Skip to main content
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2023 Mar 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021 Dec 29;134:104487. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.12.010

Table 2.

Study design, sample characteristics, and study results for articles on widow(er)s

Authors (year) Quality Study Design; Length of Observation (years) Reference Group Time Since Loss Total N (% female) n Reference Group; n Widow(er)s Mean Age (SD) or Age Range or % 65+ Cognitive Assessment(s) Cognitive Domain Results Controlled for age, sex, SES?

Aartsen et al. (2005) 3 Longitudinal; 6 Married subjects 37 months 1164 (40.7%) 940 (36 %); 204 (66 %) 2089 (59.9 %) 60–85 AVLT Memory Widow(er)s had greater memory decline than control Yes
Amieva et al. (2010) 3 Longitudinal; 15 Married subjects nr 1267 (nr); 679 (nr) 73.7 (6.0) Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia Verbal memory, Being widowed was not associated with greater odds of dementia or AD than being married Widow(er)s had poorer cognitive function than married Yes
Barragán- García et al. (2021) 3 Cross-sectional Married subjects 0–11+ years 6898 (59.3 %) 4349 (50 %); 1624 (76.8 %) 70.86 (7.43) CCCE Verbal memory, visuospatial ability/ memory Widow(er)s had poorer cognitive function than married adults, with longer widowhood duration associated with lower overall cognition. Yes
Bickel and Cooper (1994) 3 Longitudinal; 7.5 Married subjects nr 314 (65.0 %) 133 (nr); 149 (nr) 73.8 (6.0) Dementia diagnosis (clinician, patient records, proxy report) Incident dementia Being widowed was not associated with increased odds of dementia No
Biddle et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 3.1 Married subjects nr 257 (59.5 %) 145 (45.5 %); 35 (88.6 %) 73.5 (6.1) Preclinical Alzheimer Cognitive Composite included: MMSE, LMDR, DSST, FCSRT Overall cognitive function, memory, executive function Widow(er)s declined in cognitive performance compared to married subjects Yes
Brenowitz et al. (2014). 3 Prospective; 3.2 Married subjects nr 5335 (67.1 %) 3106 (nr); 1263 (nr) 10,940 (48.4 %) 83.7 % MCI diagnosis (clinician) nr Widowers had lower risk of MCI than controls. Yes
Brown et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 14 Married subjects 1–14 years 9951 (53.2 %); 419 (79.5 %) 50–65 TICS-m Global cognition; cognitive impairment Widowed men had higher rates of cognitive impairment than married/re-partnered men. Widowed women did not show differences in cognitive function compared to married women. Yes
Byrne and Raphael (1997) 1 Cross-sectional Married subjects 6 weeks 114 (0%) 57 (0%); 57 (0%) 74.94 (nr)* MMSE Global cognition Widowed men did not differ from married men on overall cognitive performance. Yes
Espinosa del Pozo et al. (2020) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 80 (67 %) 48(nr); 16(nr) 7964 (50.5 %) 100% MMSE, AD8 global cognition, self-report dementia symptoms Widow(er)s did not differ on global cognition or risk for dementia than married adults. Yes
Fan et al. (2015) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 5111 (nr); 2471(nr) 2874 (nr) 75.69 (nr)* CDR, MMSE Global cognition; cognitive impairment Being widowed was associated with increased odds of dementia. Yes
Farr on et al. (2020) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr nr; nr 2498 (56.6 %) 1857 (56.6 %); 68.6 (nr) Summary cognition score of 18 cognitive tests: WR, animal naming, MoCA CDT, HMSE, DSF, symbol cancellation, LM, constructional praxis, hand sequence, token test, judgment, problem solving, CSI-D, CPM, GNG Overall cognition: memory, retrieval fluency, language, visuospatial skills, executive function, orientation, attention/speed, judgment Widow(er)s had worse overall cognition than married adults. Yes
Feng et al. (2014) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 414 (90.8 %) 66.0 MMSE (cognitive impairment = <24) Global cognition Widowed men had greater odds of cognitive impairment than married men. No differences were found between widowed and married women. No differences found when widow (er)s were compared with married adults. Yes
Forbes et al. (2019) 3 Longitudinal; 1.9–8 Married subjects 0–5 years 247,586 (65.7 %) 123,793 (65.7 %) 123,793(65.7%) 74.2c Dementia diagnosis (patient records) Incident dementia Losing a spouse is associated with receiving a dementia diagnosis within 3 and 6 months of the loss but not associated with long-term risk of dementia Yes
