jones, & Field [127] 2016 UK Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 100 Heavy social drinkers 52 women M = 20.86; SD = 3.93, range not reported Negative POMS [112] Yes (stress) Ad libitum (milliliters of beverage consumed) r = .21 Stress was positively related to consumption.
47 Magrys & Olmstead [128] 2015 Canada Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 75 Undergraduate students 40 women M = 20.12, SD & range not reported Negative STAI [129] Yes (stress) Ad libitum (number of standard drinks consumed, level of intoxication, blood alcohol level) r = .09 Stress was positively related to consumption.
48 Aldridge-Gerry et al. [130] 2011 USA Experience Sampling Method 365 College students 252 women M = 20.1, SD & range not reported Negative Describe the most stressful event that happened that day and rate on a 5-point scale how stressful it was. Yes (stress) Number of drinks r = -.05 Stress was negatively related to consumption.
49 Emery & Simons [131] 2020 USA Experience Sampling Method 92 Moderate-to-heavy drinkers, undergraduate students 58 women M = 20.17, SD & range not reported Both PANAS-X [97] and mood circumplex [86] No Number of drinks r (negative) = .10; r (positive) = .15 Positive affect was positively associated with consumption. Negative affect was not associated with consumption.
50 Hamilton, Armeli, & Tennen [132] 2020 USA Diary study, 2 waves 906 College students 489 women M = 19.18, SD = 1.26 = > M = 24.56, SD = 1.33, ranges not reported Both PANAS [38] and Mood Circumplex [86] No Number of standard drinks Wave 1: r (social positive) = .01; r (social negative) = -.06; r (solitary positive) = -.03; r (solitary negative) = .11. Wave 2: r (social positive) = .01; r (social negative) = .00; r (solitary positive) = -.07; r (solitary negative) = -.04 Whereas daytime positive affect predicted greater social consumption, it was also related to lower solitary alcohol consumption among college students who were low in state social drinking motives.
51 Tovmasyan, Monk, Bunting, Qureshi, & Heim Under revision UK Experience Sampling Method 79 General population 49 women M = 29.31, SD = 9.70, range = 20–63 Both PANAS [38] Yes (all items from PANAS, [38]) Number of drinks r (between day positive) = -.05; r (between day negative) = .01; r (within day positive) = -.03; r (within day negative) = .03 Being more enthusiastic and less alert was associated with drinking onset, being ashamed was associated with higher number of drinks following drinking onset, feeling strong and interested was associated with decreased drinking volume.
52 Stamates, Linden-Carmichael, Preonas, & Lau-Barraco [46] 2019 USA Experience Sampling Method 24 Adult drinkers 14 women M = 23.83, SD = 1.83, range not reported Both PANAS [38] No Number of standardised drinks r (negative) = .41; r (positive) = -.09 Higher negative affect was inversely related to number of drinks consumed.
53 Mohr, Brannan, Mohr, Armeli, & Tennen [133] 2008 USA Diary study 118 College students 67 women M = 18.9, SD = 1.16, range not reported Both Combination of PANAS [38] and mood circumplex [86] Yes (angry, sad, bored, nervous, ashamed, hostile, guilty, jittery, dejected) Number of drinks Drinking at home: r (angry) = .21; r (sad) = .04; r (bored) = .15; r (nervous) = –.05; r (ashamed) = .21; r (hostile) = .27; r (guilty) = .12; r (jittery) = .07, r (dejected) = .12, r (positive mood) = .05. Drinking away: r (angry) = .04, r (sad) = –.07; r (bored) = .05; r (nervous) = –.04; r (ashamed) = .12; r (hostile) = .13; r (guilty) = .05; r (jittery) = –.02; r (dejected) = .01; r (positive mood) = .09 Both positive and negative were associated with higher consumption volume.
54 Schroder & Perrine [134] 2007 USA Interactive Voice Response 173 General population 81 women M = 42.3, SD = 11.9, range = 21–74 Both Rate stress, anger, sadness, happiness, quality of the day (the best day I had, the worst day I had) on an 11-point scale Yes (stress, anger, sadness, happiness) Number of standard drinks Between-subject: r (stress) = -.08; r (anger) = -.05; r (sadness) = -.04; r (happiness) = -.10; r (negative emotions) = -.06; r (positive emotions) = -.12. Within-subject: r (sadness) = -.02; r (anger) = -.01; r (stress) = -.03; r (happiness) = .10, r (negative emotions) = -.02; r (positive emotions) = .12 Among women, those with higher average levels of sadness, anger, and stress reported higher levels of alcohol consumption; among men, those with higher negative mood ratings reported significantly less alcohol consumption. When not separated by gender, on both within- and between-participant levels, correlations of mood and drinking did not differ significantly from zero.
55 Waddell, Sher, & Piasecki [135] 2021 USA Experience Sampling Method 403 General population 202 women M = 23.3, SD = 7.2, range = 18–70 Negative 5-point Likert scale of distressed and sad Yes (distressed, sad) Number of drinks r = .07 Negative affect did not predict consumption directly but did so through alcohol craving.
56 Larsen, Engels, Granic, & Huizink [136] 2013 Netherlands Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 106 Unversity students Men only M = 21.37, SD = 2.32, range = 18–27 Negative Physiological Arousal Questionnaire (PAQ; [137]) Yes (stress) Ad libitum (centilitres alcohol consumed r = .14 There was no difference in alcohol consumed between stress and no-stress conditions.
57 Sacco et al. [138] 2015 USA Telephone interviews 71 Continuing care retirement community 45 women M = over 80, SD and range not reported Both PANAS-S [139] No Number of standard drinks r (negative) = -.08; r (positive) = .07 No temporal relationship between negative and positive affect and amount consumed.
58 Lindgren et al. [140] 2018 Experimental study, ad libitum taste test; mood induction 149 University students 71 women M = 21.55, SD = .68, range = 21–25 Both 6-Item Brief Affect Measure [141] No Ad libitum (amount in mls) r (negative) = .07, r (positive) = -.07 Implicit alcohol excite associations were more negatively associated with drinking in negative mood condition and more positively associated with drinking in positive/neutral mood condition.

Meta-analyses

Analytical strategy

Prior to the main analysis, laboratory studies which provided Cohen’s d and its standard error (or other statistics from which these numbers could be calculated) were analysed separately. This was only done for studies examining the impact of negative affect on alcohol consumption (n = 14), as there were only two eligible laboratory studies on positive affect for this analysis. Random effects model was fitted in R Studio [69] version 1.4.1106 using metagen function of the meta [70] package.

Pearson’s r correlation coefficients were used as the effect size for the main meta-analyses, with generic inverse-variance pooling to combine correlations from different studies into one pooled correlation estimate. As Pearson’s r is not normally distributed, effect sizes were first converted to Fisher’s z using the following formula: (z = ½ ln ((1+r)/(1-r)). After the analysis, the coefficients were converted back to Pearson’s r (following [68]).

Some data sets provided multiple correlations between constructs of interest (e.g., the correlation between sadness and consumption and anger and consumption, or both within- and between-person associations; N = 25 for negative affect, N = 16 for positive affect). Given that including more than one effect size from a study violates the assumption of independence, we used the robust variance estimation (RVE; [71]) method to control for dependencies between effect sizes. Because correlations between the effect sizes reported within each study were not known, we assumed a Spearman’s rho (ρ) of .80 [71]. We also performed a series of sensitivity analyses by testing different values of ρ in intervals of .10. This did not affect inferences about effect sizes; therefore, these results are not reported in the paper. Correlated effects model with small-sample corrections was fitted in R Studio [69] using robumeta [72] package. Heterogeneity was assessed using I2 and τ2 statistics.

To assess potential publication bias, we conducted the Egger’s test [73], which was performed by regressing effect estimates against their standard errors. If the slope for the regression line is significant, that would suggest publication bias. Additionally, selection model analysis was performed using JASP ([74], following Bartoš et al. [75]). P-curve analysis was also performed in the online app (http://p-curve.com/) to assess potential p-hacking [76].

