Abstract
Background
Social relationships are important for pain management among individuals with HIV, but the impact of daily social contact on pain responses in real-time, real-world settings has never been specifically examined.
Purpose
The purpose of the present study was to examine the relationship between social contact frequency and pain, and the role of negative and positive affect in this relationship among older adults with HIV using ecological momentary assessment (EMA).
Methods
A total of 66 (Mage = 59.3, SD = 6.3, range: 50–74) older adults with HIV completed EMA surveys that included social contact frequency, pain level, and negative and positive affect four times per day for 2 weeks. Mixed-effects regression models were used to examine concurrent and lagged associations between social contact frequency, pain, and negative and positive affect.
Results
Greater recent social contact frequency was associated with less severe current pain (unstandardized B = −0.04, 95% CI: −0.08, −0.01, p = .014), while greater current pain was associated with lower subsequent social contact frequency (unstandardized B = −0.07, 95% CI: −0.11, −0.03, p < .001). Further, higher current negative affect was related to greater current pain, and this relationship was dampened by increased recent social contact frequency (unstandardized B = −0.17, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.08, p < .001). Neither negative nor positive affect was significantly associated with the relationship between current pain and subsequent social contact frequency.
Conclusions
Social contact frequency and pain are bidirectionally and inversely associated among older adults with HIV. Further, recent social contact influences current pain by attenuating negative affect. Together, these results highlight the need to address social engagement in interventions for pain among older adults with HIV.
Keywords: Mobile health, Ambulatory assessment, Social engagement, Loneliness, Chronic illness, Mood
Among older adults with HIV, recent social contact was associated with less current pain by attenuating negative affect, within persons.
Introduction
Due to the success of antiretroviral therapy (ART), the number of older adults living with HIV has rapidly increased [1]. As a result, there is a need to better understand factors that interfere with daily functioning and quality of life in this population, particularly pain. Pain is a common and highly impairing comorbidity among people with HIV (PWH) [2]. Pain in PWH is associated with poorer physical function and ART adherence, and greater depressive symptoms, substance use, and healthcare utilization [3–7]. Pain may be especially problematic among older PWH. For example, normal aging processes contribute to pain chronification in older adults via neurodegeneration, changes in circadian rhythms, and a gradual increase in age-related inflammation, termed “inflammaging” [8]. Subsequently, increased pain can exacerbate maladaptive coping mechanisms, such as behavioral avoidance of daily activities and social interactions, which may further exacerbate pain in a vicious downward cycle [9]. Identifying and understanding factors that disrupt this cycle may be key to maximizing functioning among older PWH experiencing pain.
Social relationships play an important role in physical and mental health [10–12]. The measurement of social relationships is inherently complex and multifaceted. Most research focuses on social support, typically measured by perceived or received support, and social integration, which refers to one’s engagement across a range of social activities [10, 13]. In regard to pain, social support and social integration tend to reduce pain experiences by attenuating negative affective experiences, which has been termed the social buffering effect [14]. It is also possible that social buffering operates through the modulation of positive affect; however, this mechanism has received less attention. There is evidence positive affect influences the experience of pain over and above that of negative affect and is associated with subsequent social engagement among individuals with chronic pain [15]. Thus, it is important to consider positive affect in addition to negative affect as a potential mechanism by which social buffering of pain may occur.
In this study, we were interested in a particular facet of social integration, social contact frequency. Social contact frequency refers to the quantity rather than the quality of interactions over a period of time [16]. There are several reasons why social contact frequency may be important among older PWH. First, older PWH are at high risk for social isolation [17], which can exacerbate pain as well as depressive symptoms, poor sleep, and suicidal ideation [18–22]. While this implies increased social contact frequency may be associated with improved outcomes among older PWH, to our knowledge this has never been specifically examined. Second, outside the HIV literature, social contact frequency mediated the relationship between physical pain and suicidal ideation among older adults [23], and is associated with decreased all-cause mortality [16]. Third, social contact frequency is a relatively objective measure. While other measures of social integration (e.g., satisfaction with one’s role) and social support are important for understanding the context around social interactions, social contact frequency is less prone to subjective interpretation [16].