Guaita et al. (2015) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 1321 (54.1 %) 872 (nr); 325 (nr) 1999 (72.4 %) 1157 (56.4%); 71.69 (1.45) Diagnosis of dementia or cognitive impairment no dementia (clinician) incident dementia or mild cognitive impairment Being widowed was not associated with increased odds of dementia or cognitive impairment Those who were widowed at midlife were more likely to show cognitive impairment later in Yes
Hakansson et al. (2009) 3 Longitudinal; 21 Married/cohabiting subjects nr 116 (95.7%) 3633 (nr) 50.4 (6.0)d Incident dementia Incident dementia life than those cohabiting with a partner in mid-life. Still widowed individuals had 3× risk of cognitive impairment thaan married/cohabiting people. Yes
Hatch (2013) 3 Longitudinal; 13 Nonbereaved subjects nr 2395 (47.2 %); 1088 (42.0 %) 74.5 (nr)* Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia There was no association between widowhood and risk of dementia. Yes
Helmer et al. (1999) 3 Longitudinal; 5 Married subjects nr 3675 (58 %) 2106 (nr); 1287 (nr) 100 % Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia Widowed adults did not differ from married adults in dementia risk. Yes
Jia et al. (2020) 3 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 39,938 (nr); 3676 (nr) 6476 (61.3 %) 70.26 (7.51) Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia Being widowed was associated with greater risk of dementia or mild cognitive impairment. Yes
Karlamangla et al. (2009) 3 Longitudinal; 9 Married subjects nr 3354 (nr); 2583 (nr) 77.1 (nr) TICS Global cognition Widow(er)s had faster declines on total cognition and recall than married adults. Baseline marital status was not associated with baseline cognitive function. Widow(er)s had greater Yes
Kim et al. (2019) 3 Longitudinal; 2 Married subjects with diverse social networka nr 1960 (48.2 %) 781 (36.5 %); 321 (80.7 %) 923 (62.0 %) 72.05 (5.35) MMSE Global cognition cognitive decline than married adults who had close contact with social groups. Widow(er)s have higher rates of cognitive impairment than married adults. Widowed Yes
Kramer et al. (1985). 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 395 (41.0 %); 393 (83.7 %) 2618 (58.6 %) 65–74 MMSE (severe impairment = 0—17) Global cognition females have higher rates of cognitive impairment than married females. No differences across males by marital status. No
Lee et al. (2019). 3 Longitudinal; 4 Married subjects nr 2072 (51.5 %); 546 (85.5 %) 2462 (50.4 %) 51–93 Total cognition: WR, SS7, Backward counting memory, working memory, attention and processing speed There were no significant association between widowhood and cognitive function across a 4-year period. Yes
Li et al. (2018) 2 Longitudinal; 30 Married subjects 2131 (nr); 61 (nr) 40–65 Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia Widowhood was associated with increased risk of dementia for those aged 50 +. No
Liu et al. (2019) 3 Longitudinal; 7 Married subjects nr 7508(58.2%) 3609 (41.1%); 2542 (81.2%) 15,379 (54.97 %) 100% Overall cognition and independent domains assessed using the following assessments: WR, CDT, time orientation. If unable to complete the tests, cognitive status measured by AD diagnosis or AD8. Overall cognitive score was categorized into 3 groups: dementia, cognitive impairment, normal cognition. Memory, orientation, executive function Widow(er)s had greater cognitive impairment in memory, executive function and orientation than married adults. They also had higher prevalence rates of cognitive impairment (not dementia) and dementia. Yes
Liu et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 14 Married subjects nr 10,105(46.2%); 3007 (81.9 %) 1145 (63.4%) 65.91 (9.88) TICS, proxy assessment of memory and IADLs Global cognition/ dementia category Widowed respondents (and all unmarried groups) had higher odds of dementia than married respondents. The transition into widowhood, but not to divorce or remarriage, is associated with higher odds of dementia. Yes
Lopes et al. (2007) 1 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 660 (nr); 315 (nr) 3660 (42.5 %) 70.9 (60–100) MMSE (< 26), FOME (< 35), IQCODE (> 3.40), B-ADL (> 3.19) global cognition: language, attention, memory, verbal fluency, verbal and tactile recognition; activities of daily living Widowhood was not associated with cognitive and functional impairment Yes
Lyu et al. (2019) 3 Longitudinal; 8 Married subjects 0–6+ years 3266(nr); 394 (nr) 3329 (nr) 68.5 (6.16) MMSE Global cognition Widow(er)s bereaved for 4—6 years had steeper declines in global cognitive functioning than nonwidowed individuals. Widowed women had lower initial levels of cognitive Yes
