Summary
Singing ability is a complex human skill influenced by genetic and environmental factors, the relative contributions of which remain unknown. Currently, genetically informative studies using objective measures of singing ability across a range of tasks are limited. We administered a validated online singing tool to measure performance across three everyday singing tasks in Australian twins (n = 1189) to explore the relative genetic and environmental influences on singing ability. We derived a reproducible phenotypic index for singing ability across five performance measures of pitch and interval accuracy. Using this index we found moderate heritability of singing ability (h2 = 40.7%) with a striking, similar contribution from shared environmental factors (c2 = 37.1%). Childhood singing in the family home and being surrounded by music early in life both significantly predicted the phenotypic index. Taken together, these findings show that singing ability is equally influenced by genetic and shared environmental factors.
Subject areas: quantitative genetics, anthropology
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
We measured singing ability in a large sample of Australian twins
-
•
Singing ability is moderately heritable
-
•
Shared environmental factors are equally important
-
•
Of these, early but not current musical environments shaped singing ability
Quantitative genetics; Anthropology
Introduction
Singing is central to being human, maintaining our emotional wellbeing and sociocultural identity. There is a large amount of variability in musical talent and proficiency, ranging from people with congenital amusia to prodigies. For decades, the model of 10,000 h of deliberate practice has been the accepted explanation for the acquisition and maintenance of music expertise (Ericsson et al., 1993). This model rejects the role of “innate” predispositions on expert development (Ericsson et al., 1993). However, recent work indicates that deliberate practice likely accounts for only 30% of the variance in expert musicianship, implicating other factors (Hambrick et al., 2018; Macnamara et al., 2014). Research has now turned to exploring the behavioral and molecular genetic bases of music ability to assess the unique and interactive effects of genes and environment.
Singing ability is a complex behavioral trait that requires robust, objective phenotyping to establish its genetic basis (Gingras et al., 2015). Singing provides an ideal phenotype for exploring the genetic basis of musical talent as it exists in all cultures, has universal features (Jacoby et al., 2019; Mehr et al., 2019), and emerges early and spontaneously during human development (Welch, 2015). Phenotyping singing ability, however, is not a trivial issue. As singing is a complex behavioral skill involving the coordination of perceptual and productive processes, it is likely influenced by many genetic variants of small effect. Arguably, these could differentially contribute to pitch accuracy, rhythm, timbre, vocal production, lyrical expression, and harmonization with others. The challenge then is to identify the core phenotypic components of singing and their genetic variants.
Limited molecular evidence has identified genetic variants related to music aptitude in humans that are also linked to song learning in songbirds, suggesting shared evolutionary origins for human singing (Järvelä, 2018; Kanduri et al., 2015). The occurrence of untrained individuals with high singing pitch accuracy supports an innate predisposition (Larrouy-Maestri et al., 2013; Watts et al., 2003; Wilson et al., 2011). To date, Park et al. (2012) is the only study to explore the genetic basis of singing ability using an objective measure. They used a simple vocal pitch-matching task in an isolated Mongolian sample showing that vocal pitch-matching is moderately heritable (h2 = 40%) and linked to UGT8. However, their isolated homogeneous sample limited the investigation of the role of environmental influences, and their use of a simple pitch-matching task did not sufficiently capture the range of skills required for everyday singing. Two additional studies (Coon and Carey, 1989; Morley et al., 2012) have also investigated the genetic basis of participating in singing, which may not be related to objective singing ability. There is a critical need to expand on previous work using more robust and comprehensive measures that capture the complexity of singing phenotypes.
Here, we administered a recently validated online singing tool, the Melbourne Singing Tool (Tan et al., 2021), in a large sample of Australian twins (n = 1189) to objectively measure singing pitch accuracy. We hypothesized that singing accuracy would show evidence of heritability. As a secondary aim, we explored the relationship between singing pitch accuracy performance and environmental variables such as early and current experiences of singing with family.
Results
Objective performance on singing tasks
The Melbourne Singing Tool included three main singing tasks: vocal pitch-matching (Single Note task), singing “Happy Birthday” (Familiar Tune task), and singing novel melodies (Unfamiliar Tune task; see Table 1 and STAR Methods). These tasks were designed to capture “everyday” singing behaviors that the general population typically engages in irrespective of professional singing training. They were also chosen for comparability with tools used in previous studies (Berkowska and Dalla Bella, 2013; Pfordresher and Demorest, 2020).
Table 1.
Summary of the three singing tasks in the Melbourne Singing Tool
Task | Task requirements | No. trials | Frequency range of stimuli | Variable extracted |
---|---|---|---|---|
Single Note | Sing back 5 sine tones Pitch classes B, C#, D#, F, G |
10 | Female: 246.94–397Hz Male: 123.47–196Hz |
PD |
Familiar Tune | Sing Happy Birthday in different conditions (paced/unpaced, with/without lyrics). The paced condition was used in the current study, sung first with the lyrics and then twice on the syllable “dah” after listening to a drum beat of Happy Birthday at 120bpma |
3 | Participants choose their preferred starting note | PD ID |
Unfamiliar Tune | Sing back 5 different 7-note piano tunes | 10 | Female: 220–440 Hz Male: 110-220Hz |
PD ID |
PD, pitch deviation; ID, interval deviation.
The average of trials two and three was used to measure performance as evidence suggests that singing with a syllable produces a more accurate performance than singing with lyrics (Berkowska and Dalla Bella, 2013; Dalla Bella et al., 2007; Tan et al., 2021).
The tasks yielded five measures of singing pitch accuracy for each participant – three for mean absolute pitch deviation (PD) and two for mean interval deviation (ID) measured in cents (see STAR Methods). Pitch deviation reflects the average difference between the frequency of a sung note and a target note for each task. Interval deviation reflects the average difference between a sung interval (the space between two notes in a tune) and a target interval for the tune tasks, thereby capturing singing accuracy over time. For each of the raw measures, lower deviation values reflect greater accuracy to the target pitch/interval and therefore higher performance.