The purpose of the present study was to better understand the relationship between social contact frequency, pain, and affect among older adult PWH in real-time, real-world settings using ecological momentary assessment (EMA). EMA involves the repeated sampling of behavior and experiences within natural environments that allows for the examination of short-term shifts and specific contexts on outcomes within individuals. While a previous study used EMA to examine the association between social support, pain, and personality characteristics among PWH [24], this is the first study to use EMA to examine the interrelationships between social contact frequency, pain, and affect. There were two overall aims. First, we examined the impact of recent social contact frequency on current pain (Aim 1a), and the impact of current pain on subsequent social contact frequency (Aim 1b), within-persons. We expected an inverse relationship between recent social contact frequency and current pain, as well as between current pain and subsequent social contact frequency. Second, we examined interactions among social contact frequency (recent and subsequent), pain, and affect (negative and positive). That is, we examined if recent social contact frequency interacted with current affect to influence current pain (Aim 2a), and similarly, if current pain interacted with current affect to influence subsequent social contact frequency (Aim 2b). We expected that higher current negative affect and lower current positive affect would be associated with higher current pain, and this relationship would be weakened by increased recent social contact frequency. Further, we expected the relationship between higher current pain and less subsequent social contact would be strongest when current negative affect was high and current positive affect was low.
Methods
Participants
Participants were 66 PWH enrolled in the Real-Time Mobile Assessment of Daily Functioning Among Older HIV-Infected Adults study conducted at the UC San Diego HIV Neurobehavioral Research Program (HNRP). Data were collected from February 2016 to May 2019. Inclusion criteria for the parent study were: (a) being at least 50 years of age, (b) English fluency, and (c) ability to provide written, informed consent. Exclusion criteria for the parent study were: (a) serious mental illness (e.g., schizophrenia); (b) history of a non-HIV neurological disease (e.g., stroke); (c) brain injury with loss of consciousness >30 min, (d) history of severe learning disability (i.e., WRAT-4 reading score <70), or (e) positive alcohol breathalyzer or urine toxicology for drugs of abuse (other than marijuana) at the baseline visit. Additional inclusion criteria for the current study was having HIV. The UC San Diego Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures. All participants demonstrate decisional capacity [25] and provided written informed consent.
Participant demographics and clinical characteristics are presented in Table 1. Participants were 59.3 years old on average (SD = 6.3; range: 50–74) and mostly non-Hispanic White (n = 42; 64%). The majority of participants were men (n = 53; 80%), which is comparable to gender demographics of HIV in the United States [26]. Most were unmarried (n = 59; 89.4%) and living alone (n = 38; 57.6%). In terms of HIV disease characteristics, participants were well controlled, with 94% (n = 62) on ART and 97% (n = 60) with undetectable HIV plasma viral loads.
Table 1.
Mean (SD) or N (%) | |
---|---|
Demographics | |
Age | 59.27 (6.31). Range: 50–74 |
Sex (women) | 13 (19.7) |
Years of education | 13.94 (2.45) |
Race/ethnicity | |
Non-Hispanic White | 42 (63.6%) |
Black | 15 (22.7%) |
Hispanic | 7 (10.6%) |
Other | 2 (3.0%) |
Marital status (currently married) | 7 (10.6%) |
Household size (living alone) | 38 (57.6%) |
HIV disease characteristics | |
History of AIDS (yes) | 46 (69.7%) |
Current CD4 count | 706.00 (199.96) |
Nadir CD4 | 188.92 (192.67) |
On ART (yes) | 62 (93.9%) |
Undetectable plasma viral loada | 60 (96.8%) |
Estimated years living with HIV | 22.76 (7.83) |
Aggregate EMA ratings | |
Pain | 2.93 (2.12) |
Negative affect | 1.44 (0.67) |
Positive affect | 3.26 (0.92) |
Number of social interactions | 1.72 (0.90) |
aLower limit of quantification = 50 copies/ml.
Procedure and Measures
Participants completed an in-person baseline visit followed by 14 days of EMA surveys in their natural environments.