Monserud (2019). 3 Longitudinal; 17 Married men nr nr; nr 1882 (54.5 %) 1422 (47.3 %); 65–109 MMSE Global cognition Widow(er)s bereaved for 4—6 years had steeper declines in global cognitive functioning than nonwidowed individuals. Widowed women had lower initial levels of cognitive Yes
Mousavi-Nasab et al. (2012) 3 Longitudinal; 5 Married or single subjects nr 242 (86.8 %) 77 (72 %) 32 (68.8 %); 35–85b Free/cued recall and recognition tests, vocabulary and fluency tests Episodic memory, semantic memory divorced adults. Older widow (er)s had poorer semantic memory performance (vocabulary) than older married people. Widow(er)s showed no differences in recall or fluency from married, divorced, single adults. Widow(er)s with complicated No
O’Connor and Arizmendi (2014) NA Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 45(73.3%) 71.85 (nr)* e-Stroop, MMSE, DSB, WCST Global cognition, working memory, executive function, emotional inhibition grief had poorer overall cognitive performance and exhibited greater interference (slower reaction times) to emotional words, than nonbereaved group. No No
Paul et al. (2010). 2 Cross-sectional Married or single subjects nr 1268(70.4%) 704(nr); 442(nr) 9171 (52.8 %) 5586 (33.8 %); 70.3 (8.7) MMSE Global cognition differences in working memory or executive functioning. Widow(er)s did not have higher rates of cognitive impairment than nonbereaved group (single or married). Widows and widowers had worse memory performance than married counterparts. When widowhood duration was No
Perkins et al. (2016) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects 0–10+ years 3585 (82.5 %) 64.9 % WR Memory accounted for, men bereaved for 5—9 years had lower memory ability than married men. Women bereaved for 0—4 years or 10+ years had lower memory ability than married women. Being widowed was not Yes
Ribeiro et al. (2013) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 683 (70.9 %) 284 (nr); 279 (nr) 211 (85.3 %) 84(nr); 78.24 (6.95) Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia associated with greater odds of dementia than being married. Being widowed was not associated with cognitive Yes
Rosnick et al (2010). 3 Cross-sectional Nonbereaved subjects 6 months 127(nr) 69.97 (6.33) SR, disc positioning on a board, BNT, WAIS-R Similarities subtest, copying objects, overall cognitive score (summary statistic of all assessments) Episodic and spatial memory, visuospatial skills, verbal ability, reasoning outcomes. Bereaved men did worse than nonbereaved men on immediate story recall task. Bereaved younger adults did worse on immediate and delayed story recall compared to nonbereaved younger adults. Yes
Saha et al. (2010) 1 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 179 (100 %) nr; nr 64 (7.6) MMSE (cognitive impairment<24) Global cognition Being widowed was associated with cognitive impairment No
Shahar et al. (2001) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects 2.9 months 126 (82 %) 58 (82 %); 58 (82 %) 6766 (51.01%) 77.6 (nr) Combined score: MMSE, digit symbol substitution Global cognition No significant difference in cognitive functioning between widowed and married subjects. Widow(er)s had lower global No
Shin et al. (2018) 3 Longitudinal; 17 0-16 years Married subjects 4040 (nr); 2726 (65.1 %) 240 (62.5 %) 72.79(5.46) Total cognition score (WR, mental status summary score) Global cognition cognition than married subjects. Cognition scores also declined significantly as time since spousal loss increased. Being widowed was associated Yes
Subramanian et al. (2021) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 160 (nr); 80 (nr) 1177 (57.4 %) 63.9 (7.1) HMSE (cognitive impairment <26) Global cognition with greater prevalence of cognitive impairment than being married. Being widowed was associated Yes
Sun et al. (2021) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 402(nr); 775(nr) 2,288,489 (51 %) 81.25 (nr)* MCI (clinician) Cognitive impairment with greater risk of severe cognitive impairment Being widowed was associated No
Sundstrom et al. (2016). 3 Longitudinal; 10 Married subjects nr 1,484,754 (48.6 %); 191,401 (79.8 %) 60.5 (7.3) Dementia diagnosis (patient records) Incident dementia with increased odds of dementia for young-old (50—64 yrs) and middle-old (65—74 yrs) Yes
3 Married subjects nr 74.74 (nr)* Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident dementia No
Sundstrom et al.(2014b) 524 (nr) Longitudinal; 8.6 1609 (56.12 %) 926 (nr); 4370 (45.6 %) 3007 (45.1 %); Widow(er)s have an increased risk of dementia compared to married adults.