Performance on all five measures was heavily skewed toward lower deviation values. Median performance on each measure fell below 100 cents (Figure 1A), a deviation of less than one semitone, and a common cut-off score for accurate singing (Dalla Bella and Berkowska, 2009; Dalla Bella et al., 2007). For the tune tasks, the medians for the PD and ID measures were similar for each task. The lowest median PD performance was observed for the Single Note task likely reflecting its simplicity, whereas the Familiar Tune task showed the least variance likely owing to its high familiarity. In general, however, performance across the tasks was similar (Table 2).
Figure 1.
The five singing measures used to construct the singing ability phenotypic index (n = 1189)
(A) Boxplots showing accuracy in cents on the five singing measures, presented on a log-transformed scale. Lower scores represent greater accuracy. The blue dotted line marks a deviation of 100 cents (one semitone).
(B) Loadings of the five singing measures on the single singing performance factor. PD = pitch deviation; ID = interval deviation.
Table 2.
Descriptive statistics for the five singing measures in cents in the twin sample (n = 1189)
Singing measure | M | SD | Participants scoring <100 cents | Median |
---|---|---|---|---|
Single Note PD | 96.99 | 118.47 | 69.0% | 32.92 |
Familiar Tune PD | 81.35 | 77.42 | 73.4% | 51.83 |
Familiar Tune ID | 61.91 | 46.80 | 86.0% | 47.65 |
Unfamiliar Tune PD | 114.66 | 119.31 | 64.1% | 63.97 |
Unfamiliar Tune ID | 108.70 | 90.59 | 58.5% | 77.21 |
PD, pitch deviation; ID, interval deviation.
A singing ability phenotypic index
To explore possible latent factors that capture singing ability across tasks we conducted an exploratory factor analysis with principal axis factoring on the five measures. First, we applied a natural log transformation to each measure to minimize the skew from its distribution. For the ease of interpretation of results, we then reverse-coded the log-transformed values so that higher values reflect better singing performance on each measure. The five measures showed high internal reliability (α = 0.92), with factor analysis indicating a single factor provided the best solution. All five measures loaded strongly on this factor (Figure 1B) explaining 75.3% of the variance in singing performance. Factor scores for this latent variable were calculated using Thurstone’s regression method and are henceforth referred to as the singing factor score. Of note, the factor score correlated highly with the average of the five log-transformed measures (r = 0.986, 95% confidence interval (CI:) [0.984, 0.987]). The high internal reliability and strong loadings of this factor suggest that it is a robust phenotypic index, capturing the complexity of everyday singing ability.
Heritability of singing ability
To examine whether individual differences in singing ability are influenced by genetic factors, we first calculated twin correlations on the singing factor score, split by zygosity and adjusted for age and sex during modeling (Table 3). This showed similarly high twin correlations for male and female monozygotic (MZ) twins compared to consistently lower twin correlations for dizygotic (DZ) twins, with assumption tests showing no significant sex differences (p > 0.01; Table 4). Modeling the combined data we found MZ twins had a higher twin correlation (rMZ = 0.78, 95% CI: [0.73, 0.81]) than DZ twins (rDZ = 0.58, 95% CI: [0.46, 0.68]) with nonoverlapping CIs (Figure 2).
Table 3.
Twin correlations for the singing factor score by zygosity (n = 517 twin pairs)
Zygosity group | N pairs (%a) | Twin correlation [95% CI] |
---|---|---|
MZ male | 98 (26%) | 0.75 [0.65, 0.83] |
MZ female | 279 (74%) | 0.78 [0.73, 0.83] |
DZ male | 30 (23%) | 0.50 [0.18, 0.72] |
DZ female | 99 (77%) | 0.60 [0.46, 0.71] |
DZ opposite sex | 11 | 0.43 [-0.15, 0.79] |
MZ, monozygotic; DZ, dizygotic.
% for same sex zygotic twin pairs.
Table 4.
Model-fitting statistics for the assumption testing of quantitative and qualitative sex differences
Model | Base model | Comparison model | Estimated parameters | -2LL | AIC | df | Change in -2LL | Change in df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Saturated model | 27 | 2,466.51 | 452.51 | 1007 | NA | NA | NA | |
2 | Saturated model | Means equated across twin order | 23 | 2,467.38 | 445.38 | 1011 | 0.864 | 4 | 0.930 |
3 | Means equated across twin order | Means equated across zygosity | 21 | 2,468.61 | 442.61 | 1013 | 1.231 | 2 | 0.541 |
4 | Means equated across zygosity | Means across same sex | 19 | 2,476.11 | 446.11 | 1015 | 7.501 | 2 | 0.024 |
5 | Means across same sex | Means across both sexes | 18 | 2,476.11 | 444.11 | 1016 | <0.001 | 1 | 1 |
6 | Means across both sexes | Variances equated across twin order | 14 | 2,477.62 | 437.62 | 1020 | 1.514 | 4 | 0.824 |
7 | Variances equated across twin order | Variances equated across zygosity | 12 | 2,477.64 | 433.64 | 1022 | 0.02 | 2 | 0.990 |
8 | Variances equated across zygosity | Variances equated across same sex | 10 | 2,478.30 | 430.30 | 1024 | 0.656 | 2 | 0.720 |
9 | Variances equated across same sex | Variances equated across both sexes | 9 | 2,480.41 | 430.41 | 1025 | 2.111 | 1 | 0.146 |
10 | Variances equated across both sexes | Covariances equated between MZ male and female | 8 | 2,480.41 | 428.41 | 1026 | 0.004 | 1 | 0.951 |
11 | Covariances equated between MZ male and female | Covariances equated between DZ male and female | 7 | 2,480.64 | 426.64 | 1027 | 0.229 | 1 | 0.632 |
12 | Covariances equated between DZ male and female | Covariances equated across DZ opposite sex | 6 | 2,480.67 | 424.67 | 1028 | 0.028 | 1 | 0.868 |
Alpha criterion is set at p < 0.01. -2LL = minus two log-likelihood, AIC = Akaike Information Criterion, df = degrees of freedom. A lower AIC represents a better model fit. A significant p-value (p < 0.01) indicates a significantly worse model fit.
