Skip to main content
. 2022 Feb 18;11:e63405. doi: 10.7554/eLife.63405

Figure 2. Effect of sensory modality on participants' responses.

(A) Geometric definition of analysis variables.

The gray solid line indicates an example trajectory. The target and response distance and angle relative to the starting position of the participant are given by r,θ (thin lines) and r~,θ~ (thick lines), respectively. (B C) Example participant: Comparison of the radial distance r~ of an example participant’s response (final position) against the radial distance r of the target (B), as well as the angular eccentricity of the participant’s response θ~ vs. target angle θ (C), across all trials for one participant, colored according to the sensory condition (green: vestibular, cyan: visual, purple: combined visual and vestibular; Figure 2—source data 1). Radial and angular response gains were defined as the slope of the corresponding regressions. Black dashed lines show unity slope, and the solid lines represent slopes of the regression fits (intercept set to 0). (D) All participants: Radial and angular gains in each sensory condition plotted for each individual participant (Figure 2—source data 2). Ellipses show 68% confidence intervals of the distribution of data points for the corresponding sensory condition. Diamonds (centers of the ellipses) represent the mean radial and angular response gains across participants. Dashed lines indicate unbiased radial or angular position responses. Solid diagonal line has unit slope. (E) Magnitudes of radial and angular components of control inputs across sensory conditions for an example participant. Shaded regions represent ±1 standard deviation across trials. The gray zone corresponds to the target presentation period.

Figure 2—source data 1. Radial and angular responses.
Figure 2—source data 2. Response gains.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Trajectories in all conditions and sex differences in performance.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Random subset of trajectories of an example participant under each sensory condition. The corresponding radial and angular response gains are indicated for each condition (green: vestibular, cyan: visual, purple: combined). Gray region represents the target range. (B) Sex differences in participants’ performance: radial and angular gains (see Figure 2D) grouped based on sex (F: female, M: male; see legend; p-values of differences in response gains between male and female participants: Radial gain – vestibular: p = 0.17, visual: p = 0.09, combined: p = 0.09; angular gain – vestibular: p = 0.58, visual: p = 0.38, combined: p = 0.21; two-sample t-test). (C) Sex differences in participants’ performance: correlation coefficients between the time constant and the residual errors (radial and angular components; see Figure 3C) grouped based on sex. Specifically, the x and y axes represent the correlation values between the time constant and the radial and angular residual errors, respectively (p-values of differences in correlation coefficients between male and female participants: Radial – vestibular: p = 0.5, visual: p = 0.66, combined: p = 0.71; angular – vestibular: p = 0.51, visual: p = 0.97 combined: p = 0.82 two-sample t-test).
Figure 2—figure supplement 2. Joystick inputs.

Figure 2—figure supplement 2.

(A–G) Linear (left) and angular (right) joystick input over time, for a subset of participants in all conditions (see legend; bottom right). The joystick control had shorter duration in the vestibular condition, reflecting our findings of the smaller response gains. Shaded regions represent ±1 standard deviation across trials.