Vidarsdottir et al. (2014) 3 Longitudinal; 25 Married subjects 0–10+ years 1363 (74.9 %) 537,513 (46.4 %) 76.68 (nr) composite cognition score (CVLT, DSST, Figure comparison test, Stroop, DSB, SWM of CANTAB); dementia diagnosis (clinician), MCI diagnosis (clinician) Memory, processing speed, executive function Widow(er)s did not differ on cognitive performance than married individuals and did not have greater risk of dementia or mild cognitive impairment. Widowed women had lower executive functioning performance than married women but no differences were seen across widowed and married men. Yes
Wandell et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 5.4 Married subjects nr 292,736(36.0 %); 133,627 (74.3 %) 50 (72 %) 25(72 %); 77.1 % Dementia diagnosis (patient records) Incident dementia Widowed men had lower risk of dementia than married men. No differences in dementia risk between widowed and married women. Yes
Ward et al. (2007) NA Cross-sectional Non-bereaved subjects < 18 months 25(72 %) 1269 (42%) 850 (nr); 65–80 WTAR, MMSE, AVLT, Benton VRT, TEA subtests: elevator counting and visual elevator, TMT, RCFT, COWAT, SDMT MMSE, Coding task, CPM, AVLT MMSE Intelligence quotient, global cognitive ability, memory, attention, information processing, visuospatial ability, verbal fluency Widow(er)s performed worse on tests of attention, processing speed, and verbal fluency than nonbereaved group but after depression, stress and anxiety were accounted for, these differences were no longer significant. Yes
Worn et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 20 Married subjects 0–4+ years 419 (64.4 %) 5872 (55.81 %) 3267 (nr); 75.98 (6.60) MMSE, Coding task, CPM, AVLT MMSE, Coding task, CPM, AVLT Global cognition, processing speed, reasoning, memory Bereaved women performed showed decrease in reasoning performance compared to married women; no differences in memory, global cognitive functioning, and processing speed were found. No cognitive differences between bereaved men and nonbereaved men. Widow(er)s were more likely to experience cognitive decline than married subjects. When stratified by gender, widowhood negatively impacted cognitive function in men but did not impact women. Yes
Xiang et al. (2021) 3 Longitudinal; 9 Married subjects nr 2605 (nr) 80.64 (10.32) MMSE Global cognition When widowhood was divided into 5 categories of widowhood duration, men bereaved for 5 years or less, 16—20 years, or 21+ years were more likely to have lower cognitive scores than married men. Women bereaved Yes
Xu et al. (2020) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 1018 (50.20 %); 285 (70.53 %) 5897(51%) 71 (60–94) MMSE Global cognition for 21+ years had worse cognitive function than currently married women. Window(er)s did not differ from married adults on global cognitive functioning. No effect of gender. Yes
Yin et al. (2020) 3 Longitudinal; 5.32 Widowed individuals nr 2448(nr); 3253(nr) 5550 (55.5 %) 81.7 (9.7) MMSE Global cognition, cognitive impairment Married individuals have lower risk of developing cognitive impairment than widowed individuals Widowed men have greater odds Yes
Zhang et al. (2019a) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 940 (61.2 %); 352 (64.8 %) 73.30 (7.56) MMSE, CDR, cognitive impairment (clinician) Incident cognitive impairment no of cognitive impairment than married men. No differences Yes
352 (64.8 %) dementia between widowed and married women
Zhang et al. (2006) 2 Cross-sectional Married subjects nr 34,807 (53.8 %) 25,910 (nr); 56.3 % Dementia diagnosis (clinician) Incident AD or incident vascular dementia Widowed adults had higher prevalence of AD cases and lower prevalence of vascular Yes
Zhang et al. (2019b) 3 Longitudinal; 2 Married subjects nr 1293 (72.2 %o) 40.9 % WR, TICS (orientation and subtraction) Memory, orientation, calculation continually married adults. Newly widowed adults did not differ from continually married adults in episodic memory changes Yes
Zhang et al. (2021) 3 Longitudinal; 16 Married subjects nr 14,788 (55.2 %) 9700 (nr); 2975 (nr) 30,578 (nr) 27,732 (49 %); 65.89 (nr) TICS, proxy assessment of memory and IADLs Global cognition/ dementia category Being widowed was associated with greater odds of dementia in both blacks and whites. Being widowed was negatively associated with global cognition, objective memory, crystallized Yes
Zhao et al., 2021 3 Longitudinal; 18 Married subjects 0–4+ years 2846 (69 %); 67.49 (nr)* Cognitive constructs formed from: WR, backward counting, SS, TICS Global cognition, memory, mental status cognition and fluid cognition. Being widowed was not associated with backward counting, subjective memory, or date naming. Yes