Figure 2.
Intra-twin correlations between monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins on the singing factor score (n = 1189).
The twin correlation for MZ twins was less than double that of DZ twins, suggesting that shared familial factors (c2) rather than dominant genetic factors play a role. We, therefore, used structural equation modeling to examine the influence of shared environmental effects, fitting a univariate ACE model over an ADE model (Grasby et al., 2017). Supporting our main hypothesis, singing ability showed a moderate level of heritability (h2 = 40.7%, 95% CI: [22.5%, 63.4%]). Strikingly, we found a similar level of influence of shared environmental factors (c2 = 37.1%, 95% CI: [14.7%, 54.7%]). In contrast, unshared environmental factors accounted for only 22.2% (95% CI: [18.8%, 26.3%]) of the phenotypic variance in singing ability. To assess whether these estimates were significant, we fitted a series of sub-models without the A, C, or E path (Table 5). All sub-models led to a significantly worse fit compared to the full ACE model (p < 0.01), and so all paths were retained.
Table 5.
Model-fitting statistics for the univariate ACE model, comparing a full ACE model against various constrained submodels
Base model | Comparison model | Estimated parameters | -2LL | AIC | df | Change in -2LL | Change in df | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ACE | 6 | 2,480.670 | 424.670 | 1028 | ||||
ACE | AE | 5 | 2,489.763 | 431.763 | 1029 | 9.093 | 1 | 0.003 |
ACE | CE | 5 | 2,505.025 | 447.025 | 1029 | 24.356 | 1 | <0.001 |
ACE | E | 4 | 2,886.138 | 826.138 | 1030 | 405.469 | 2 | <0.001 |
A, additive genetic effects; C, shared environmental effects; E, non-shared environmental effects; -2LL, minus two log-likelihood; AIC, Akaike Information Criterion; df, degrees of freedom.
ACE represents the full model, while AE and CE are submodels that drop the C and A paths, respectively.
A lower AIC represents a better model fit. A significant p-value indicates a significantly worse model fit.
The importance of early shared musical environments
In view of the strong evidence for the role of shared environmental factors in singing ability, we regressed the singing factor score against three self-report measures of childhood and current shared musical experiences from the Melbourne Singing Tool (n = 1174; see STAR Methods). We chose measures that likely captured early environmental influences that twin pairs would experience together, as well as a related measure of current shared environmental influence. These included early experiences of singing with family and being surrounded by music in childhood, as well as current singing with family. We included age and sex as covariates in our analysis.
Singing with family and being surrounded by music in childhood predicted a higher singing factor score, and therefore more accurate performance (p < 0.001). In contrast, current singing with family did not predict singing performance (p = 0.852). This model explained 26.5% of the variance, and age was a significant negative covariate (p < 0.001; Table 6).
Table 6.
Multiple regression showing the predictive effect of the shared environmental measures on the singing factor score, adjusted for age and sex (n = 1174)
Variable | B | β | SE | t | P |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Singing with family (childhood) | 0.269 | 0.322 | 0.028 | 9.790 | <0.001 |
Being surrounded by music (childhood) | 0.205 | 0.228 | 0.027 | 7.669 | <0.001 |
Singing with family (current) | 0.005 | 0.006 | 0.026 | 0.186 | 0.852 |
Age | −0.005 | −0.087 | 0.002 | −3.328 | <0.001 |
Sex | −0.067 | −0.030 | 0.057 | −1.180 | 0.238 |
B, unstandardized beta coefficient; β, standardized beta coefficient; SE, standard error.
Discussion
Using a well-validated multi-platform online singing tool, we derived a phenotypic index of singing ability that objectively quantitates pitch accuracy for a range of everyday singing tasks. Importantly, we showed that this phenotypic index is heritable, at a level consistent with previous behavioral studies of >17,000 human traits (Polderman et al., 2015) including music perception and production (Drayna et al., 2001; Mosing et al., 2014b; Ullén et al., 2014). Strikingly, we also demonstrated an equal impact of the shared environment on this phenotypic index, which was more than double the average effect reported in previous behavioral genetics studies (Polderman et al., 2015). Further analyses provided clues for the type of environmental experiences that may shape singing ability, including early singing with family and being surrounded by music in childhood. These findings point to a combined equal effect of genetic and environmental influences in determining an individual’s objective singing performance.
We used tasks that the general population routinely engage in, such as singing well-known tunes on special occasions and learning new tunes through imitation. Our sample’s performance on each of the five measures derived from the Melbourne Singing Tool is commensurate with the general population as reported in behavioral singing research (Dalla Bella and Berkowska, 2009; Pfordresher and Brown, 2007; Pfordresher et al., 2010; Pfordresher and Demorest, 2021; Tan et al., 2021). Although there was substantial variation in performance between individuals, the distribution of singing accuracy on each of the measures was skewed toward accurate performance. Most participants demonstrated a moderate level of accuracy, with the majority singing target pitches or pitch intervals, on average within one semitone (Dalla Bella and Berkowska, 2009; Pfordresher and Brown, 2007; Pfordresher et al., 2010; Pfordresher and Demorest, 2021). This is broadly consistent with previous studies showing that the general population tend to sing accurately (Dalla Bella and Berkowska, 2009; Dalla Bella et al., 2007; Pfordresher and Brown, 2007; Pfordresher et al., 2010; Pfordresher and Demorest, 2021). Our findings were also consistent with recent findings showing a negative relationship between age and accuracy in singing tasks (Pfordresher and Demorest, 2021). Our study shows that objectively measured singing ability can be captured by a strong single factor, allowing us to construct a phenotypic index of everyday singing.