Several categorical moderators were examined: study quality, country, study design (laboratory vs field), alcohol consumption measure (e.g., number of millilitres consumed during the day, number of drinks consumed during the day), whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. The effects of categorical moderators (i.e., country and study design) were assessed using meta-regression approach, as suggested by Harrer et al. [77]. Additionally, year of publication was a continuous moderator, which was examined using metatest [78] package. While examining the differences between the sample types was initially planned, it was deemed inappropriate due to inconsistent reporting—for example, many studies included anyone who was not diagnosed with alcohol use disorder, while others only included heavy drinkers. Thus, there is a potential overlap between sample types of different studies. As mean AUDIT scores were not reported consistently, we decided not to include sample type as a moderator.

The results of meta-analyses of negative and positive affect were then compared using Cohen’s q statistic [79] by imputing the obtained correlations to online calculator (https://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html, Lenhard & Lenhard. [Unpublished]). This method transforms correlation coefficients to z scores and then subtracts them.

Data and R scripts for the analyses are available on the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/fe9au/files/.

Results

Quantity of research available

Electronic and hand search identified 15372 articles, which, once duplicates were removed, left 2472 unique citations to be screened for inclusion (Fig 1). Their titles and abstracts were assessed for their relevance to the review, resulting in 22 potential articles being retained. The full texts of all but three studies were obtained. After applying exclusion criteria for the remaining full-text papers, nine articles were excluded; the most common reason for exclusion was that the studies did not look at affective state on the day and prior to consumption. After that, full texts of eligible articles were screened to obtain additional citations. This resulted in screening 264 additional articles. All but 22 were retrieved. After applying exclusion criteria for the remaining full-text papers, 206 were excluded; the most common reason for exclusion was that studies did not examine the variables of interest. Additionally, one study from our laboratory which is currently in preparation was included. Following the supplementary search, two additional articles were included. Overall, 58 studies were eligible for systematic review. Fifty-five studies were eligible for meta-analysis on negative affect, however, two did not allow for effect size extraction, leaving 53 studies. For the meta-analysis on positive affect, 35 studies were eligible and included in analysis. The PRISMA flow diagram summarises the included studies for both negative and positive affect (see Fig 1).

Fig 1. Flowchart of study selection process.

Fig 1

Included studies were published between 1975 and 2021 and were conducted in various countries: Australia (n = 2), Canada (n = 4), China (n = 1), France (n = 1), Netherlands (n = 3), the USA (n = 42), and the UK (n = 5). Most of the studies were either conducted in the laboratory and looked ad libitum consumption, or utilized experienced sampling method (EMA), or similar (diary study, telephone interviews throughout several days). Study characteristics and effect sizes are provided in Table 1.

Association between daily negative affect and volume of alcohol consumed

Analysis of laboratory studies

Fourteen studies (1100 participants) were included in this meta-analysis. Analysis revealed a significant post-mood induction increase in amount of alcohol consumed by participants that was small-to-medium effect size, d = .28, 95% CI [.11, .44], t = 3.351, p = .004. Heterogeneity between the studies was significant, I2 = 47.7%, Q (13) = 24.87, p = .024.

Main analysis

A total of 127 effect sizes were extracted from 53 studies (8355 participants). Correlations between negative affect and consumption ranged from -.33 to .70. The pooled correlation coefficient for our data was r = .09, 95% CI [.03, .14], t (48.5) = 3.32, p = .002. As per I2 and τ2 indexes, I2 = 70.02%, whereas τ2 = .02.

Publication bias

According to Egger’s test, there was a publication bias (t = 3.53, p < .01). Selection model also demonstrated publication bias, χ2 (3) = 25.67, p < .001. After adjusting for publication bias, the relationship between negative affect and drinking volume was still positive and significant, r = .17, 95% CI [.07, .26], p = .004.

P-curve analysis indicated that evidential value is present, and that evidential value is not absent or inadequate (see Fig 2). This means that p-curve estimates that there is a “true” effect size underlying finding, and that the results are unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone. When correcting for selective reporting, the power of tests included in the meta-analysis was 69% (see Fig 3).

Fig 2. P-curve plot for studies on negative affect and alcohol consumption.

Fig 2

Note: The observed p-curve includes 20 statistically significant (p < .05) results, of which 15 are p < .025. There were 107 additional results entered but excluded from p-curve because they were p > .05.

Fig 3. Estimated power of meta-analysis on negative affect and drinking volume.

Fig 3

Meta-regression

Several moderators were examined: year of publication, country, study design (laboratory vs field), study quality, alcohol measure, whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. Alcohol measure was a significant moderator, as studies that looked at number of drinks as an outcome produced significantly lower effect sizes than studies that used other measures. On the other hand, analysis demonstrated that studies that looked at number of units as an outcome produced higher effect sizes, however, since the degrees of freedom were lower than four, this estimate could not be trusted. Similarly, while analysis showed that studies that treated affect as a continuum (rather than separate entities) and that were published in China and France demonstrated lower effect sizes, the degrees of freedom were lower than four, hence this estimate could not be trusted. Non-significant predictors were omitted from the final model. See Table 2 for a summary of moderator analysis results.

Table 2. Moderators of the relationship between negative affect and alcohol consumption volume.
Estimate SE t df 95% CI P
Intercept .12 .12 1.01 1.34 [-.71, .95] .460
Canada .04 .11 .34 2.24 [-.41, .48] .761
China -.43 .11 -4.06 2.29 [-.83, -.03] .044
France -.46 .11 -4.35 2.29 [-.86, -.06] .038
Netherlands .07 .11 .66 2.17 [-.37, .51] .570
UK .08 .11 .77 2.29 [-.32, .48] .512
USA .11 .10 1.02 1.11 [-.94, 1.56] .480
Measuring affect as continuum -.30 .03 -11.81 2.20 [-.40, -.20] .005
Number of drinks as an alcohol measure -.19 .06 -3.49 23.68 [-.31, -.09] .002
Number of units as an alcohol measure .46 .04 10.99 2.28 [.30, .63] .005
Number of sips as an alcohol measure .10 .06 1.67 14.47 [-.03, .22] .116

Association between daily positive affect and volume of alcohol consumed

A total of 50 effect sizes were extracted from 35 studies (6384 participants). Correlations between negative affect and consumption ranged from -.19 to .96. The pooled correlation coefficient for our data was r = .17, 95% CI [.04, .30], t (34) = 2.70, p = .011. As per I2 and τ2 indexes, I2 = 98.29%, whereas τ2 = .31.

Publication bias

According to Egger’s test, there was a publication bias (t = 3.01, p = .006). Selection model also demonstrated publication bias, χ2 (1) = 36.35, p < .001. After adjusting for publication bias, the relationship between negative affect and drinking volume was still positive and significant, r = .52, 95% CI [.35, .66], p < .001.

P-curve analysis indicated that evidential value is present, and that evidential value is not absent or inadequate (see Fig 4). This means that P-curve estimates that there is a “true” effect size underlying finding, and that the results are unlikely to be the product of publication bias and p-hacking alone. When correcting for selective reporting, the power of tests included into meta-analysis was 96% (see Fig 5).

Fig 4. P-curve plot for studies on positive affect and alcohol consumption.

Fig 4

Note: The observed p-curve includes 11 statistically significant (p < .05) results, of which 9 are p < .025. There were 39 additional results entered but excluded from p-curve because they were p > .05.

Fig 5. Estimated power of meta-analysis on positive affect and drinking volume.

Fig 5

Meta-regression

Several moderators were examined: year of publication, country, study quality, study design (laboratory vs field), alcohol measure, whether studies examined distinct emotions or averaged them, and whether study considered affective state to be a continuum or not. While measuring amount consumed in units was a significant predictor of higher effect sizes in the initial model, this was not significant anymore when the model was reduced. Year published was a significant moderator—with later years, effect sizes decreased, t (48) = -2.93, p = .005. None of the other moderaors were significant.

Comparing the results of meta-analyses of negative and positive affect

Cohen’s q statistics was calculated by comparing the results for negative affect (r = .09) and positive affect (r = .17). The obtained q value was .08, indicating that there is no significant difference between the effect sizes of two coefficients.

