Skip to main content
. 2022 Apr 1;18(Suppl 3):e13345. doi: 10.1111/mcn.13345

Table 2.

General characteristics of the 12 cross‐sectional, prospective and retrospective studies addressing baby behaviours and infant feeding

No Author (year), country Study design: Infant feeding outcome Participants (n) Baby behaviour related to infant decision
1 Mohebati et al. (2021), Mexico Prospective. Self‐reported milk insufficiency, BF problems, maternal confidence Primiparas mothers with healthy and full‐term infants delivered in a Baby Friendly Hospital in Mexico City, planning to breastfeed and who did not undertake paid work until infants were 6 months (n = 475) ‘Crying frequency’ associated with ↑ lactation problem score (OR = 1.12), expectation of a baby ‘crying more than other babies’ ↑ reports of milk insufficiency at 2–4 weeks (OR = 2.07). Higher ‘crying expectations’ ↓ maternal self‐confidence (ρ = −0.16)
2 Bulut & Alemdar (2021), Turkey Cross‐sectional. BF motivation Mothers of infants 3 weeks to 6 months seeking paediatric outpatient care due to excessive crying and who were BF (n = 210) BF enjoyment associated with ↑ positive thoughts (i.e., needs support, trying to communicate) about ‘infant crying’ (ρ = 0.235)
3 Wood et al. (2017), USA One‐group pre–post test pilot intervention. Self‐reported milk insufficiency Mother–infant dyads followed up postpartum through home visits (n = 15) Counselling home visits associated with ↓ perception of milk insufficiency due to ‘crying’. At 6 days, score attributing milk insufficiency to crying was 2.43 and at 27 days 4.93 (lower score greater attribution)
4 Taut et al. (2016), Ireland Cross‐sectional. BF duration Dyads of normal weight, singleton infants and healthy mothers (n = 5955) ‘Fussiness/difficultness’ associated with ↓ BF duration (OR = 0.98 at <90 days and OR = 0.98 at ≥90 days)a
5 Kronborg et al. (2014), Denmark Cross‐sectional. Early introduction of complementary foodsb Women in urban and rural municipalities who had delivered in the prior 6 months (n = 4503) Only among primipara mothers, perception of infant as ‘not temperamental’ was associated with ↑ odds for introduction of complementary foods after week 25 (OR = 1.77)
6 Keemer (2013), Australia Cross‐sectional. BF confidence BF women, singleton, healthy term infant (n = 128) 40% of women using second‐line strategies (cup, syringe, bottles, nipples shields) due to ‘unsettled infant’. Using second‐line strategies associated with ↓ BF self‐efficacy (15‐points lower score)
7 Wasser et al. (2011), USA Cross‐sectional.c Early introduction of complementary foods Primipara mother–infant dyads enroled in the Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants and Children (n = 217) Infants perceived to have higher score on ‘temperament scale’ associated with ↑ likelihood to be fed complementary foods at 3 months (OR = 1.97 distress to limitaion, OR = 1.75 activity level)
8 Karaçam (2008), Turkey Cross‐sectional. Early introduction of complementary foods Mothers getting services in primary healthcare facilities with babies 0–4 months (n = 514) ‘Frequent crying’ associated with ↑ use of complementary foods (OR = 1.687)
9 Howard et al. (2006), USA Prospective cohort. BF duration Mother–infant dyads at a university hospital (n = 700) ‘Physician‐diagnosed colic’ associated with ↓ duration of breast milk as predominant source of nutrition (hazard ratio = 2.43).
10 Vandiver (1997), USA Prospective cohort. BF duration Primiparas married women with healthy newborns (n = 50) Easier ‘infant temperament’ associated with ↑ BF duration (weaned their infants after 12 weeks, F = 4.67)
11 Loughlin et al. (1985), USA Prospective. BF discontinuation Healthy newborns who were initially breastfed (n = 94) At 8 weeks, 30% of the mothers not BF: rating of nursery staff of infant's ‘excessive crying’ and infant's ‘demanding personality’, associated with BF cessation
12 Forsyth et al. (1985), USA Cross‐sectional. Early introduction of formula and formula changes Mothers of BF and FF singleton newborns (BF n = 189, FF n = 184) ‘Excessive crying and colic’ associated with ↑ introduction of a specialty formula (11% of the BF and 25% of the FF infants given special formulas), mothers believed cause of the problem was intrinsic child (disease, allergy).

Abbreviations: BE, breastfeeding; FF, formula‐fed; OR, odds ratio.

a

Data comes from a cohort study that included babies up to 9 months. However, in this particular study, there is a cutoff point showing that fussy babies at 90 days are more likely to have shorter BF duration.

b

Although early introduction of complementary foods (i.e., before age 4 months) is not a direct outcome of interest in the review, it is an indirect outcome of BF duration.

c

Even though the study was collected prospectively, this study only uses cross‐sectional data from the 3‐month visit.