For the first time, we showed that singing ability is heritable to a degree consistent with music genetics and behavioral genetics research (Butkovic et al., 2015; Hambrick and Tucker-Drob, 2015; Mosing et al., 2014b; Polderman et al., 2015; Ullén et al., 2014). In a recent meta-analysis of behavioral genetics studies spanning 50 years, Polderman et al. (2015) showed that the average heritability of human traits is approximately 49%. For musical traits, perceptual processes such as pitch perception (h2 = 38–51%) (Mosing et al., 2015; Seesjärvi et al., 2016; Ullén et al., 2014), and tune recognition (h2 = 59–80%) (Drayna et al., 2001; Mosing et al., 2014b; Ullén et al., 2014) are also moderately heritable, along with the propensity to practice (h2 = 38–70%) (Butkovic et al., 2015; Hambrick and Tucker-Drob, 2015; Mosing et al., 2014a) and attain music achievements (h2 = 55–57% in males, 9–13% in females) (Mosing et al., 2015; Wesseldijk et al., 2019). Notably, our heritability estimate is consistent with the sole previous study using vocal pitch-matching that reported 40% heritability (Park et al., 2012). To our knowledge, our study is the first to establish the heritability of singing ability using everyday singing tasks such as singing familiar and unfamiliar tunes, adding to the growing body of evidence suggesting that, like many other human traits, musical traits have a genetic basis (Polderman et al., 2015).
A large, shared environment effect is atypical in behavioral genetics research. Many human traits are only weakly influenced by shared environment, if at all (Polderman et al., 2015), with Polderman et al. (2015) finding an average effect of just 17.4%. Our estimate of 37.1% is more than double the average estimates observed in behavioral genetics studies for related phenotypes in cognitive (17.7%) and psychiatric (15.8%) domains, as well as those for social interactions (18.2%) and social values (27.1%) (Polderman et al., 2015). Arguably, this large effect may be partially explained by the importance of early childhood singing and immersion in music for both MZ and DZ twins. It is consistent with evidence for early sensitive periods of music development, where an enriched musical environment is thought to foster neurodevelopmental change supporting music processing (Steele et al., 2013; Vaquero et al., 2016) and the acquisition of musical skills (Hannon and Trehub, 2005; Merrett et al., 2013; Wilson et al., 2012). Evidence also supports a sensitive period for sensorimotor integration, a crucial process that links perception and action for music and singing production (Bailey and Penhune, 2012; Steele et al., 2013) including vocal pitch-matching (Hutchins et al., 2014; Pfordresher and Brown, 2007).
Importantly, Wesseldijk et al. (2019) showed that measures of a musically-enriched childhood correlate with achievements in music and that heritability of musical achievement increases with higher mean levels of musical enrichment. Relatedly, Hambrick and Tucker-Drob (2015) found that the heritability of accomplishment was greater in twins who engaged in regular practice, which in turn has a heritable component (Butkovic et al., 2015; Hambrick and Tucker-Drob, 2015; Mosing et al., 2014a). In the case of pitch-accurate singing, our findings suggest that a genetic predisposition may be more likely expressed when an individual is exposed to an early enriched singing and musical environment, potentially leading to changes in the neural networks underpinning singing (Wilson et al., 2011) that may occur through interactions between genes and early environments. These gene-environment interactions take place when children with genetic predispositions are actively or passively exposed to environments that foster these predispositions. An interaction between genetic and shared familial influences would inflate the estimate of C, and so may explain our high C value (Grasby et al., 2017).
To reliably establish the genetic and neurobiological bases of music-related traits, objective phenotypes need to be established (Gingras et al., 2015). Our findings provide an important first step toward this endeavor for everyday singing ability. Although genes such as UGT8 (Park et al., 2012) and SLC6A4 (Morley et al., 2012) have been implicated in previous studies of vocal pitch-matching and choral participation, respectively, to date no studies have explored genetic variants of more complex objective phenotypes of singing ability. Recent findings have shown shared genes between song learning in songbirds and transcriptome regulation in humans after musical performance and listening, providing potential variants of interest underlying these complex phenotypes (Kanduri et al., 2015; Nair et al., 2019, 2020; Oikkonen et al., 2016).
In conclusion, we demonstrate an objective phenotypic index of singing ability that captures performance on a range of everyday singing tasks. This phenotype is moderately heritable, suggesting a genetic basis. However, shared environmental factors equally influence this singing ability phenotype. Our findings highlight the need for further investigation of these heritable and shared environmental effects, including the interplay between our genetic predispositions and the environments in which we grow up.
Limitations of the study
There are some limitations to our study. First, the measures of early shared environment that we used in our exploratory analyses were based on self-report. Although they provided an initial indication of shared environmental factors associated with singing ability, they warrant more detailed investigation using prospective longitudinal studies. These measures themselves may also be influenced by genetic factors, and so multivariate modeling may help unpick these relationships. Second, our study did not directly measure the rhythmic accuracy of singing, although our choice of singing tasks captured various rhythms. Our five measures showed similar pitch and interval accuracy scores despite this rhythmic variation and all loaded strongly onto one factor, suggesting that rhythmic ability was not a major factor in our phenotype. Third, self-selection bias may have occurred in our recruitment as people already interested in music and/or singing may have been more likely to participate. Research has clearly described the cultural and social barriers for Australian boys and men to engage in singing activities (Harrison, 2007; Powell, 2014). In addition, Twins Research Australia, who assisted with recruitment (see STAR Methods), has a higher number of females in its database (Murphy et al., 2019). Both these points likely account for our higher female participation rate. Future investigations in samples with lower rates of musical training, and higher proportions of men, would be valuable. However, given that the performance of our sample is commensurate with that reported in the general population (Berkowska and Dalla Bella, 2013; Dalla Bella and Berkowska, 2009; Dalla Bella et al., 2007) it is unlikely this affected our results.