Baseline visit
At the baseline visit, participants completed neuromedical and neurobehavioral evaluations. To lessen participant burden, participants who had been enrolled in another study at the HNRP within the past 6 months did not retake the neuromedical and neurobehavioral evaluations. HIV serostatus was determined using an HIV/HCV antibody point-of-care rapid test and confirmatory western blot analyses. HIV disease characteristics were collected via structured interview (i.e., estimated duration living with HIV, historical and current ART regimen, nadir CD4 count, and historical AIDS diagnosis) and reverse transcriptase-polymerase chain reaction on blood samples (i.e., current CD4 count and plasma HIV RNA). Participants were provided a touch-screen Samsung smartphone with a 4G Android Operating System, trained on its use and on EMA survey completion, and given a smartphone Operating Manual to take home. The mobile platform used an encrypted native application framework to ensure data could not be accessed if the device was lost or stolen.
Fourteen-day EMA study period
Participants received four EMA surveys per day on the study smartphone. Completion time for each survey was about 3 min. The delivery of EMA surveys occurred at random intervals separated by approximately 3 h and was timed to accommodate each participant’s sleep-wake schedule. At each survey delivery time, participants were alerted every 2 min until they responded or until the survey deactivated (i.e., 16 min after the initial alert). Every EMA survey included inquiries about social contact frequency, pain, and affect. To quantify social contact frequency, participants were asked, “Since the last alarm, how many times did you socialize with someone else [e.g., spent more than five minutes talking/communicating with someone else]?” with five response options from “0 (you had no interactions)” to “4 or more interactions”). The duration or modality of contact was not captured. Note that this question refers to social interactions experienced between the previous and current prompt. Pain was assessed by the question, “What is your pain level right now?” for which participants responded using a visual analog scale from 1 (minimal or no pain) to 10 (severe pain). Affect was assessed via five individual survey items asking participants to rate their current feelings of happiness, depression, worthlessness, anxiety, and worry (e.g., “I feel happy…”) on a Likert-type scale from 0 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Ratings from depression, worthlessness, anxiety, and worry were averaged to create a summary score reflecting negative affect. Ratings of happiness were used as a proxy for positive affect.
Statistical Analyses
To determine potential demographic covariates for all analyses examining the primary study aims, Pearson r correlations or t-tests were used to explore the relationships between continuous and dichotomous demographic characteristics (i.e., age, education level, sex, and race/ethnicity [non-Hispanic White vs. other], marital status [currently married vs. unmarried], household size [living alone vs. living with one or more persons]) and average levels of EMA-assessed variables of interest. To examine the primary aims (i.e., relationships between social contact frequency and pain), we conducted both concurrent and lagged analyses. In concurrent analyses with current pain as the outcome, social contact frequency represents recent social contact frequency because the prompt specified the number of social interactions since the last survey. In lagged analyses, we examined current pain as a predictor of subsequent social contact frequency (i.e., number of social interactions reported at the next survey).
For the first study aim, bivariate linear mixed-effects models were conducted to examine the unadjusted within-person relationships between recent social contact frequency and current pain (Aim 1a) and between current pain and subsequent social contact frequency (Aim 1b) controlling for between-person levels of pain and social contact frequency. To examine Aim 2a, linear mixed-effects models were used to determine the within-person independent and interactive effects of recent social contact frequency and current affect (negative and positive) on current pain level, covarying for time of day (i.e., 1 = morning, 2 = midday, 3 = afternoon/evening, 4 = night time), average affect, and average social contact frequency [27]. Current positive and negative affect were person-mean centered. These models included person-specific random intercepts and random slopes for social interactions and affect. To examine Aim 2b, linear mixed-effects models were used to determine the within-person independent and interactive effects of current pain and current affect (negative and positive) on subsequent social contact frequency, covarying for time of day, average pain level, and average affect. Current pain and affect were person-mean centered. For these lagged analyses, pain and affect responses from the last survey of each day were not used to predict social contact frequency from the next morning survey. These models included person-specific random intercepts and random slopes for pain and affect. Both unstandardized and standardized regression estimates are reported. Standardized regression estimates were used as an estimate of effect size for individual predictors [28]. All analyses were conducted using R, version 3.5.0. Multilevel models were examined using the “lme4” package [29].