Discussion

With the aim of examining the extent to which alcohol consumption can be explained theoretically by accounts which posit that people drink to enhance positive or to overcome negative affective states, meta-analyses of eligible non-clinical research outputs spanning 46 years were performed. Findings can be summarized as follows. First, both elevated negative and increased positive affect were associated with increased alcohol consumption volume, although the effect sizes were small. This may indicate that the mixed findings to date may be due to a predominance of underpowered individual studies in this field of research. Second, we did not find that affect measure used impacted the nature of the results observed within studies. Third, for negative affect, studies that used number of drinks as the alcohol consumption measure found lower effect sizes than research that used other metrics (i.e., number of units, amount in milliliters, number of sips, number of drinks). Other moderators were also examined in exploratory analyses (e.g., country), though the only significant moderator of effect sizes for positive affect was year published, pointing to a tendency for effect sizes to decline over time.

The relationship between daily affect and alcohol consumption volume: Theoretical implications

The results of our meta-analysis indicate that both negative and positive affective states over the course of a day are associated with increased consumption volume in non-clinical populations. This temporal positive association is consistent with affect regulation models of alcohol consumption (self-medication hypothesis, [3]; tension-reduction theory, [11]; stress-response dampening theory, [12]; negative reinforcement model of alcohol use, [4]; positive reinforcement theory of alcohol use, [26]). Our analyses build on the meta-analysis by Bresin et al. [2], which found that laboratory negative mood induction is associated with increased consumption, and extend this work in two ways. First, we demonstrate that the association between increased negative affect and alcohol consumption occurs in field as well as laboratory studies, thereby helping to overcome concerns regarding the ecological validity of laboratory-based work (although laboratory studies yielded higher effect sizes for negative affect). Second, we also found that positive affect, not previously considered meta-analytically, was also associated with elevated same day alcohol consumption.

The finding that both negative and positive affect are associated with increased alcohol consumption raises questions about whether it is necessary to retain both negative and positive reinforcement models of alcohol use, or whether a more parsimonious theoretical account of the mood-alcohol nexus may be possible. There are two reasons for entertaining this thought. First, the effect sizes for negative and positive affect were similar (.09 difference), and, when compared statistically using the q denominator [79], were not significantly different from each other. Therefore, increases of affect (i.e., affect intensity, [142,143]) may play a more determinant role than affect valence (i.e., pleasantness) in explaining the relationship between mood and consumption. Second, as indicated by the moderator analysis, whether distinct emotions were examined did not appear to impact the affect-alcohol relationship. Our analyses, in this way, indicate that specific emotions are not differently associated with consumption, but the intensity of these emotions is. For example, it did not make a difference whether happiness or sadness were examined as predictors of alcohol consumption; rather how strongly happiness or sadness were experienced appeared to be important.

What emerges from these findings is that it may be useful to subsume existing affect regulation models, which posit that alcohol consumption is driven by a desire to alleviate or heighten particular affective states, with an account that emphasises affect intensity: it may be that particular affective states per se are less important in explaining increased alcohol use than the regulation of their intensity. This model would suggest that, on a given drinking day, alcohol consumption is likely to be elevated in individuals whose mood is more intense (or whose affect is less ‘flat’). Such an approach does not contradict the notion that negative and positive affect are distinct entities. Instead, it asserts that the contents of emotions are of less importance in explaining alcohol consumption than the intensity with which these are experienced. The proposed account is consistent with the notion that people can experience negative and positive affect at the same time [57], and postulates that both may simultaneously shape alcohol consumption.

This way of thinking about drivers of alcohol consumption may have wider theoretical implications. For example, when considering the drinking motives literature [144,145]—which is also characterised by inconsistent and mixed finding (although see meta-analysis by [146])—it may be possible to think about distinct emotional drinking motive categories (enhancement and coping, [147]) in terms of ‘affect regulation’. In this way, Littlefield et al. found that drinkers who consume alcohol for either coping or enhancement motives do not form two distinct groups [14], suggesting that these motives may be best viewed as dimensional variables that covary such that individuals who are high in one internal motive tend to be high in the other motive. This is not to say that negative and positive affect (or coping / enhancement motives) predict all forms of alcohol consumption in the same way. Nevertheless, based on the results of our meta-analyses, these factors may similarly be associated with daily drinking volume on a drinking day. Future research could fruitfully investigate whether affect intensity is associated with other variables of interest such as drinking onset or craving.

Methodologically, an affect intensity regulation hypothesis of alcohol consumption suggests that studies could focus on affect intensity instead of overall levels of affect. This could involve asking participants to report the extent to which they feel the intensity of their negative and positive mood rather than asking them multiple questions about distinct emotions. Approaches requiring participants to only express the intensity of their overall negative and positive mood could lead to decreased participant burden and increased compliance in studies, which is particularly relevant to real-time research designs. While more research (in clinical populations) is required, current findings suggest that prevention and intervention efforts might usefully target overall levels of affect rather than focusing on affect valence. As such, providing individuals with alternative strategies for improving affect regulation may be of particular benefit.

Moderator analyses

Moving on from possible theoretical implications of current findings, it is worthwhile to briefly consider significant moderators of the affect-alcohol consumption relationship. For negative affect, we found that studies that used number of drinks as an outcome tended to generate lower effect sizes than research that used other measures of drinking behavior such as the amount consumed in milliliters. For positive affect, year of publication was a significant moderator pointing to a tendency for effect sizes to decline over time. This is perhaps an indication that the field of alcohol research is not immune to the well-documented decline effect [148] whereby effect sizes can decrease over time for a variety of possible reasons that include false positive results, overestimation of effect sizes, under-specification of the conditions of the study, or genuinely decreasing effect sizes [149]. In the current context, it is also possible that advances in methodology and statistical analysis may have contributed to a more accurate effect size detection with the passage of time.

Limitations and further research

The results of the current meta-analyses need to be considered in light of a number of limitations. First, the original correlation coefficients were not always available, and were extracted from standardised beta weights [65] which were converted from d, obtained from unstandardized beta weights [66], or F-values [67], using an online effect size calculator [Lenhard & Lenhard, Unpublished]. Furthermore, as r can only be obtained from standardised beta coefficient when it is between -.05 and .05, one study had to be excluded from analyses, while another study was omitted as it did not report any statistics from which effect sizes could be calculated. As such, the current meta-analysis for negative affect could not exhaustively represent all published data, although sensitivity analyses conducted indicated that there would have been no significant differences if it had been possible to use the excluded studies in the analyses. Second, while we separated the meta-analyses based on affect valence, we did not account for difference in affect arousal and how this could potentially impact the relationship between mood and consumption.

It is also important to note that our meta-analyses were concerned with drinking volume. That is, we examined whether intra-day affect influenced the amount of alcohol consumed on the day and future research could therefore usefully examine other variables of interest such as drinking onset, likelihood, blood alcohol concentration, or alcohol cravings. Similarly, further studies may consider examining the differences in the relationship between daily affect and heavy drinking (versus any drinking, as was examined in the present review). Moreover, this review only focused on intra-day consumption. While this allowed us to examine the association between state affect, further examination of trait affect (i.e., tendency to experience particular affective states) could help answer the question how longer-term affective states may be associated with alcohol consumption (e.g., [150]). Furthermore, the current meta-analyses focused on affective states in general without looking at potential differences between mood and emotional state. While this decision was made because there is an overlap between these constructs and, for this reason, many studies use these terms interchangeably, further meta-analytical studies may wish to look at conceivably different effects of mood versus emotional state on alcohol consumption.

We also recommend that future research should routinely report the direct relationship between mood and consumption, and include correlation coefficients between all variables of interest. More generally, there is also a need for studies to be adequately powered and to conduct longitudinal investigations given the dominance of cross-sectional work in this area. As outlined, future research may also benefit from utilising affect intensity as a primary outcome variable as this could help reduce participant burden. Furthermore, since most of the studies on the topic were conducted in USA, additional research in other national contexts, which may differ with regards to the sociocultural positioning of alcohol [151], is advised.

Conclusion

Overall, results of the present meta-analyses converge to suggest that both positive and negative affective states are associated with elevated daily alcohol consumption volume in non-clinical populations. While in apparent support of both positive and negative reinforcement models, present findings thereby suggest that greater insights into the relationship between mood and alcohol may be garnered through a more parsimonious focus on the intensity of emotional experiences (i.e., aggregate intensity of both negative and positive affect) rather than on valence. Consistent with idea that facets of positive and negative affect may be experienced simultaneously it therefore appears possible to posit an affect intensity regulation hypothesis. According to this, the intensity (rather than valence) of people’s affective states on a given drinking day is associated with increased consumption of alcohol. While future research is required to test this theory, it is evident that much remains to be uncovered with regards to the mood-alcohol nexus, and that this endeavour will continue to exercise philosophers, writers, and scientists for some time to come.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

(DOC)

S1 File. S1, S2—Unpussblished references.