STAR★Methods
Key resources table
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Software and algorithms | ||
IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 27) | https://www.ibm.com/products/spss-statistics | RRID:SCR_016479 |
OpenMX (version 2.19.1) | https://openmx.ssri.psu.edu/; https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/OpenMx/index.html | NA |
TONY | https://code.soundsoftware.ac.uk/projects/tony | NA |
R (version 4.0.4) | http://www.r-project.org/ | RRID:SCR_001905 |
RStudio | https://www.rstudio.com/ | RRID:SCR_000432 |
psych (version 2.1.3) | https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/psych/index.html | RRID:SCR_021744 |
polycor | https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/polycor/index.html | NA |
Original code for analyses and figures | This paper | https://doi.org/10.26188/621c4f3d17610 |
Melbourne Singing Tool | Tan, Y. T., Peretz, I., McPherson, G. E., & Wilson, S. J. (2021). Estabilishing the reliability and validity of web-based singing research.Music Perception, 38(4), 386-405. | https://doi.org/10.1525/MP.2021.38.4.386 |
Resource availability
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contact, Daniel Yeom (yeom.d@unimelb.edu.au).
Materials availability
This study did not generate new unique reagents.
Experimental model and subject details
Participants
1273 twins from across Australia were recruited for this study. 964 participants (75.7%) were recruited with assistance from Twins Research Australia (TRA), a nationwide twin registry that supports twin studies. Twins from the wider community were also recruited through social media, advertisements around the University of Melbourne and by word-of-mouth. The recruitment material specified that vocal ability was not a prerequisite to participate in the study to reduce the likelihood of self-selection from participants who may already have an interest in singing. As an incentive for participation, twin pairs that had both twins complete the entire study went into two successive prize draws; first for an iPad mini, and second for gift cards equivalent to AUD$1000. This was done to reduce self-selection bias. Participants were informed that by taking part in the study, they were providing informed consent. Ethics was approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of the University of Melbourne.
84 participants (6.6% of the original sample) were removed from the analyses due to missing values on at least one of the five singing measures. This meant they were not included in the calculation of the factor score, and therefore the heritability analyses. The final sample contained 1189 individuals (875 females). There were 377 MZ (279 female, 98 male) and 140 DZ (99 female, 30 male, 11 opposite-sex) twin pairs, as well as 155 individual cases without their co-twin. The individual cases were retained for the univariate genetic analyses because they contribute to the estimation of means, variances and covariances.
The mean age of the final sample was 43.4 years (SD = 16.5) with a range of 15–90 years. On average, participants had 15.0 years of education (SD = 2.5, range = 8–18). 113 participants (9.5%) reported voice as their primary instrument, with a median of 5 years of training (1–3 years = 37.2%, 4–6 years = 28.3%, >6 years = 31.9%, missing = 2.7%). 755 (63.5%) reported a primary instrument other than voice, with a median of 5 years of training (1–3 years = 34.7%, 4–6 years = 30.1%, >6 years = 34.2%; missing = 1.1%). There was no significant difference in years of training between MZ and DZ twins (F(1, 1121) = 0.727, p = 0.394). A full breakdown of instrument categories in the sample is available in SI Appendix, Table S1.
Method details
The Melbourne Singing Tool
Data for this study were collected using a purpose-built online singing tool, described in Tan et al. (2021). The tool is written in HTML5, ensuring compatibility with most modern smartphones, tablets, and computers, as well as most modern operating systems. Participants used their own devices to complete the study online and were asked to undertake the tasks in a quiet place to minimise background disruptions. Completion of the singing tool took 20 min on average and all singing recordings were sent to a secure server.
The Melbourne Singing Tool has been found to have high internal consistency, convergent validity, and test-retest reliability across an average 4.5 year period (Tan et al., 2021). In addition, similar performance of the singing tasks was shown by web-based participants to those who completed the tool in a controlled laboratory environment (Tan et al., 2021). In the current sample, 271 participants completed a pilot version of the singing tool, with 78 of these completing both the pilot and current versions, with singing performance highly correlated across the two versions (SI Appendix, Table S2).
The Melbourne Singing Tool first requires participants to provide informed consent, test their audio and recording set-up, and complete a demographic questionnaire. This is followed by two music perception tasks and the three main singing tasks used in this study, presented in the appropriate vocal range for the reported sex of the participant (Table 1). Participants then complete a series of self-report questionnaires about their early and current musical experiences and training. From the childhood and current environmental variables, three questions were chosen to reflect singing engagement most likely involving shared environmental experiences: (1) “I sang by myself [in childhood]” (1 = Never, 5 = Always), (2) “I sang with family [in childhood]” (1 = Never, 5 = Always) and (3) I sing with my family [in my current everyday life] (1 = Never, 5 = Always). To avoid practice effects, pilot data were used for the 78 participants who completed both versions of the task.
Singing variables
The singing data was processed using a modified version of the open-source TONY software (Mauch et al., 2015; Tan et al., 2021). TONY automatically identifies stable pitch segments and provides an initial estimate of each note’s median fundamental frequency, fi (Mauch et al., 2015). Each participant’s singing data was then inspected both visually and aurally by a member of the research team using TONY’s graphical interface and manually adjusted to account for incorrectly identified notes, including octave errors, where necessary. Our previous work has shown that this method of supplementing TONY’s automatic segmentation with manual inspection correlates highly with purely manual pitch segmentation, thereby demonstrating its robustness and reliability (Tan et al., 2021). Five measures were extracted from the audio: Single Note PD, Familiar Tune PD and ID and Unfamiliar Tune PD and ID.