Results
Being currently married and living with one or more persons was associated with greater social contact frequency (p’s > .05). Other demographic variables (age, sex, years of education, race/ethnicity) were unrelated to social contact frequency, pain, negative affect, or positive affect (p’s < .05). Bivariate within-person relationships among recent and subsequent social contact frequency, current pain, and current negative and positive affect are shown in Table 2. In regard to Aims 1a and 1b, higher recent social contact frequency was associated with lower current pain (unstandardized B = −0.04, 95% CI: −0.08, −0.01, p = .014; standardized B = −0.06), and higher current pain was associated with less subsequent social contact frequency (unstandardized B = −0.07, 95% CI: −0.11, −0.03, p < .001; standardized B = −0.03), respectively, controlling for between-person levels of pain and social contact frequency. To verify the strength of these associations, two sets of sensitivity analyses were performed by including average negative and positive affect as covariates, and by including marital status and household size as covariates to these models. Findings were unchanged.
Table 2.
Predictors | Subsequent social contact frequencya | Pain level | Negative affect | Positive affect |
---|---|---|---|---|
Recent social contact frequency | 0.18 (0.14, 0.22)** | −0.04 (−0.08, −0.01)* | −0.01 (−0.02, 0.00) | 0.10 (0.08, 0.01)** |
Pain level (person-centered) | −0.07 (−0.11, −0.03)** | − | 0.04 (0.03, 0.05)** | −0.09 (−0.11, −0.07)** |
Negative affect (person-centered) | −0.10 (−0.24, 0.04) | 0.43 (0.31, 0.55)** | − | −0.54 (−0.61, −0.46)** |
Positive affect (person-centered) | 0.14 (0.07, 0.02)** | −0.21 (−0.26, −0.16)** | −0.11 (−0.13, −0.09)** | − |
Note. Values are unstandardized regression coefficients (95% CI).
aValues in this column represent the lagged relationship between each variable and subsequent social contact frequency.
*p < .05; **p < .001.
Results of all four linear-mixed effects models associated with Aim 2 are displayed in Table 3. In regard to Aim 2a, the linear mixed-effects model examining the within-person independent and interactive effects of recent social contact frequency and current negative affect on current pain revealed a significant interaction (unstandardized B = −0.17, 95% CI: −0.26, −0.08, p < .001; standardized B = −0.051). This finding was unchanged by including marital status and household size as covariates. When specifying recent social contact frequency as the moderator, simple slopes analysis revealed that the within-person relationship between current negative affect and current pain was weakened at times when recent social contact frequency was high (Fig. 1). When examining positive affect, results showed that there was no significant interaction between subsequent social contact frequency and current positive affect on current pain within persons (p = .35). In regard to Aim 2b, the interaction between current pain and current negative affect did not significantly predict subsequent social contact frequency within persons (p = .54). The interaction between current pain and current positive affect, however, was marginally significant within persons (p = .077; Table 3). An exploratory simple slopes analysis showed that the negative within-person relationship between current pain and subsequent social contact frequency was weakest at times when current positive affect was high.
Table 3.
Unstandardized estimate (95% CI) | Standardized estimate (95% CI) | p-value | |
---|---|---|---|
Outcome: pain level (Aim 2a) | |||
Time of day | 0.037 (0.003, 0.070) | 0.017 (0.002, 0.033) | .032 |
Average recent social contact frequency | 0.284 (−0.278, 0.845) | 0.107 (−0.105, 0.319) | .326 |
Average negative affect | 1.143 (0.391, 1.894) | 0.307 (0.105, 0.509) | .004 |
Recent social contact frequency | −0.029 (−0.072, 0.013) | −0.018 (−0.044, 0.008) | .181 |
Negative affect (person-centered) | 0.629 (0.361, 0.898) | 0.087 (0.050, 0.124) | <.001 |
Recent social contact × negative affect | −0.169 (−0.260, −0.077) | −0.051 (−0.079, −0.023) | <.001 |
Outcome: pain level (Aim 2a) | |||
Time of day | 0.032 (−0.001, 0.065) | 0.015 (−0.001, 0.031) | .057 |
Average recent social contact frequency | 0.122 (−0.486, 0.730) | 0.046 (−0.183, 0.275) | .696 |
Average positive affect | −0.234 (−0.829, 0.361) | −0.089 (−0.315, 0.137) | .443 |