(DOCX)

Data Availability

All data files are available on Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/fe9au/.

Funding Statement

This work was funded by Edge Hill University PhD Studentship.

References

  • 1.Bukowski C. Women. USA: HarperCollins Publishers; 1978. 632 p.
  • 2.Bresin K, Mekawi Y, Verona E. The Effect of Laboratory Manipulations of Negative Affect on Alcohol Craving and Use: A Meta-analysis. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2018. Sep;32(6):617–27. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Khantzian E J. The Self-Medication Hypothesis of Substance Use Disorders: A Reconsideration and Recent Applications: Harvard Review of Psychiatry: Vol 4, No 5. Taylor & Francis Online. 1996. [cited 2020 Dec 3]. Available from: https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.3109/10673229709030550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Baker TB, Piper ME, McCarthy DE, Majeskie MR, Fiore MC. Addiction motivation reformulated: an affective processing model of negative reinforcement. Psychol Rev. 2004. Jan;111(1):33–51. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.1.33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Appleton A, James R, Larsen J. The Association between Mental Wellbeing, Levels of Harmful Drinking, and Drinking Motivations: A Cross-Sectional Study of the UK Adult Population. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018. Jul;15(7):1333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Bolton JM, Robinson J, Sareen J. Self-medication of mood disorders with alcohol and drugs in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Affect Disord. 2009. Jun 1;115(3):367–75. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2008.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Cho SB, Su J, Kuo SI-C, Bucholz KK, Chan G, Edenberg HJ, et al. Positive and negative reinforcement are differentially associated with alcohol consumption as a function of alcohol dependence. Psychol Addict Behav. 2019;33(1):58–68. doi: 10.1037/adb0000436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Holt LJ, Armeli S, Tennen H, Austad CS, Raskin SA, Fallahi CR, et al. A person-centered approach to understanding negative reinforcement drinking among first year college students. Addict Behav. 2013. Dec 1;38(12):2937–44. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.08.015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Shadur JM, Hussong AM, Haroon M. Negative affect variability and adolescent self-medication: The role of the peer context. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015. Nov;34(6):571–80. doi: 10.1111/dar.12260 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stewart SH, Morris E, Mellings T, Komar J. Relations of social anxiety variables to drinking motives, drinking quantity and frequency, and alcohol-related problems in undergraduates. J Ment Health. 2006. Jan 1;15(6):671–82. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Conger J. Reinforcement theory and the dynamics of alcoholism. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcohol. 1956;17:296–305. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Sher K, Levenson R. Risk for alcoholism and individual differences in the stress-response-dampening effect of alcohol. 1982 [cited 2021 Mar 30]; /paper/Risk-for-alcoholism-and-individual-differences-in-Sher-Levenson/32ec3b4e27cc27041a41575b41654f914f720b25. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 13.Greeley J, Oei T. Alcohol and tension reduction. In: Psychological theories of drinking and alcoholism, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 1999. p. 14–53. (The Guilford substance abuse series). [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Swendsen JD, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G, Willard A, Hromi A. Mood and alcohol consumption: an experience sampling test of the self-medication hypothesis. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(2):198–204. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Field M, Quigley M. Mild stress increases attentional bias in social drinkers who drink to cope: A replication and extension.—PsycNET. 2009. [cited 2021 Apr 9]. https://doi.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Fa0017090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hepworth R, Mogg K, Brignell C, Bradley BP. Negative mood increases selective attention to food cues and subjective appetite. Appetite. 2010. Feb 1;54(1):134–42. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.09.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Higgins R, Marlatt G. Effects of anxiety arousal on the consumption of alcohol by alcoholics and social drinkers. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1973;41(3):426–33. doi: 10.1037/h0035366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Marlatt GA, Kosturn CF, Lang AR. Provocation to anger and opportunity for retaliation as determinants of alcohol consumption in social drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol. 1975;84(6):652–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Moskal D, Maisto SA, De Vita M, Ditre JW. Effects of experimental pain induction on alcohol urge, intention to consume alcohol, and alcohol demand. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;26(1):65–76. doi: 10.1037/pha0000170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Fairlie AM, Cadigan JM, Patrick ME, Larimer ME, Lee CM. Unplanned Heavy Episodic and High-Intensity Drinking: Daily-Level Associations With Mood, Context, and Negative Consequences. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(3):331–9. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2019.80.331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Bresin K, Fairbairn CE. The Association Between Negative and Positive Affect and Alcohol Use: An Ambulatory Study. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(6):614–22. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2019.80.614 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Breslin FC, O’Keeffe, Burrell L, Ratliff-Crain J, Baum A. The effects of stress and coping on daily alcohol use in women. Addict Behav. 1995;20(2):141–7. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)00055-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.de Leon AND, Dvorak RD, Kramer MP, Peterson R, Pinto DA, Leary AV, et al. Daily Patterns of Emotional Functioning on Drinking and Nondrinking Days. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2020;44(12):2598–610. doi: 10.1111/acer.14480 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Dvorak RD, Stevenson BL, Kilwein TM, Sargent EM, Dunn ME, Leary AV, et al. Tension Reduction and Affect Regulation: An Examination of Mood Indices on Drinking and Non-Drinking Days among University Student Drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2018;26(4):377–90. doi: 10.1037/pha0000210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Simons JS, Dvorak RD, Batien BD, Wray TB. Event-level associations between affect, alcohol intoxication, and acute dependence symptoms: Effects of urgency, self-control, and drinking experience. Addict Behav. 2010;35(12):1045–53. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Wit H, Phan L. Positive reinforcement theories of drug use. In: Substance abuse and emotion. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2010. p. 43–60. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Howard AL, Patrick ME, Maggs JL. College student affect and heavy drinking: Variable associations across days, semesters, and people. Psychol Addict Behav J Soc Psychol Addict Behav. 2015;29(2):430–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Weiss NH, Forkus SR, Contractor AA, Schick MR. Difficulties regulating positive emotions and alcohol and drug misuse: A path analysis. Addict Behav. 2018;84:45–52. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2018.03.027 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Stein KD, Goldman MS, Boca FKD. The Influence of Alcohol Expectancy Priming and Mood Manipulation on Subsequent Alcohol Consumption. J Abnorm Psychol. 2000;109(1):106–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.VanderVeen JD, Plawecki MH, Millward JB, Hays J, Kareken DA, O’Connor S, et al. Negative urgency, mood induction, and alcohol seeking behaviors. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;165:151–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2016.05.026 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Holzhauer CG, Wemm SE, Wulfert E, Cao Z (Tim). Fluctuations in progesterone moderate the relationship between daily mood and alcohol use in young adult women. Addict Behav. 2020;101:106146. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Monk RL, Qureshi A, Heim D. An examination of the extent to which mood and context are associated with real-time alcohol consumption. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2020;208. Available from: https://www.scopus.com/inward/record.uri?eid=2-s2.0-85079050841&doi=10.1016%2fj.drugalcdep.2020.107880&partnerID=40&md5=b1c9153d4d5fc50146f778d2d82cf21d. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107880 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Dvorak RD, Simons JS. Daily associations between anxiety and alcohol use: Variation by sustained attention, set shifting, and gender. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014;28(4):969–79. doi: 10.1037/a0037642 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Green D, Goldman S, Salovey P. Measurement Error Masks Bipolarity in Affect Ratings. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1993;64:1029–41. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.1029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Russell J. A circumplex model of affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1980;39(6):1161–78. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Matthews G, Jones DM, Chamberlain AG. Refining the measurement of mood: The UWIST Mood Adjective Checklist. Br J Psychol. 1990;81(1):17–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Russell JA, Weiss A, Mendelsohn GA. Affect Grid: A single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1989;57(3):493–502. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54(6):1063–70. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.54.6.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Stern RA, Arruda JE, Hooper CR, Wolfner GD, Morey CE. Visual analogue mood scales to measure internal mood state in neurologically impaired patients: Description and initial validity evidence. Aphasiology. 1997;11(1):59–71. [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Barrett LF, Mesquita B, Ochsner KN, Gross JJ. The Experience of Emotion. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58(1):373–403. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085709 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Posner J, Russell JA, Peterson BS. The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Dev Psychopathol. 2005;17(3):715–34. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050340 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ekman P. Emotions revealed. BMJ. 2004;328(Suppl S5):0405184. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Lench HC, Flores SA, Bench SW. Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: a meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(5):834–55. doi: 10.1037/a0024244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Duif M, Thewissen V, Wouters S, Lechner L, Jacobs N. Affective Instability and Alcohol Consumption: Ecological Momentary Assessment in an Adult Sample. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2019;80(4):441–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Richardson CME, Hoene THM, Rigatti HL. Self-critical perfectionism and daily drinking to cope with negative emotional experiences among college students. Personal Individ Differ. 2020;156:109773. [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Stamates AL, Linden-Carmichael AN, Preonas PD, Lau-Barraco C. Testing daily associations between impulsivity, affect, and alcohol outcomes: a pilot study. Addict Res Theory. 2019;27(3):242–8. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2018.1498846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.O’Donnell R, Richardson B, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, Liknaitzky P, Arulkadacham L, Dvorak R, et al. Ecological momentary assessment of drinking in young adults: An investigation into social context, affect and motives. Addict Behav. 2019;98:106019. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.06.