For the Single Note task, we calculated absolute PD as the absolute deviation from the target pitch in cents. To account for differences in duration of sung notes, we calculated PD using a 2-s segment from the onset of the note. If participants did not hold a note for 2 s, PD was calculated using the entire available segment. This was averaged over all trials, using the following equation:
where fi is the fundamental frequency (Hz) of the ith pitch produced by the participant, ri is the fundamental frequency of the target pitch and n is the number of trials. For octave errors the following adjustment was used to prevent unfairly penalising participants:
where fi is the fundamental frequency (Hz) of the ith pitch produced by the participant and PDoct is the octave-adjusted PD. If TONY was unable to extract an estimate for fi from a trial (either due to a blank audio recording or poor audio quality) this was treated as missing data, and subsequently excluded from calculations of the average PD for this task.
For the Unfamiliar Tune task, in addition to PD we calculated ID in terms of the absolute difference between each sung interval and its expected target interval in the tune using the following equation:
where fi is the fundamental frequency (Hz) of the ith pitch produced by the participant, fi-1 is the fundamental frequency of the note preceding the ith pitch, ri is the fundamental frequency of the target pitch, ri-1 is the fundamental frequency of the note preceding the target pitch and n is the number of notes in the task. Octave adjustments were applied as per the Single Note task. Where participants were missing notes audio files were inspected and the location of the missing note was determined from the contour of the sung tune. ID was then calculated between the ith pitch and the previous non-missing note.
For the Familiar Tune task, since participants chose their preferred starting note a reference pitch was first calculated for each trial before then applying the above process for calculating PD and ID. A stable reference pitch for each trial was estimated from the sung frequencies of the first, second and fourth notes of the first phrase of Happy Birthday (which share the same pitch), using the following equation:
where ri represents the fundamental reference frequency of the ith note in the tune, ri-1 is the fundamental reference frequency of the preceding note and int refers to the expected interval in cents between the two notes. Octave adjustments were not required as all expected pitch values were derived from the participant’s starting note.
Quantification and statistical analysis
Analyses of the raw singing data were conducted in R (version 4.0.4) (R Core Team, 2020) and the factor analysis was performed using SPSS version 27. Both the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin test of sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity suggested that the data was adequate for factor analysis (KMO = 0.79; χ2 = 6841.56, p < 0.001). To account for skew, each raw accuracy measure was first log-transformed. These values were then reverse-coded by multiplying by −1 and adding 7, with higher values representing higher accuracy on the singing tasks. Principal axis factoring was used as the extraction method, with no rotation since only one factor was extracted. The number of factors to retain was based on Kaiser’s criterion (eigenvalues >1) and inspection of the scree plot. Factor scores for the single latent factor were calculated using a Thurstone regression method (Thurstone, 1935).
All genetic analyses were conducted in R using the OpenMx (version 2.19.1) (Boker et al., 2011; Neale et al., 2016), psych (version 2.1.3) (Revelle, 2019) and polycor (version 0.7–10) (Fox and Dusa, 2019) packages. Assumption tests for quantitative and qualitative differences between sexes and zygosities were performed with an alpha criterion of p = 0.01. First, a saturated model was fitted to the data to estimate all parameters, allowing for means, variances and covariances to differ between first and second born twins, sexes and zygosities (i.e. Five groups – MZ female, MZ male, DZ female, DZ male and DZ opposite sex pairs). This full model was then compared against various constrained submodels. Means were first equated across twin order, and then by zygosity and sex. Variances were equated in the same order. Lastly, covariances were equated across sexes and then across zygosity. Model fitting statistics for all assumption tests are shown in Table 4. We then fitted a univariate ACE model with the singing factor score as the variable of interest, and age and sex as covariates (Table 5). Since assumption testing indicated that there were no sex differences, sex was not modeled separately.
To explore the potential contribution of shared family environment on singing ability, an exploratory multiple regression analysis was conducted in R, with age and sex included as covariates. Listwise deletion was used to remove participants with incomplete data on all predictors.
Acknowledgments
We thank all the twins for their participation in the study. We thank Dr. Laura Wesseldjik for reviewing the R code and outputs for the univariate genetic analysis, and for her valuable input on the analyses. We also thank Mark Solly and Oscar Correa for their work in developing the Melbourne Singing Tool. This research was supported by an Australian Research Council Discovery Project Grant (DP170102479) and an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship. This research was facilitated through access to Twins Research Australia, a national resource supported by a Centre of Research Excellence Grant (ID: 1079102), from the National Health and Medical Research Council. The graphical abstract was created with BioRender (www.biorender.com).
Author contributions
S.J.W., I.P., Y.T.T., G.E.M., and S.F.B. designed research; Y.T.T. and T.F. performed research; D.Y., S.J.W., N.H., V.M.Z.Y., and M.M. analyzed data; and D.Y., S.J.W., N.H., Y.T.T., G.E.M., I.P, M.M., M.S.H., S.F.B., and V.M.Z.Y. wrote the article.
Declaration of interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Inclusion and diversity
We worked to ensure gender balance in the recruitment of human subjects. We worked to ensure ethnic or other types of diversity in the recruitment of human subjects. We worked to ensure that the study questionnaires were prepared in an inclusive way.
Published: June 17, 2022
Footnotes
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2022.104360.
Supplemental information
Data and code availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the lead contact upon reasonable request, and with the permission of Twins Research Australia. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
All original code has been deposited at Figshare and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table. The standard version of TONY is freely available at https://code.soundsoftware.ac.uk/projects/tony.