Recent social contact frequency | −0.001 (−0.046, 0.046) | −0.001 (−0.028, 0.027) | .987 |
Positive affect (person-centered) | −0.245 (−0.385, −0.105) | −0.074 (−0.116, −0.032) | .001 |
Recent social contact × positive affect | 0.021 (−0.022, 0.064) | 0.014 (−0.016, 0.044) | .349 |
Outcome: subsequent social contact frequency (Aim 2b) | |||
Time of day | −0.127 (−0.181, −0.073) | −0.073 (−0.104, −0.042) | <0.001 |
Average pain level | 0.088 (−0.017, 0.194) | 0.129 (−0.025, 0.283) | 0.106 |
Average negative affect | −0.421 (−0.757, −0.084) | −0.185 (−0.334, −0.037) | 0.017 |
Pain level (person-centered) | −0.057 (−0.116, 0.001) | −0.046 (−0.092, 0.001) | 0.064 |
Negative affect (person-centered) | −0.026 (−0.211, 0.158) | −0.006 (−0.048, 0.036) | 0.781 |
Pain level × negative affect | −0.033 (−0.137, 0.072) | −0.011 (−0.047, 0.024) | 0.540 |
Outcome: subsequent social contact frequency (Aim 2b) | |||
Time of day | −0.126 (−0.180, −0.072) | −0.072 (−0.103, −0.041) | <0.001 |
Average pain level | 0.052 (−0.047, 0.152) | 0.077 (−0.069, 0.222) | 0.307 |
Average positive affect | 0.390 (0.159, 0.621) | 0.244 (0.100, 0.388) | 0.002 |
Pain level (person-centered) | −0.056 (−0.111, −0.001) | −0.045 (−0.088, 0.000) | 0.052 |
Positive affect (person-centered) | 0.132 (0.050, 0.214) | 0.066 (0.025, 0.106) | 0.003 |
Pain level × positive affect | 0.045 (−0.005, 0.095) | 0.031 (−0.003, 0.064) | 0.077 |
Discussion
This study sought to better understand the association between social contact frequency, pain, and affect among older adult PHW in real-time, real-world settings using advances in mobile technology. Recent social contact frequency was inversely associated with current pain, and current pain was inversely associated with subsequent social contact frequency. Further, there was a significant within-person effect that demonstrated higher current negative affect was related to higher current pain, and this relationship was weakest at times when recent social contact frequency was high. The influence of pain on subsequent social contact frequency was not significantly moderated by within-person variability in current positive or negative affect, suggesting current pain relates to less subsequent social interactions regardless of affect.
While previous work has shown an association between social contact frequency and pain [23], to our knowledge this is the first study to report the bidirectional impact of social contact frequency and pain within-persons using EMA. Given that we covaried for average levels of pain, affect, and social contact frequency, as well as marital status and household size, our findings suggest that significant within-person effects were independent of between-person variability in these variables or living situation. That is, our findings demonstrate how observable changes within a person relate to behavioral patterns at the individual level. This highlights the importance of considering the role of pain in the context of social isolation among older PWH. Specifically, pain may exacerbate social isolation in a downward spiral, such that greater pain may lead to less social contact, which in turn leads to more pain. Given the importance of maintaining social networks for promoting QoL and successful aging in PWH [30], it is especially important that clinicians assess pain severity and interference when discussing social activity in older PWH.
The relationship between negative affect and pain is well-established, with many studies showing negative affective states are generally associated with greater pain sensitivity (see [31] for a review). Results showed that the relationship between current negative affect and current pain was weakened by recent social contact frequency (i.e., social buffering). Again, we would like to emphasize that this effect was found within-persons. This is in contrast to a previous EMA study that showed the association between increased social support and lower pain varied by personality characteristics (attachment-related insecurity) among PWH [24], a between-person factor. Our findings suggest that individual variations in negative affect influence when individuals benefit from social contact. For example, older PWH may be more likely to experience social buffering on pain at times when negative affect is high. Or said the opposite, when social contact has been less recent, a strong and positive association between current pain and current negative affect would be expected.