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Rohsenow DJ, Smith RE, Johnson S. Stress management training as a prevention program for heavy social drinkers: Cognitions, affect, drinking, and individual differences. Addict Behav. 1985;10(1):45–54. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(85)90052-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Feldman LA. Distinguishing Depression and Anxiety in Self-Report: Evidence from Confirmatory Factor Analysis on Nonclinical and Clinical Samples. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1993;61(4):631–8. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.61.4.631 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Tsai JL. Ideal Affect: Cultural Causes and Behavioral Consequences. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2007;2(3):242–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00043.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kuntsche E, Bruno R. Moody booze: Introducing the special section on affect regulation and substance use. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):569–70. doi: 10.1111/dar.12349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological Momentary Assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Larsen, Engels RCME, Granic I, Overbeek G. An Experimental Study on Imitation of Alcohol Consumption in Same-Sex Dyads. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44(3):250–5. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agp002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Spijkerman R, Larsen H, Gibbons FX, Engels RCME. Students’ Drinker Prototypes and Alcohol Use in a Naturalistic Setting. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2009;34(1):64–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01067.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Quigley BM, Collins RL. The modeling of alcohol consumption: A meta-analytic review. J Stud Alcohol. 1999;60(1):90–8. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1999.60.90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Jones A, Button E, Rose AK, Robinson E, Christiansen P, Di Lemma L, et al. The ad-libitum alcohol ‘taste test’: secondary analyses of potential confounds and construct validity. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(5):917–24. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4171-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Diener E, Emmons RA. The independence of positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984. Nov;47(5):1105–17. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.47.5.1105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Larsen, McGraw A, Cacioppo J. Can People Feel Happy and Sad at the Same Time? J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001;81(4):684–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ekkekakis P. Affect, mood, and emotion. In: Measurement in sport and exercise psychology. Champaign, IL, US: Human Kinetics; 2012. p. 321–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Page MJ, Moher D, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:n160. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n160 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.APA. Reporting Standards for Research in Psychology. Am Psychol. 2008;63(9):839–51. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.63.9.839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.APA. Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association, Sixth Editio. https://apastyle.apa.org/products/4200066. [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kmet LM, Cook LS, Lee RC. Standard Quality Assessment Criteria for Evaluating Primary Research Papers from a Variety of Fields. ERA. 2004 [cited 2021 May 19]. https://era.library.ualberta.ca/items/48b9b989-c221-4df6-9e35-af782082280e.
  • 64.Thalheimer W, Cook S. How to calculate effect sizes from published research: A simplified methodology. Work-Learning Research. 2002;1:1–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Peterson RA, Brown SP. On the Use of Beta Coefficients in Meta-Analysis. J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(1):175–81. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Feingold A. A Regression Framework for Effect Size Assessments in Longitudinal Modeling of Group Differences. Rev Gen Psychol J Div 1 Am Psychol Assoc. 2013. Mar 1;17:111–21. doi: 10.1037/a0030048 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Lipsey MW, Wilson D. Practical Meta-Analysis. 1st edition. Thousand Oaks, Calif: SAGE Publications, Inc; 2000. 264 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Baird HM, Webb TL, Sirois FM, Gibson-Miller J. Understanding the effects of time perspective: A meta-analysis testing a self-regulatory framework. Psychol Bull. 2021;147(3):233–67. doi: 10.1037/bul0000313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.RStudio | Open source & professional software for data science teams. [Internet]. N.D. [cited 2021 Jan 29]. https://rstudio.com/.
  • 70.Balduzzi S, Rücker G, Schwarzer G. How to perform a meta-analysis with R: a practical tutorial. Evid Based Ment Health. 2019;22(4):153–60. doi: 10.1136/ebmental-2019-300117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Hedges LV, Tipton E, Johnson MC. Robust variance estimation in meta-regression with dependent effect size estimates. Res Synth Methods. 2010;1(1):39–65. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Fisher Z, Tipton E. robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Egger M, Davey Smith G, Schneider M, Minder C. Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test. BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629–34. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.JASP team. JASP. 2020.
  • 75.Bartoš F, Maier M, Wagenmakers E-J. Adjusting for Publication Bias in JASP—Selection Models and Robust Bayesian Meta-Analysis. PsyArXiv; 2020 [cited 2021 Sep 20]. https://psyarxiv.com/75bqn/.
  • 76.Simonsohn U, Nelson LD, Simmons JP. P-curve: A key to the file-drawer. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143(2):534–47. doi: 10.1037/a0033242 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Harrer M, Cuijpers P, Furukawa TA, Ebert DD. Doing Meta-Analysis with R: A Hands-On Guide. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC; 2021. 500 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Huizenga H m, Visser I, Dolan C. Testing overall and moderator effects in random effects meta-regression. Br J Math Stat Psychol. 2011;64:1–19. doi: 10.1348/000711010X522687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Routledge; 1988. 579 p. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Austin HRT, Notebaert L, Wiers RW, Salemink E, MacLeod C. Potions for Emotions: Do self-reported individual differences in negative-emotional drinking predict alcohol consumption in the laboratory following exposure to a negative experience? Addict Behav Rep. 2020;11:100243. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Cyders MA, Zapolski TCB, Combs JL, Settles RF, Fillmore MT, Smith GT. Experimental Effect of Positive Urgency on Negative Outcomes From Risk Taking and on Increased Alcohol Consumption. Psychol Addict Behav. 2010;24(3):367–75. doi: 10.1037/a0019494 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.de Castro JM. Social, circadian, nutritional, and subjective correlates of the spontaneous pattern of moderate alcohol intake of normal humans. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1990;35(4):923–31. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90380-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Dinc L, Cooper AJ. Positive affective states and alcohol consumption: The moderating role of trait positive urgency. Addict Behav. 2015;47:17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.03.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Gabel PC, Noel NE, Keane TM, Lisman SA. Effects of sexual versus fear arousal on alcohol consumption in college males. Behav Res Ther. 1980;18(6):519–26. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(80)90044-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Gautreau C, Sherry S, Battista S, Goldstein A, Stewart S. Enhancement motives moderate the relationship between high-arousal positive moods and drinking quantity: Evidence from a 22-day experience sampling study. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;6(34):595–602. doi: 10.1111/dar.12235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Larsen R, Diener E. Promises and problems with the circumplex model of emotion. Rev Personal Soc Psychol Emot. 1992;13. [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Liu S, Wang MO, Zhan Y, Shi J. Daily work stress and alcohol use: testing the cross-level moderation effects of neuroticism and job involvement. Pers Psychol. 2009;62(3):575–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Gable SL, Reis HT, Elliot AJ. Behavioral activation and inhibition in everyday life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(6):1135–49. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.6.1135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Mohr CD, Arpin S, McCabe CT. Daily affect variability and context-specific alcohol consumption. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):581–7. doi: 10.1111/dar.12253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Mohr CD, Brannan D, Wendt S, Jacobs L, Wright R, Wang M. Daily mood–drinking slopes as predictors: A new take on drinking motives and related outcomes. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(4):944–55. doi: 10.1037/a0032633 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Peacock A, Cash C, Bruno R, Ferguson SG. Day-by-day variation in affect, arousal and alcohol consumption in young adults. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2015;34(6):588–94. doi: 10.1111/dar.12238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Pihl RO, Yankofsky L. Alcohol consumption in male social drinkers as a function of situationally induced depressive affect and anxiety. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1979;65(3):251–7. doi: 10.1007/BF00492212 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Zuckerman M, Lubin B. The Multiple Affect Adjective Checklist (MAACL). 1965; https://www.semanticscholar.org/paper/The-Multiple-Affect-Adjective-Checklist-(MAACL)-Zuckerman-Lubin/c94a8b1f3c4ba0907399357518069a48d677d9b8.
  • 94.Rohsenow DJ. Control over interpersonal evaluation and alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. Addict Behav. 1982;7(2):113–21. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(82)90036-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Simons JS, Gaher RM, Oliver MNI, Bush JA, Palmer MA. An experience sampling study of associations between affect and alcohol use and problems among college students. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(4):459–69. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.459 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Simons JS, Wills TA, Neal DJ. The many faces of affect: A multilevel model of drinking frequency/quantity and alcohol dependence symptoms among young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. 2014;123(3):676–94. doi: 10.1037/a0036926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Watson D, Clark LA. The PANAS-X: Manual for the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule—Expanded Form. University of Iowa; 1994 [cited 2021 Jan 29]. https://iro.uiowa.edu/discovery/fulldisplay/alma9983557488402771/01IOWA_INST:ResearchRepository.
  • 98.Stasiewicz PR, Lisman SA. Effects of infant cries on alcohol consumption in college males at risk for child abuse. Child Abuse Negl. 1989;13(4):463–70. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(89)90050-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Stevenson BL, Dvorak RD, Kramer MP, Peterson RS, Dunn ME, Leary AV, et al. Within- and between-person associations from mood to alcohol consequences: The mediating role of enhancement and coping drinking motives. J Abnorm Psychol. 2019;128(8):813–22. doi: 10.1037/abn0000472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Sutker. Alcohol use, negative mood states. 1983.