References
- Bailey J., Penhune V.B. A sensitive period for musical training: contributions of age of onset and cognitive abilities. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2012;1252:163–170. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06434.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkowska M., Dalla Bella S. Uncovering phenotypes of poor-pitch singing: the sung performance battery (SPB) Front. Psychol. 2013;4:714. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00714. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boker S., Neale M., Maes H., Wilde M., Spiegel M., Brick T., Spies J., Estabrook R., Kenny S., Bates T., et al. OpenMx: an open source extended structural equation modeling framework. Psychometrika. 2011;76:306–317. doi: 10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butkovic A., Ullén F., Mosing M.A. Personality related traits as predictors of music practice: underlying environmental and genetic influences. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2015;74:133–138. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.10.006. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Coon H., Carey G. Genetic and environmental determinants of musical ability in twins. Behav. Genet. 1989;19:183–193. doi: 10.1007/bf01065903. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalla Bella S., Berkowska M. Singing proficiency in the majority: Normality and "phenotypes" of poor singing. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009;1169:99–107. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04558.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dalla Bella S., Deutsch D., Peretz I., Deutsch D. Singing proficiency in the general population. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2007;121:1182–1189. doi: 10.1121/1.2427111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drayna D., Manichaikul A., Lange M.d., Snieder H., Spector T. Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in humans. Science. 2001;291:1969–1972. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5510.1969. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson K.A., Krampe R.T., Tesch-Romer C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev. 1993;100:363–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.100.3.363. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fox J., Dusa A. McMaster University; 2019. Polycor: Polychoric and Polyserial Correlations.https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/polycor/index.html [Google Scholar]
- Gingras B., Honing H., Peretz I., Trainor L.J., Fisher S.E. Defining the biological bases of individual differences in musicality. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2015;370:20140092. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grasby K.L., Verweij K.J.H., Mosing M.A., Zietsch B.P., Medland S.E. In: Statistical Human Genetics. Elston R., editor. 2017. Estimating heritability from twin studies; pp. 171–194. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hambrick D.Z., Tucker-Drob E.M. The genetics of music accomplishment: evidence for gene-environment correlation and interaction. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2015;22:112–120. doi: 10.3758/s13423-014-0671-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hambrick D.Z., Burgoyne A.P., Macnamara B.N., Ullen F. Toward a multifactorial model of expertise: beyond born versus made. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018;1423:284–295. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hannon E.E., Trehub S.E. Tuning in to musical rhythms: infants learn more readily than adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A. 2005;102:12639–12643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504254102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison S.D. A perennial problem in gendered participation in music: what's happening to the boys? Br. J. Music Educ. 2007;24:267–280. doi: 10.1017/s0265051707007577. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchins S., Larrouy-Maestri P., Peretz I. Singing ability is rooted in vocal-motor control of pitch. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 2014;76:2522–2530. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0732-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jacoby N., Undurraga E.A., McPherson M.J., Valdés J., Ossandón T., McDermott J.H. Universal and non-universal features of musical pitch perception revealed by singing. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:3229–3243.e12. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Järvelä I. Genomics studies on musical aptitude, music perception, and practice. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2018;1423:82–91. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanduri C., Kuusi T., Ahvenainen M., Philips A.K., Lahdesmaki H., Järvelä I. The effect of music performance on the transcriptome of professional musicians. Sci. Rep. 2015;5:9506. doi: 10.1038/srep09506. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Larrouy-Maestri P., Leveque Y., Schon D., Giovanni A., Morsomme D. The evaluation of singing voice accuracy: a comparison between subjective and objective methods. J. Voice. 2013;27:259.e1–259.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2012.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Macnamara B.N., Hambrick D.Z., Oswald F.L. Deliberate practice and performance in music, games, sports, education, and professions: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:1608–1618. doi: 10.1177/0956797614535810. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mauch M., Cannam C., Bittner R., Fazekas G., Salamon J., Dai J., Bello J., Dixon S. In: Proceedings of the First International Conference on Technologies for Music Notation and Representation. Battier M., Bresson J., Couprie P., Davy-Rigaux C., Fober D., Geslin Y., Genevois H., Picard F., Tacaille A., editors. Université Paris-Sorbonne; 2015. Computer-aided melody note transcription using the TONY software: accuracy and efficiency; pp. 23–31. [Google Scholar]
- Mehr S.A., Singh M., Knox D., Ketter D.M., Pickens-Jones D., Atwood S., Lucas C., Jacoby N., Egner A.A., Hopkins E.J., et al. Universality and diversity in human song. Science. 2019;366:eaax0868. doi: 10.1126/science.aax0868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merrett D.L., Peretz I., Wilson S.J. Moderating variables of music training-induced neuroplasticity: a review and discussion. Front. Psychol. 2013;4:606. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00606. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morley A.P., Narayanan M., Mines R., Molokhia A., Baxter S., Craig G., Lewis C.M., Craig I. AVPR1A and SLC6A4 polymorphisms in choral singers and non-musicians: a gene association study. PLoS One. 2012;7:e31763. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0031763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosing M.A., Madison G., Pedersen N.L., Kuja-Halkola R., Ullen F. Practice does not make perfect: no causal effect of music practice on music ability. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:1795–1803. doi: 10.1177/0956797614541990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosing M.A., Pedersen N.L., Madison G., Ullen F. Genetic pleiotropy explains associations between musical auditory discrimination and intelligence. PLoS One. 2014;9:e113874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mosing M.A., Verweij K.J.H., Madison G., Pedersen N.L., Zietsch B.P., Ullén F. Did sexual selection shape human music? Testing predictions from the sexual selection hypothesis of music evolution using a large genetically informative sample of over 10,000 twins. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2015;36:359–366. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2015.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy K., Lam J., Cutler T., Tyler J., Calais-Ferreira L., Li S., Little C., Ferreira P., Craig J.M., Scurrah K.J., Hopper J.L. Twins Research Australia: a new paradigm for driving twin research. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2019;22:438–445. doi: 10.1017/thg.2019.101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nair P.S., Kuusi T., Ahvenainen M., Philips A.K., Järvelä I. Music-performance regulates microRNAs in professional musicians. PeerJ. 2019;7:e6660. doi: 10.7717/peerj.6660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nair P.S., Raijas P., Ahvenainen M., Philips A.K., Ukkola-Vuoti L., Järvelä I. Music-listening regulates human microRNA expression. Epigenetics. 2020;16:554–566. doi: 10.1080/15592294.2020.1809853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neale M.C., Hunter M.D., Pritikin J.N., Zahery M., Brick T.R., Kirkpatrick R.M., Estabrook R., Bates T.C., Maes H.H., Boker S.M. OpenMx 2.0: extended structural equation and statistical modeling. Psychometrika. 2016;81:535–549. doi: 10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oikkonen J., Kuusi T., Peltonen P., Raijas P., Ukkola-Vuoti L., Karma K., Onkamo P., Järvelä I. Creative activities in music - a genome-wide linkage analysis. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0148679. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park H., Lee S., Kim H.J., Ju Y.S., Shin J.Y., Hong D., von Grotthuss M., Lee D.S., Park C., Kim J.H., et al. Comprehensive genomic analyses associate UGT8 variants with musical ability in a Mongolian population. J. Med. Genet. 2012;49:747–752. doi: 10.1136/jmedgenet-2012-101209. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pfordresher P.Q., Brown S. Poor-pitch singing in the absence of "tone deafness. Music Percept. 2007;25:95–115. doi: 10.1525/MP.2007.25.2.95. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pfordresher P.Q., Demorest S.M. In: The Routledge Companion to Interdisciplinary Studies in Singing, Volume I: Development. Russo F.A., Ilari B., Cohen A.J., editors. Routledge; 2020. Construction and validation of the Seattle Singing Accuracy Protocol (SSAP): an automated online measure of singing accuracy. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pfordresher P.Q., Demorest S.M. The prevalence and correlates of accurate singing. J. Res. Music Educ. 2021;69:5–23. [Google Scholar]
- Pfordresher P.Q., Brown S., Meier K.M., Belyk M., Liotti M. Imprecise singing is widespread. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010;128:2182–2190. doi: 10.1121/1.3478782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polderman T.J.C., Benyamin B., de Leeuw C.A., Sullivan P.F., van Bochoven A., Visscher P.M., Posthuma D. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat. Genet. 2015;47:702–709. doi: 10.1038/ng.3285. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Powell S.J. Masculinity and choral singing: an Australian perspective. Int. J. Music Educ. 2014;33:233–243. doi: 10.1177/0255761414533307. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team . R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2020. R: A Language Environment for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Revelle W. Northwestern University; 2019. Psych: Procedures for Psychological, Psychometric, and Personality Research.https://personality-project.org/r/psych/ [Google Scholar]
- Seesjärvi E., Särkämö T., Vuoksimaa E., Tervaniemi M., Peretz I., Kaprio J. The nature and nurture of melody: a twin study of musical pitch and rhythm perception. Behav. Genet. 2016;46:506–515. doi: 10.1007/s10519-015-9774-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steele C.J., Bailey J.A., Zatorre R.J., Penhune V.B. Early musical training and white-matter plasticity in the corpus callosum: evidence for a sensitive period. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:1282–1290. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3578-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan Y.T., Peretz I., McPherson G.E., Wilson S.J. Establishing the reliability and validity of web-based singing research. Music Percept. 2021;38:386–405. doi: 10.1525/mp.2021.38.4.386. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Thurstone L.L. University of Chicago Press; 1935. The Vectors of Mind. [Google Scholar]
- Ullén F., Mosing M.A., Holm L., Eriksson H., Madison G. Psychometric properties and heritability of a new online test for musicality, the Swedish Musical Discrimination Test. Pers. Individ. Dif. 2014;63:87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vaquero L., Hartmann K., Ripolles P., Rojo N., Sierpowska J., Francois C., Camara E., van Vugt F.T., Mohammadi B., Samii A., et al. Structural neuroplasticity in expert pianists depends on the age of musical training onset. Neuroimage. 2016;126:106–119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watts C., Murphy J., Barnes-Burroughs K. Pitch matching accuracy of trained singers, untrained subjects with talented singing voices, and untrained subjects with nontalented singing voices in conditions of varying feedback. J. Voice. 2003;17:185–194. doi: 10.1016/s0892-1997(03)00023-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Welch G.F. In: The Child as Musician: A Handbook of Musical Development. 2nd. McPherson G.E., editor. Oxford University Press; 2015. Singing and vocal development; pp. 441–461. [Google Scholar]
- Wesseldijk L.W., Mosing M.A., Ullén F. Gene-environment interaction in expertise: the importance of childhood environment for musical achievement. Dev. Psychol. 2019;55:1473–1479. doi: 10.1037/dev0000726. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson S.J., Abbott D.F., Lusher D., Gentle E.C., Jackson G.D. Finding your voice: a singing lesson from functional imaging. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011;32:2115–2130. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21173. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson S.J., Lusher D., Martin C.L., Rayner G., McLachlan N. Intersecting factors lead to absolute pitch acquisition that is maintained in a “fixed do” environment. Music Percept. 2012;29:285–296. doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.29.3.285. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the lead contact upon reasonable request, and with the permission of Twins Research Australia. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
All original code has been deposited at Figshare and is publicly available as of the date of publication. DOIs are listed in the key resources table. The standard version of TONY is freely available at https://code.soundsoftware.ac.uk/projects/tony.