Notes: nr = not reported; NA = not applicable; CE 8 = Eight-item Informant Interview to Differentiate Aging and Dementia; AVLT = Auditory Verbal Learning Test; B-ADL = Bayer-Activities of daily living scale; BNT = Boston Naming Test; CCCE = Cross Cultural Cognitive Examination; CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; CDT = clock drawing test; COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; CVLT = California Verbal Learning Test; DSB = Digit Span Backward; DSF = Digit Span Forward; DSST = Digit Symbol Substitution Test; e-Stroop = emotional counting-Stroop; FOME = Fuld Object Memory Evaluation; FCSRT = Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test; GNG = Go-No-Go Test; HMSE = Hindi Mental State Exam; IADLs = Instrumental Activities of Daily Living; IQCODE = Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly; LMDR = Logical Memory Delayed Recall; MCI = mild cognitive impairment; MMSE = Mini Mental State Exam; RCFT = Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test ; CPM = Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices; TEA = Test of Everyday Attention; TICS = Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; TMT = Trail Making Test; SDMT = Symbol Digit Modalities Test; SS = Serial 7 Test; SR = Story Recall (immediate and delayed); TICS-m = modified TICS; WAIS- R = Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale Revised; WR = Word recall (immediate and/or delayed); VRT = Visual Retention Test ; WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.

a

Reference group is widowed network (mostly widowed individuals) but reported results are based on comparison between widowed network and married couples with diverse networks.

b

Age was divided into two cohorts, 35–60 and 65–85.

c

Value is median age.

d

Age at mid-life/baseline.

*

Indicates estimates calculated by Wu et al. based on information provided by authors.