  • 101.Wardell JD, Read JP, Curtin JJ, Merrill JE. Mood and Implicit Alcohol Expectancy Processes: Predicting Alcohol Consumption in the Laboratory. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2012;36(1):119–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01589.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Mohr CD, Tennen H, Temple M, Clark J, Armeli S, Todd M, et al. Moving beyond the keg party: A daily process study of college student drinking motivations. Psychol Addict Behav. 2005;19(4):392–403. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.4.392 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Hussong AM, Galloway CA, Feagans LA. Coping motives as a moderator of daily mood-drinking covariation. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(3):344–53. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.344 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Affleck G. A Daily Diary Validity Test of Drinking to Cope Measures. Psychol Addict Behav. 2003;17(4):303–11. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.17.4.303 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A Global Measure of Perceived Stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Carney MA, Armeli S, Tennen H, Affleck G, O’Neil TP. Positive and negative daily events, perceived stress, and alcohol use: A diary study. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68(5):788–98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Armeli S, Carney MA, Tennen H, Affleck G, O’Neil TP. Stress and alcohol use: A daily process examination of the stressor–vulnerability model. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;78(5):979–94. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.78.5.979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Stone AA, Neale JM. Effects of severe daily events on mood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1984;46(1):137–44. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.46.1.137 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Day AM. Ecological Momentary Assessment of Acute Alcohol Use Disorder Symptoms: Associations With Mood, Motives, and Use on Planned Drinking Days. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22(4):285–97. doi: 10.1037/a0037157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Thomas SE, Merrill JE, von Hofe J, Magid V. Coping Motives for Drinking Affect Stress Reactivity but Not Alcohol Consumption in a Clinical Laboratory Setting. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2014;75(1):115–23. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Grant VV, Stewart SH, Mohr CD. Coping-anxiety and coping-depression motives predict different daily mood-drinking relationships. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23(2):226–37. doi: 10.1037/a0015006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Steptoe A, Wardle J. Mood and drinking: a naturalistic diary study of alcohol, coffee and tea. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;141(3):315–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.McNair DM, Lorr M, Droppleman LF. POMS manual. Educational and Industrial Testing Service. 1981. [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Hull JG, Young RD. Self-consciousness, self-esteem, and success–failure as determinants of alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1983;44(6):1097–109. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.44.6.1097 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.O’Hara RE, Armeli S, Tennen H. College students’ daily-level reasons for not drinking. Drug Alcohol Rev. 2014;33(4):412–9. doi: 10.1111/dar.12162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H, Carney MA, Ball SA, Kranzler HR, et al. Drinking to cope: a comparison of questionnaire and electronic diary reports. J Stud Alcohol. 2005;66(1):121–9. doi: 10.15288/jsa.2005.66.121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Todd M, Armeli S, Tennen H. Interpersonal problems and negative mood as predictors of within-day time to drinking. Psychol Addict Behav. 2009;23(2):205–15. doi: 10.1037/a0014792 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Collins RL, Morsheimer ET, Shiffman S, Paty JA, Gnys M, Papandonatos GD. Ecological momentary assessment in a behavioral drinking moderation training program. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1998;6(3):306–15. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.6.3.306 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Ehrenberg E, Armeli S, Howland M, Tennen H. A daily process examination of episode-specific drinking to cope motivation among college students. Addict Behav. 2016;57:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.02.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Higgins R, Marlatt G. Fear of interpersonal evaluation as a determinant of alcohol consumption in male social drinkers. J Abnorm Psychol. 1975;84(6):644–51. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.84.6.644 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Holroyd KA. Effects of social anxiety and social evaluation on beer consumption and social interaction. J Stud Alcohol. 1978;39(5):737–44. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1978.39.737 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Dvorak RD, Pearson MR, Sargent EM, Stevenson BL, Mfon AM. Daily associations between emotional functioning and alcohol involvement: Moderating effects of response inhibition and gender. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2016;163:S46–53. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.09.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Armeli S, Dehart T, Tennen H, Todd M, Affleck G. Daily Interpersonal Stress and the Stressor–Vulnerability Model of Alcohol Use. J Soc Clin Psychol. 2007;26(8):896–921. [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Corcoran KJ, Parker PS. Alcohol expectancy questionnaire tension reduction scale as a predictor of alcohol consumption in a stressful situation. Addict Behav. 1991;16(3):129–37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Kidorf M, Lang AR. Effects of social anxiety and alcohol expectancies on stress-induced drinking. Psychol Addict Behav. 1999;13(2):134–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Tucker CJ. Remote sensing of leaf water content in the near infrared. Remote Sens Environ. 1980;10(1):23–32. [Google Scholar]
  • 127.McGrath E, Jones A, Field M. Acute stress increases ad-libitum alcohol consumption in heavy drinkers, but not through impaired inhibitory control. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2016;233(7):1227–34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Magrys SA, Olmstead MC. Acute Stress Increases Voluntary Consumption of Alcohol in Undergraduates. Alcohol Alcohol. 2015;50(2):213–8. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agu101 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Spielberg CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Adults—PsycNET. 1983 [cited 2021 Sep 20]. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft06496-000.
  • 130.Aldridge-Gerry AA, Roesch SC, Villodas F, McCabe C, Leung Q, Da Costa M. Daily Stress and Alcohol Consumption: Modeling Between-Person and Within-Person Ethnic Variation in Coping Behavior. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2011; 72(1):125–34. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2011.72.125 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Emery NN, Simons JS. The role of affect, emotion management, and attentional bias in young adult drinking: An experience sampling study. Psychopharmacol Berl. 2020;1557–75. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Hamilton HR, Armeli S, Tennen H. Affect and alcohol: The moderating role of episode-specific drinking motives. Addict Behav. 2020;110:106521. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106521 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Mohr CD, Brannan D, Mohr J, Armeli S, Tennen H. Evidence for positive mood buffering among college student drinkers. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 2008. Sep;34(9):1249–59. doi: 10.1177/0146167208319385 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Schroder KEE, Perrine MW. Covariations of emotional states and alcohol consumption: Evidence from 2 years of daily data collection. Soc Sci Med. 2007;65(12):2588–602. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.07.011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Waddell JT, Sher KJ, Piasecki TM. Coping motives and negative affect: An ecological study of the antecedents of alcohol craving and alcohol use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2021;35(5):565–76. doi: 10.1037/adb0000696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Larsen H, Engels R, Granic I, Huizink A. Does Stress Increase Imitation of Drinking Behavior? An Experimental Study in a (Semi-)Naturalistic Context. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2013;37. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Dieleman GC, van der Ende J, Verhulst FC, Huizink AC. Perceived and physiological arousal during a stress task: Can they differentiate between anxiety and depression? Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(8):1223–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.02.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Sacco P, Burruss K, Smith CA, Kuerbis A, Harrington D, Moore AA, et al. Drinking behavior among older adults at a continuing care retirement community: affective and motivational influences. Aging Ment Health. 2015;19(3):279–89. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2014.933307 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Mackinnon A, Jorm AF, Christensen H, Korten AE, Jacomb PA, Rodgers B. A short form of the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: evaluation of factorial validity and invariance across demographic variables in a community sample. Personal Individ Differ. 1999;27(3):405–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Lindgren KP, Ramirez JJ, Wiers RW, Teachman BA, Norris J, Olin CC, et al. Mood selectively moderates the implicit alcohol association-drinking relation in college student heavy episodic drinkers. Psychol Addict Behav. 2018;32(3):338–49. doi: 10.1037/adb0000360 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Holland RW, de Vries M, Hermsen B, van Knippenberg A. Mood and the attitude–behavior link: The happy act on impulse, the sad think twice. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2012;3(3):356–64. [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Diener E, Larsen RJ, Levine S, Emmons RA. Intensity and frequency: Dimensions underlying positive and negative affect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48(5):1253–65. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.48.5.1253 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Larsen RJ, Diener E. Affect intensity as an individual difference characteristic: A review. J Res Personal. 1987;21(1):1–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Cooper ML. Motivations for alcohol use among adolescents: Development and validation of a four-factor model.—PsycNET. 1994. [cited 2021 May 20]. /doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2F1040-3590.6.2.117. [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Cooper ML, Frone MR, Russell M, Mudar P. Drinking to regulate positive and negative emotions: A motivational model of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;69(5):990–1005. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.69.5.990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Bresin K, Mekawi Y. The “Why” of Drinking Matters: A Meta-Analysis of the Association Between Drinking Motives and Drinking Outcomes. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2021;45(1):38–50. doi: 10.1111/acer.14518 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Littlefield AK, Talley AE, Jackson KM. Coping motives, negative moods, and time-to-drink: Exploring alternative analytic models of coping motives as a moderator of daily mood-drinking covariation. Addict Behav. 2012;37(12):1371–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Schooler JW. Metascience could rescue the ‘replication crisis’. Nat News. 2014. Nov 6;515(7525):9. doi: 10.1038/515009a [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Protzko J, Schooler JW. Decline effects: Types, mechanisms, and personal reflections. In: Psychological science under scrutiny: Recent challenges and proposed solutions. Wiley Blackwell; 2017. p. 85–107. [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Treeby M, Bruno R. Shame and guilt-proneness: Divergent implications for problematic alcohol use and drinking to cope with anxiety and depression symptomatology. Personal Individ Differ. 2012;53(5):613–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Gordon R, Heim D, MacAskill S. Rethinking drinking cultures: A review of drinking cultures and a reconstructed dimensional approach. Public Health. 2012;126(1):3–11. doi: 10.1016/j.puhe.2011.09.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Jose M Moran