Although the moderating role of current affect in the association between higher current pain and less subsequent social contact frequency was not statistically significant, there was a non-significant trend whereby the negative impact of current pain on subsequent social interactions was diminished at times when current positive affect was high. Negative and positive affect are related but distinct constructs [32], and a 2015 review highlights the importance of considering the protective role of positive affect in the management of pain [15]. Nonetheless, our findings suggest additional processes influence the relationship between current pain and subsequent social interactions. One possibility is pain expectancy. In both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, pain expectancy was associated with greater behavioral avoidance [33, 34], even after controlling for pain level and negative affect [35]. Indeed, among individuals with chronic pain, the threat of increased future pain prevented social engagement with family and friends [36]. This was also found among PWH, who reported avoidance of social activity as a self-management strategy for pain [37]. Thus, an important clinical implication from the current study is that pain management treatments for PWH may benefit from including psychoeducation on the benefits of social activity as well as interventions to increase social interactions.
It is important to point out that not all social interactions confer pain benefits but may instead exacerbate pain. Among research on couples, negative interactions (e.g., expressing frustration and anger about pain) and solicitous responses (e.g., encouragement to be less active) from significant others are generally associated with greater pain levels and disability among individuals with chronic pain [38, 39]. Interaction types associated with decreased pain behavior and improved functioning include validating responses and emotional disclosure [40, 41]. Although we cannot directly speak to the content, quality, or duration of interactions in this study, our bivariate analyses revealed a significant positive association between recent social contact frequency and current positive affect, suggesting that on average interactions were likely positive. This is fitting with past research showing social interactions tend to be more supportive among older adult PWH relative to younger adult PWH [42]. Nonetheless, because social interactions may have a buffering or amplifying effect on pain, it is important that clinical interventions carefully consider the dynamics within social networks and devise approaches to minimize negative consequences (e.g., decreasing the impact of negative interactions, managing solicitous responses) and maximize positive consequences (e.g., encouragement for emotional disclosure where appropriate, strategies for increasing the enjoyment and meaning of interactions).
In general, our findings suggest being less socially engaged is associated with greater pain in older PWH. While data were collected prior to the COVID-19 pandemic, this is especially relevant in the context of the ongoing pandemic where social isolation and loneliness are likely to be exacerbated. This is compounded in older PWH given they are at high risk for adverse events if infected by the virus and may be asked or required to adhere to stay-at-home orders for the foreseeable future. It may be imperative to implement social outreach programs in this vulnerable group to help manage pain, as well as to help preserve overall physical and mental health functioning.
There are several strengths to the current study. First, the study adds to the limited literature on the social buffering effect on pain among older PWH and provides additional rationale for addressing social contact frequency in clinical intervention approaches. Second, the social buffering effect on pain was moderated by a within-person variation of negative affect, suggesting high generalizability. Third, the present study provides evidence for the bidirectional influence of pain and social interactions in older PWH. The study also has limitations. First, due to our modest sample size, additional studies with larger samples sizes are needed to replicate findings. Second, chronic pain was not an inclusion criterion for the study. Our findings may not completely generalize to PWH who have chronic pain. Third, we did not have access to other variables that are important to consider when examining social relationships. In addition to adding measures of perceived and received social support, we also encourage future research to collect information on the form of interactions (in-person vs. digital), which is especially relevant given COVID-19 related physical distancing requirements. Finally, only one variable, happiness, was used as a proxy for positive affect. Other components of positive affect include joy, contentment, and excitement [43]. Future studies are encouraged to capture additional components of positive affect to more thoroughly understand its contribution to the association between social engagement and pain.
Conclusions
This study demonstrated the within-person association between current negative affect and current pain was buffered by recent social contact frequency. Higher current pain was also associated with fewer subsequent social interactions; however, this was not dependent on current levels of negative or positive affect. Methods to increase social contact frequency should be considered in pain management interventions for PWH. To accomplish this, additional research is encouraged to identify barriers and facilitators of social engagement despite the presence of pain.
Funding
Funding for this project came from the National Institute of Mental Health K23 MH107260 (PI: Moore) as well as P30MH062512. Dr Herbert is supported by VA RR&D CDA Grant 1IK2RX002807-01A2 and partially supported by R01DK106415 from National Institute of Diabetes and Kidney Disease. Dr Wooldridge is supported by the VA Office of Academic Affiliates advanced fellowship in women’s health. Ms. Paolillo is supported by T32 AA013525 and F31AA027198. Dr Moore is supported by R01AG062387 and R21MH116104. The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not reflect the official policy or position of the funding agency, Department of Veterans Affairs, the United States Government, or any institutions with which the authors are affiliated.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards Dr Moore is a co-founder of KeyWise AI and a consultant for NeuroUX. All other authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Authors’ Contributions M.S.H. conceived the study, interpreted the data, and drafted the manuscript. J.S.W. assisted in the conceptualization of the study, interpreted the data, and contributed to manuscript drafting. E.W.P. analyzed the data and contributed to manuscript drafting. C.A.D. and R.C.M. assisted in the design of the study and critically revised the article for important intellectual content. All authors approved the final manuscript.
Ethical Approval All procedures were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinski Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.
Informed Consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
References
- 1. Brooks JT, Buchacz K, Gebo KA, Mermin J. HIV infection and older Americans: The public health perspective. Am J Public Health. 2012;102:1516–1526. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Parker R, Stein DJ, Jelsma J. Pain in people living with HIV/AIDS: A systematic review. J Int AIDS Soc. 2014;17:18719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Jiao JM, So E, Jebakumar J, George MC, Simpson DM, Robinson-Papp J. Chronic pain disorders in HIV primary care: Clinical characteristics and association with healthcare utilization. Pain. 2016;157:931–937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Surratt HL, Kurtz SP, Levi-Minzi MA, Cicero TJ, Tsuyuki K, O’Grady CL. Pain treatment and antiretroviral medication adherence among vulnerable HIV-positive patients. AIDS Patient Care STDS. 2015;29:186–192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Merlin JS, Westfall AO, Chamot E, et al. . Pain is independently associated with impaired physical function in HIV-infected patients. Pain Med. 2013;14:1985–1993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Uebelacker LA, Weisberg RB, Herman DS, Bailey GL, Pinkston-Camp MM, Stein MD. Chronic pain in HIV-infected patients: Relationship to depression, substance use, and mental health and pain treatment. Pain Med. 2015;16:1870–1881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. Newville H, Roley J, Sorensen JL. Prescription medication misuse among HIV-infected individuals taking antiretroviral therapy. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2015;48:56–61. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8. Shpaner M, Tulipani LJ, Bishop JH, Naylor MR: The vicious cycle of chronic pain in aging requires multidisciplinary non-pharmacological approach to treatment. Curr Behav Neurosci Reports. 2017; 4:176–187. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Huggins JL, Bonn-Miller MO, Oser ML, Sorrell JT, Trafton JA. Pain anxiety, acceptance, and outcomes among individuals with HIV and chronic pain: A preliminary investigation. Behav Res Ther. 2012;50:72–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. Plos Med. 2010;7:e1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Tay L, Kuykendall L, Diener E: Satisfaction and happiness – The bright side of quality of life. In: Global Handbook of Quality of Life: Exploration of Well-Being of Nations and Continents. Netherlands: Springer; 2015:839–853. [Google Scholar]
- 12. Uchino BN. Social support and health: A review of physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease outcomes. J Behav Med. 2006;29:377–387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Fuller-Iglesias HR, Rajbhandari S. Development of a multidimensional scale of social integration in later life. Res Aging. 2016;38:3–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Che X, Cash R, Ng SK, Fitzgerald P, Fitzgibbon BM. A systematic review of the processes underlying the main and the buffering effect of social support on the experience of pain. Clin J Pain. 2018;34:1061–1076. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Finan PH, Garland EL. The role of positive affect in pain and its treatment. Clin J Pain. 2015;31:177–187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Shor E, Roelfs DJ. Social contact frequency and all-cause mortality: A meta-analysis and meta-regression. Soc Sci Med. 2015;128:76–86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Shippy RA, Karpiak SE. The aging HIV/AIDS population: Fragile social networks. Aging Ment Health. 2005;9:246–254. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Turan B, Smith W, Cohen MH, et al. . Mechanisms for the negative effects of internalized HIV-related stigma on antiretroviral therapy adherence in women: The mediating roles of social isolation and depression. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2016;72:198–205. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Fekete EM, Williams SL, Skinta MD. Internalised HIV-stigma, loneliness, depressive symptoms and sleep quality in people living with HIV. Psychol Health. 2018;33:398–415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Grov C, Golub SA, Parsons JT, Brennan M, Karpiak SE. Loneliness and HIV-related stigma explain depression among older HIV-positive adults. AIDS Care. 2010;22:630–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Quintana-Ortiz RA, Gomez MA, Báez Feliciano DV, Hunter-Mellado RF. Suicide attempts among Puerto Rican men and women with HIV/AIDS: A study of prevalence and risk factors. Ethn Dis. 2008;18:S2–219. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Paolillo EW, Tang B, Depp CA, et al. . Temporal associations between social activity and mood, fatigue, and pain in older adults with HIV: An ecological momentary assessment study. JMIR Ment Health. 2018;5:e38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Lutzman M, Sommerfeld E, Ben-David S. Loneliness and social integration as mediators between physical pain and suicidal ideation among elderly men. Int Psychogeriatrics. 2020; 1–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Crockett KB, Turan B. Moment-to-moment changes in perceived social support and pain for men living with HIV: An experience sampling study. Pain. 2018;159:2503–2511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Jeste DV, Palmer BW, Appelbaum PS, et al. . A new brief instrument for assessing decisional capacity for clinical research. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:966–974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. HIV Surveillance Report, 2018 (Updated), vol. 31. http://www.cdc.gov/hiv/library/reports/hiv-surveillance.html. 2020.
- 27. Nordgren R, Hedeker D, Dunton G, Yang CH. Extending the mixed-effects model to consider within-subject variance for Ecological Momentary Assessment data. Stat Med. 2020;39:577–590. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Lorah J: Effect size measures for multilevel models: Definition, interpretation, and TIMSS example. Large-Scale Assessments Educ. 2018;6:1–11. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker BM, Walker SC. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48. [Google Scholar]
- 30. High KP, Brennan-Ing M, Clifford DB, et al. . HIV and aging. JAIDS J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2012; 60:S1–S18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Lumley MA, Cohen JL, Borszcz GS, et al. . Pain and emotion: A biopsychosocial review of recent research. J Clin Psychol. 2011;67:942–968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Larsen JT, Hershfield HE, Stastny BJ, Hester N. On the relationship between positive and negative affect: Their correlation and their co-occurrence. Emotion. 2017;17:323–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Bostick GP, Toth C, Dick BD, Carr EC, Stitt LW, Moulin DE. An adaptive role for negative expected pain in patients with neuropathic pain. Clin J Pain. 2015;31:438–443. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Crombez G, Vervaet L, Baeyens F, Lysens R, Eelen P. Do pain expectancies cause pain in chronic low back patients? A clinical investigation. Behav Res Ther. 1996;34:919–925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Boersma K, Linton SJ. Expectancy, fear and pain in the prediction of chronic pain and disability: A prospective analysis. Eur J Pain. 2006;10:551–557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Closs SJ, Staples V, Reid I, Bennett MI, Briggs M. The impact of neuropathic pain on relationships. J Adv Nurs. 2009;65:402–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Merlin JS, Walcott M, Kerns R, Bair MJ, Burgio KL, Turan JM. Pain self-management in HIV-infected individuals with chronic pain: A qualitative study. Pain Med. 2015;16:706–714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Leonard MT, Cano A, Johansen AB. Chronic pain in a couples context: A review and integration of theoretical models and empirical evidence. J Pain. 2006;7:377–390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Jensen MP, Moore MR, Bockow TB, Ehde DM, Engel JM. Psychosocial factors and adjustment to chronic pain in persons with physical disabilities: a systematic review. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2011;92:146–160. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Prenevost MH, Reme SE. Couples coping with chronic pain: How do intercouple interactions relate to pain coping? Scand J Pain. 2017;16:150–157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Cano A, de C Williams AC. Social interaction in pain: reinforcing pain behaviors or building intimacy? Pain. 2010;149:9–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Mavandadi S, Zanjani F, Ten Have TR, Oslin DW. Psychological well-being among individuals aging with HIV: The value of social relationships. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr. 2009;51:91–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Pressman SD, Cohen S. Does positive affect influence health? Psychol Bull. 2005;131:925–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]