18 Nov 2021

PONE-D-21-30664Towards an affect intensity reinforcement hypothesis: A systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between affective states and alcohol consumptionPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Tovmasyan,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

After review of the manuscript, both the reviewers and myself agree that there are major issues in the current version of the manuscript that need to be addressed/responded to by the authors. The major concerns are associated with the search strategy used by the authors that may have missed key literature for the meta-analysis. Also of concern is the fact that the authors indicate that they have followed the PRISMA guidelines and, for example, do not present a full electronic search strategy for at least one database, including any limits used, such that it could be repeated.  The search strategy and in which database, including limits used, should be indicated. Authors should strictly verify their adherence to the PRISMA guidelines in this manuscript.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jan 02 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Jose M. Moran

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf  and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. We note that you have referenced (Lenhard W, Lenhard A. Computation of Effect Sizes [Internet]. Unpublished;)which has currently not yet been accepted for publication. Please remove this from your References and amend this to state in the body of your manuscript: (ie “Lenhard W, Lenhard. [Unpublished]”) as detailed online in our guide for authors

http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-reference-style

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The study attempted to clarify the pooled association between affective states and alcohol consumption by conducting a systematic review and meta-analysis. Overall, this study did a thorough and comprehensive discussion of previous literature and relevant theories. If the following issues can be addressed or clarified deeply, this study would be of good quality:

1. The authors stated that the operational definitions of mood and emotions in the Operational Definitions section, it is suggested that the operational definitions of alcohol consumption also be provided (page 8, line 189).

2. Although the three terms ‘mood’, ‘emotion’, and ‘feeling’ and ‘affect’ are often used interchangeably in the literature, there are many and significant debates regarding their similarities and differences in some aspects. The authors are encouraged to have a part discussing them in order to support the approach used: “... the terms ‘affect’, and ‘affective state’ are used in this review as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling”.

3. In the section of Literature Review, the authors used one of the commands for searching: ‘alcohol consumption’ rather than ‘alcohol’. This could lead to a loss of a certain amount of candidate studies for meta-analysis (page 9, line 225).

4. Also, in the section of Literature Review, the search term ‘feeling’ should be added as the authors mentioned ‘...as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling’ (page 9, line 226).

5. The meta-regression approach is used to work on “numeric moderator variables” such as year of publication. However, the study also applied this method, instead of the subgroup analysis approach, to examine the effects of categorical moderators (i.e., country and study design). Can the author clarify this?

6. Although no study with poor quality is included in this meta-analysis, running a moderator analysis of study quality has its great value.

7. In the ‘Meta-analysis – Analytical Strategy’ section, the authors mentioned ‘Several categorical moderators were examined ... alcohol consumption measure ...’. However, why the moderator analyses of such a variable (i.e., alcohol consumption measure) seem to be missing in Results.

8. In this meta-analysis, most of the included studies are from the USA; this may limit the extrapolation of the study’s result to some extent, even the related moderator analyses did not find an effect. It would be better that the study may have some discussion regarding this issue.

Reviewer #2: In this manuscript, Tovmasyan et al. performed meta-analyses for the relationship between affect and alcohol consumption. They found that the affect intensity, for both positive and negative affect, is associated with alcohol consumption. The study was well-conducted, the manuscript detailed the analysis approaches and methods, and the findings appear to be sound. The implications of the findings are likely to impact the field substantively.

Overall, there are no major issues with the manuscript. Minor issues are listed below:

1. Two separate figures were labeled as Figure 1: both the flowchart of the study selection process (line 329 – 372) and P-curve plot for studies on negative affect and alcohol consumption (line 435 – 438).

2. There were 149 references listed at the end of the manuscript. However, the text referred up to 157. The missing references need to be listed.

3. In the text, most of the time figures were referred to with a capital ‘F’. There were places where lower case ‘f’ was used (for example, line 431, 434, 469, 472). Please make it consistent.

4. Line 551: The reference number (142) should be after the word “literature”.

5. Line 614: Please add a comma after the word “consumption”.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Jan 31;17(1):e0262670. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0262670.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


14 Dec 2021

Dear Dr Moran,

Thank you for giving us the chance to revise this manuscript, and to both you and the reviewers for their constructive and helpful comments. Below we outline how we have responded to each reviewer comment. We have also uploaded a version of the manuscript with tracked changes, should this be desired by the reviewers. Having made these changes, we hope you will now find it acceptable for publication.

Best wishes,

Anna Tovmasyan

**The major concerns are associated with the search strategy used by the authors that may have missed key literature for the meta-analysis. Also of concern is the fact that the authors indicate that they have followed the PRISMA guidelines and, for example, do not present a full electronic search strategy for at least one database, including any limits used, such that it could be repeated. The search strategy and in which database, including limits used, should be indicated. Authors should strictly verify their adherence to the PRISMA guidelines in this manuscript.

Thanks for this comment. The following has been added to the methods section in order to more fully outline our search strategy, as you suggest: “The search was conducted on 2nd March 2020. For PsychINFO, after the filters ‘empirical study’ and ‘quantitative study’ were applied, the search yielded 8285 articles for screening. For PsychARTICLES, when the same filters were applied, the search yielded three articles. For Science Direct, as wildcards “*” were not supported, the search terms were ("alcohol " OR "drinking behavior” OR “drinking behaviour”) AND ("mood" OR "emotions" OR “feelings” OR "affective states") NOT "disorders". After the filter ‘research articles’ was applied, the search yielded 2327 articles. For PubMed (3189 citations), SCOPUS (1201 citations), and JSTOR (367 citations), no filters were applied. The citations were loaded to RefWorks software, and the duplicates were removed. … To ensure that all relevant literature published at the time was covered, a supplementary search was conducted on 29th January 2021, which yielded 3 additional references”.

The search has also been re-run following these suggestions proposed by reviewers (specified in more detail below). This resulted in inclusion of one additional citation.

**When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

This has now been amended.

**2. We note that you have referenced (Lenhard W, Lenhard A. Computation of Effect Sizes [Internet]. Unpublished;)which has currently not yet been accepted for publication. Please remove this from your References and amend this to state in the body of your manuscript: (ie “Lenhard W, Lenhard. [Unpublished]”) as detailed online in our guide for authors

http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-reference-style

This has now been amended.

**3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

This has now been amended.

Reviewers' comments:

**Reviewer #1: The study attempted to clarify the pooled association between affective states and alcohol consumption by conducting a systematic review and meta-analysis. Overall, this study did a thorough and comprehensive discussion of previous literature and relevant theories. If the following issues can be addressed or clarified deeply, this study would be of good quality:

1. The authors stated that the operational definitions of mood and emotions in the Operational Definitions section, it is suggested that the operational definitions of alcohol consumption also be provided (page 8, line 189).

Thank you for this helpful suggestion. The following has now been added to ‘Operational Definitions’ section: “Alcohol consumption is defined as ingesting any beverage containing ethanol.”

**2. Although the three terms ‘mood’, ‘emotion’, and ‘feeling’ and ‘affect’ are often used interchangeably in the literature, there are many and significant debates regarding their similarities and differences in some aspects. The authors are encouraged to have a part discussing them in order to support the approach used: “... the terms ‘affect’, and ‘affective state’ are used in this review as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling”.

Thank you for this valuable point. The following has now been added to ‘Operational Definitions’ section: “While it is possible that mood and emotions have different effects on alcohol consumption volume over longer periods of time, the current focus was on the effects of within a shorter timeframe, where the distinction between mood and emotions is arguably less important”. As the terms are often being used interchangeably, we decided to include all of them to make sure all the relevant literature is included.

In the discussion, we also now address this point and suggest that further research on the issue is required: “Furthermore, the current meta-analyses focused on affective states in general without looking at potential differences between mood and emotional state. While this decision was made because there is an overlap between these constructs and, for this reason, many studies use these terms interchangeably, further meta-analytical studies may wish to look at conceivably different effects of mood versus emotional state on alcohol consumption”.

**3. In the section of Literature Review, the authors used one of the commands for searching: ‘alcohol consumption’ rather than ‘alcohol’. This could lead to a loss of a certain amount of candidate studies for meta-analysis (page 9, line 225).

**4. Also, in the section of Literature Review, the search term ‘feeling’ should be added as the authors mentioned ‘...as umbrella terms for the experience of mood, emotion, or feeling’ (page 9, line 226).

Thank you for highlighting these important considerations. Given that the focus of the current paper was on the acute impact of affect on alcohol consumption, the search term ‘alcohol consumption’ was used rather than just ‘alcohol’. Given your suggestion, however, we did also carry out a new search using the broader search terms. Similarly, we felt that adding the term ‘feeling’ was unlikely result in additional relevant literature, as our review focused on quantitative research, where the scales tend to measure either mood (e.g., POMS), emotions (e.g., STAI), or affect in general (e.g., PANAS). On the other hand, feelings are more commonly measured by qualitative research studies, which were not included in the current study. That being said, it is important to question one’s (potentially biasing) assumptions and thank you for highlighting this. Accordingly, we carried out an additional search using this broader search term. When the search with updates terms has been carried out, this resulted in inclusion of one additional citation to our meta-analyses.

**5. The meta-regression approach is used to work on “numeric moderator variables” such as year of publication. However, the study also applied this method, instead of the subgroup analysis approach, to examine the effects of categorical moderators (i.e., country and study design). Can the author clarify this?

Of course. We followed the approach outlined in the book ‘Doing meta-analysis with R’ (Harrer et al., 2021, p. 198), according to which “subgroup analysis is nothing else than a meta-regression with a categorical predictor”. We have added the following clarification to our analytical strategy section “The effects of categorical moderators (i.e., country and study design) were assessed using meta-regression approach, as suggested by Harrer et al. (77).”

**6. Although no study with poor quality is included in this meta-analysis, running a moderator analysis of study quality has its great value.

We agree that this was an oversight. An analysis with study quality as a moderator has now been conducted and while study quality was not a significant moderator, this has now been added to the text.

**7. In the ‘Meta-analysis – Analytical Strategy’ section, the authors mentioned ‘Several categorical moderators were examined ... alcohol consumption measure ...’. However, why the moderator analyses of such a variable (i.e., alcohol consumption measure) seem to be missing in Results.

Thank you for highlighting this omission – the results of this moderator analysis have now been added to the manuscript.

**8. In this meta-analysis, most of the included studies are from the USA; this may limit the extrapolation of the study’s result to some extent, even the related moderator analyses did not find an effect. It would be better that the study may have some discussion regarding this issue.

This is a valuable point, which we now include in the discussion where we highlight the need for additional research in other national contexts, which may differ with regards to the sociocultural positioning of alcohol (p. 59).

**Reviewer #2: In this manuscript, Tovmasyan et al. performed meta-analyses for the relationship between affect and alcohol consumption. They found that the affect intensity, for both positive and negative affect, is associated with alcohol consumption. The study was well-conducted, the manuscript detailed the analysis approaches and methods, and the findings appear to be sound. The implications of the findings are likely to impact the field substantively.

Overall, there are no major issues with the manuscript. Minor issues are listed below:

***1. Two separate figures were labeled as Figure 1: both the flowchart of the study selection process (line 329 – 372) and P-curve plot for studies on negative affect and alcohol consumption (line 435 – 438).

Thank you for highlighting this – this has now been amended.

**2. There were 149 references listed at the end of the manuscript. However, the text referred up to 157. The missing references need to be listed.

Thank you, we appreciate your highlighting of this error – it seems to be stemming from incorrect numbering in the text rather than missing references. We have now thoroughly checked the manuscript to ensure all the references are in the correct place.

**3. In the text, most of the time figures were referred to with a capital ‘F’. There were places where lower case ‘f’ was used (for example, line 431, 434, 469, 472). Please make it consistent.

Thank you – this has now been amended

**4. Line 551: The reference number (142) should be after the word “literature”.

Thank you – this has now been amended

**5. Line 614: Please add a comma after the word “consumption”.

Thank you – this has now been amended.

Once again, we thank the reviewers and editors for their valuable feedback. This has enabled us to strengthen considerably the submission.

Decision Letter 1

Jose M Moran

4 Jan 2022

Towards an affect intensity regulation hypothesis: A systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between affective states and alcohol consumption

PONE-D-21-30664R1

Dear Dr. Tovmasyan,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Jose M. Moran

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Well written and studied. The authors have carefully and detailedly commented the remarks of the reviewers on an earlier version of the manuscript and have taken most of the remarks of the reviewers into consideration for the current version.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

Acceptance letter

Jose M Moran

20 Jan 2022

PONE-D-21-30664R1

Towards an affect intensity regulation hypothesis: Systematic review and meta-analyses of the relationship between affective states and alcohol consumption

Dear Dr. Tovmasyan:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Jose M. Moran

Section Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2009 checklist.

    (DOC)

    S1 File. S1, S2—Unpussblished references.

    (DOCX)

    Data Availability Statement

    All data files are available on Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/fe9au/.


    Articles from PLoS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES