Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2022 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2022 Feb 14;57(7):1421–1433. doi: 10.1007/s00127-022-02237-7

Psychological and social support associations with mortality and cardiovascular disease in middle-aged American Indians: the Strong Heart Study

Astrid Suchy-Dicey 1, Harry Eyituoyo 1, Marcia O’Leary 2, Shelley A Cole 3, Aminata Traore 4, Steve Verney 5, Barbara Howard 6, Spero Manson 7, Dedra Buchwald 1, Paul Whitney 8
PMCID: PMC9247016  NIHMSID: NIHMS1796536  PMID: 35157091

Abstract

Purpose

Our study examined psychosocial risk and protective features affecting cardiovascular and mortality disparities in American Indians, including stress, anger, cynicism, trauma, depression, quality of life, and social support.

Methods

The Strong Heart Family Study cohort recruited American Indian adults from 12 communities over 3 regions in 2001–2003 (N = 2786). Psychosocial measures included Cohen Perceived Stress, Spielberger Anger Expression, Cook-Medley cynicism subscale, symptoms of post-traumatic stress disorder, Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression scale, Short Form 12-a quality of life scale, and the Social Support and Social Undermining scale. Cardiovascular events and all-cause mortality were evaluated by surveillance and physician adjudication through 2017.

Results

Participants were middle-aged, 40% male, with mean 12 years formal education. Depression symptoms were correlated with anger, cynicism, poor quality of life, isolation, criticism; better social support was correlated with lower cynicism, anger, and trauma. Adjusted time-to-event regressions found that depression, (poor) quality of life, and social isolation scores formed higher risk for mortality and cardiovascular events, and social support was associated with lower risk. Social support partially explained risk associations in causal mediation analyses.

Conclusion

Altogether, our findings suggest that social support is associated with better mood and quality of life; and lower cynicism, stress, and disease risk—even when said risk may be increased by comorbidities. Future research should examine whether enhancing social support can prospectively reduce risk, as an efficient, cost-effective intervention opportunity that may be enacted at the community level.

Keywords: Social support, Stress, Depression, Quality of life, Cardiovascular disease, Mortality

Introduction

Disparities in cardiovascular disease (CVD) outcomes persist by sex, race, and ethnic groups [1, 2]. American Indians have a disproportionately high burden of CVD, with risk reports chiefly focused on conventional clinical determinants including hypertension, diabetes, and obesity [3]. Psychosocial exposures such as historical traumas, chronic stress, and other psychological or social determinants have been linked to increased risk of adverse CVD outcomes in other groups [4, 5], but remain unexplored in American Indians.

Most people will be exposed during their lifetime to at least one potentially life-threatening or traumatic experience; other stressors can be ongoing, such as exposure to bullying, harassment, dysfunctional relationships, or poverty. When stress is especially intense, chronic, uncontrolled, or overwhelming, deleterious effects on health or psychology may include atherosclerosis [6, 7] and CVD [8, 9], cognitive impairment and dementia [1012], physical disability [1316], depression [1719], and low quality of life [20, 21]. Postulated biological mechanisms or mediators for these observed associations include inflammation, autonomic nervous system dysregulation, and endothelial dysfunction, although some of the mechanistic relationships are likely to be circular and multi-causal, including depression, inflammation, and cardiovascular risk. [22, 23].

In addition, adaptive or maladaptive behaviors resulting from trauma or stress exposures, which may also mediate disease associations, include poor health habits, bad sleep hygiene, reluctance to change unhealthy lifestyle behaviors, non-adherence to medical treatment, high-risk lifestyle behaviors, and poor social bonding [24]. In particular, social and socioeconomic isolation either persists or has increased over the last several decades, particularly for individuals with limited resources, despite programs and policies aimed at social integration and mobility [25]. A recent report comparing U.S. healthcare metrics by race and by state [26] suggests that disparities affecting American Indian, Alaska Native, and African American people are particularly stark, with average mortality 5 years shorter than non-Hispanic Whites, driven in part by social factors such as proximity to care. However, in some rural counties, life expectancy is especially low: 37–45 years among some American Indian males in the Northern Plains and 39–43 among some African American males in the South and Mid-Atlantic [27]. Despite such substantial structural social inequities, a recent review found no studies addressing social isolation in rural settings, suggesting important oversights in addressing the underlying causes and contributions to social and health inequity [28]. Furthermore, to our knowledge, no large, population-based study has addressed the associations among stress, resilience, social support, and related features among American Indian adults.

Underrepresentation of American Indian peoples in social and health research is problematic because distinctive healthcare systems [2932], sociodemographics [33, 34], environmental [3537], clinical [3847], cognitive [48, 50], and neurological risk profiles [5156] are likely to result in unique patterns of association as well as health trajectories. Furthermore, historical traumas can be felt through multiple generations, with tangible effects on individual biology as well as intergenerational familial, community, and social structures. The well-known and well-documented US federal policies affecting American Indian peoples as communities, including forced relocations, tribal treaty violations, and discriminatory institutional practices continue in some cases through the present day. For example, the Indian Boarding Schools program, run by the Bureau of Indian Affairs within the US Department of the Interior, compelled attendance, often at great distance from home, with little or no contact with their families, focused on societal assimilation with little attention to quality of education [57], prohibiting cultural and linguistic expression [58], and employing harsh, militaristic disciplinary techniques, resulting in high prevalence of traumatic stress disorders, depression, [59] illness, and death [60]. The social and psychological impact of these experiences on surviving children, families, communities, and culture have been profound [61]. Parental right to refuse attendance was only granted in 1978, when attendance was near its peak at around 60–70,000 children per year. [62].

Despite such patterns of risk—and although communities and tribes may differ—American Indian peoples and populations have largely displayed remarkable resilience to stress and trauma [63, 64]. In studies among Diné people, such resilience has been attributed to Hózhó, a strength-based wellness philosophy emphasizing the wholeness of person and community and valuing engagement in cultural, social, and familial structures [63, 6568]. In studies of youth, up to 30% of the variance in physical and emotional health may be attributed to this type of psychological resilience and related cultural perspectives, with a substantial portion relying on family caring and social support [21, 69, 70]. However, risk or protection features related to stress, resilience, and social support among older American Indians have not been quantified.

This study explores features that promote resilience, including social support, and features that may inhibit resilience or result from excess trauma, such as perceived stress, anger, cynicism, depression, and poor perceived quality of life; and to establish these features in association with CVD and mortality outcomes among older American Indians. Our ultimate hypothesis is that potential negative psychological influences on health may be modified or even interrupted by positive psychological influences, suggesting that health risks may be ameliorated somewhat through targeted, culturally appropriate interventions. This work has the potential to advance public health knowledge on innovative risk and protective features in a vulnerable population.

Methods

Study setting

The Strong Heart Study (SHS) encompasses the largest population-based cohort of American Indians ever recruited, with 4549 enrolled beginning in 1989–1991 from 13 tribal communities across the Northern Plains, Southern Plains, and Southwest regions representing 67% of eligible residents then aged 35–75 years. [71] Study examinations included several waves of longitudinal data collection over the subsequent 3 decades, focused chiefly on CVD and diabetes risk, with 85–87% successful recruitment and participation at each subsequent examination visit [71]. Continuous morbidity and mortality surveillance with physician committee based adjudication for CVD and stroke events have been conducted, with complete assessment currently available through December 31, 2017 [40, 56]. The Strong Heart Family Study (SHFS) was an expansion cohort that recruited an additional 3838 related family members at SHS Exam 4 (2001–2003). One of the participating communities has subsequently withdrawn from all research, and their data have been excluded from analyses, resulting in a maximum SHS/SHFS overlapping N = 2786. Detailed recruitment methods for SHS and SHFS, including informed consent and ethical review procedures, have been previously published [71, 72].

Timeline and inclusion

Our analyses used the psychosocial data collected at the SHS Exam 2 (1993–1995) and at the SHFS Exam 4 (2001–2003), and the continuous morbidity and mortality surveillance data (through 2017). Inclusion criteria for our analyses are: participation in the SHS Exam 2 visit + SHFS Exam 4 visit, and no prior CVD or stroke event. All participants spoke fluent English; all examinations were conducted in English.

Psychosocial scales

Few standard measures have been psychometrically or theoretically validated among American Indians. Our research team has recently conducted preliminary psychometric assessments of two instruments, as noted below. However, most such measures have unknown performance characteristics. The stress scale was measured at the SHS Exam 2; all others were measured at SHFS Exam 4.

Perceived stress

On the original 10-point Cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [73], each item is rated on a 5-point scale (0–4), with ratings summed to create a total ranging from 0 to 40. Scores of 13 are considered average; scores of 20 or higher are considered high stress [74]. Our data included 7 of the original 10 questions; we adjusted our total scores by a factor of 10/7 to allow for comparability with previous studies.

Anger management and expression

The Spielberger Anger Expression (S-AX) [75, 76] scale is comprised of 20 questions on a 4-point scale (1–4). Three subscales (AX-In: likelihood of concealing anger, 9-item; AX-Out: likelihood of being hostile, 8-item; AX-Control, 3-item: extent of ability to control own anger) [77] and total summary score represent facets of anger management and experience. We focused our analyses on the AX-Out, AX-In, and AX-Control sub-scores separately due to psychometric differences in withholding, expressing, or controlling anger.

Cynicism

Originally developed for the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory, the Cook-Medley hostility scale (Cook) [78, 79] consisted of 50 items, divided across 6 subscales (cynicism, hostile attributions, hostile affect, aggressive responding, social avoidance, and other) [77]. Our data collections included only the 8-item cynicism subscale (Cook-C); each item was scored on a binary scale (0,1) and summed across items for a total score ranging from 0 to 8.

Trauma

Similar to other scales used to assess post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), our unvalidated set of questions assessed symptoms of trauma and stress, focused on recurrent thoughts, disruption of sleep, and sustained anxiety related to one or more past events and lasting for at least one month. Nine (9) items were coded on a binary scale (0,1) and summed for a total score ranging from 0 to 9.

Depression

The Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale is a screening tool for depression and depressive symptoms [80], and among the most widely used instruments in clinical medicine and psychiatry [81, 82]. The original scale consists of 20 items assessing symptoms of depression, with responses scored on a 4-point scale (0 to 3), and 4 items reverse-coded. Possible summary total scores range from 0 to 60, with scores ≥ 16 considered to be evidence of depressive symptoms [83]. Our team previously reported CES-D scores among American Indians aged 65–95 years mean 11 (SD 8) for the 20-item scale. [84].

Quality of life

The Short Form 12-question (SF-12) survey [85], a measure of health-related quality of life, is a 12-item scale with varying item coding among multiple subscales, including general health perceptions, vitality, bodily pain, physical function, emotional function, social function, and mental health. Items are coded (or reverse coded) so that higher scores corresponding to worse health, and each item is scored with a minimum score of 1, so that the total possible summary scores range from 12 to 47. In comparison to the longer SF-36 [86], SF-12 had similar score performance but larger standard errors.

Social support

Social support and social undermining (SS/U) [87, 88] were measured using 20 items derived from the National Comorbidity Survey, tailored specifically for American Indian populations, and then validated by the American Indian Service Utilization and Psychiatric Epidemiology Risk and Protective Factors Project [89]. Positive subscales covered emotional support (6 items coded 1–3) and instrumental support (5 items coded 0,1), with higher scores connoting better social support; negative subscales covered critical appraisal (6 items coded 1–3) and isolation (3 items coded 1–3), with higher scores connoting poorer social support or greater isolation. Summary scores accounting for reverse coding included the four subscales together, with a possible range 15–50.

Outcome Measures

Event surveillance and physician adjudications for clinical events were conducted by the SHS morbidity and mortality events committee and include events from before the SHS baseline visit (1989–1991) through December 31, 2017 [40], although events occurring before the time of the visit when the relevant exposure variable was measured were excluded. Surveillance methods used to identify possible events included telephone contact, interim clinic visits, and mailed questionnaires for self-report. Paper and electronic medical records at the Indian Health Service (IHS) and local non-IHS hospitals and patient care facilities were also reviewed for possible events including ICD-9 codes as well as and manual review of participant records by field staff for specific terminology (e.g. subarachnoid, intracerebral, or intracranial occlusion of cerebral or precerebral arteries including embolism or thrombosis, cerebral ischemia, stroke, cerebral atherosclerosis, hypertensive encephalopathy, and unspecified lesions). The adjudication process started with abstraction of complete records including medical history, physical examination, emergency room visit, medical consult, medical imaging, discharge summary, operation and other procedure reports, for all surveillance-identified possible events. A committee of physicians then reviewed and discussed all possible events, with consensus decision as possible, probable, or definite event.

Other measures

All participant characteristics were measured at the SHFS Exam 4 (2001–2003), when the majority of the psychosocial scales were collected. Field center was defined by geographic region. Participants self-reported age (years), sex (male, female), annual household income, years of formal education, and daily medication use. Waist and hip circumference, height, and weight were measured using standard anthropometric techniques, with body mass index (BMI) defined as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters, squared. Blood pressure was measured by sphygmomanometry, with the average of the second and third seated measure recorded. Blood samples were collected to measure fasting plasma glucose, serum cystatin C and creatinine, C-reactive protein (CRP), interleukin-6 (IL-6), and plasminogen activator inhibitor 1 (PAI-1). Estimated glomerular filtration rate (eGFR) was calculated using the CKD-EPI 2012 equation (using both serum creatinine and cystatin C). Diabetes mellitus was defined as fasting plasma glucose ≥ 126 mg/dL or use of insulin or oral diabetes medications. Hypertension was defined as measured systolic blood pressure ≥ 140, diastolic blood pressure ≥ 90, or use of antihypertensive medications.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation, count, percent) were used to summarize selected participant characteristics—including sociodemographics, anthropometrics, clinical covariates, lab assays, and psychosocial scales—at the time of the SHFS Exam 4 visit (2001–2003) among all available participants (N = 2,786). Histograms for psychosocial scale scores were used to graphically examine the range and distribution of these measures, in this understudied population. Standardized (Z scored) Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients (rho) were used to evaluate unadjusted associations between combinations of psychosocial scale measures, with graphical display based on color range for different values of rho. Cox proportional hazards time-to-event regressions evaluated risk of all-cause mortality or composite CVD event (myocardial infarction, congestive heart failure, stroke) [90] outcomes for each standardized psychosocial scale individually, with hazard ratios (and 95% confidence intervals) calculated per standard deviation on the Z-scale, to allow for direct comparability across all exposure measures. Participants with death or CVD outcomes that occurred before the end of the relevant exposure examination were excluded; exclusions were done separately for each model. Adjustment covariates were age, sex, education, income, and site (Model A); lab measures related to inflammation, diabetes, and hypertension–which may develop as a result of chronic stress, and may, therefore, lie in the causal pathway—were additionally included in Model B. Causal mediation analysis [91], a method that parses a single estimate of association into both direct and indirect estimates, with the indirect effect attributed to the mediator, was conducted for the exposures that had consistent patterns of significant findings with mortality in the Cox models, with calculation of the coefficient for the exposures with and without the mediator in the model as well as calculation of the estimated percent of effect mediated, using quasi-Bayesian Monte Carlo algorithm with 1000 replications/simulations. Missingness varied among different features, but was overall low and noninformative. Because exposures are strongly correlated, P value adjustment such as Bonferroni (P < 0.003) would be overly conservative. To protect against the problem of multiple comparisons in this non-independent testing context, we evaluated findings using a Bayesian framework, based on consistency and clinical interpretability of the full range of findings (family-wise error), rather than by adjustment of individual single P values, to ensure appropriate inference and reduced likelihood of Type I error.

Results

SHFS Exam 4 participants were generally middle-aged, mean age 40.8 years; approximately 40% were male (Table 1). On average, participants had equivalent of a high school education and below-average annual household income, although there was large variance. The waist–hip ratio and body mass index was generally high, with majority considered obese. Systolic blood pressure and glomerular filtration rate were generally not abnormal, but nearly 20% of participants had diabetes and 32% hypertension. CRP was generally high, with wide variability (reference range < 3 mg/L); IL-6 was generally normal (reference range < 16.4 pg/mL), but some participants had very high values—more than 2,000; PAI-1 was overall high (reference range 2–15 AU/mL).

Table 1:

Selected participant characteristics of American Indians from the Strong Heart Family Study (2001–2003)

N=2786

Age, years 40.8 (17.3)
Male, n (%) 1125 (40.4%)
Education, years 12.2 (2.3)
Annual Household Income <$20,000, n(%) 1178 (53.9%)
Southern Plains center, n (%) 1220 (43.8%)
Northern Plains center, n (%) 1210 (43.4%)
Southwest center, n (%) 356 (12.8%)
Waist-hip ratio (WHR) 0.9 (0.1)
Body mass index (BMI) 31.3 (7.5)
Systolic blood pressure (SBP), mmHg 123.0 (16.9)
Estimated Glomerular Filtration Rate (eGFR), mL/min 96.5 (26.2)
Diabetes Mellitus (DM), n (%) 529 (19.1%)
Hypertension, n (%) 886 (32.0%)
C-reactive protein (CRP), mg/L 6.6 (9.5)
Interleukin-6 (IL-6), pg/mL 12.7 (68.8)
Plasminogen activator inhibitor 1 (PAI1), AU/mL 57.2 (44.5)

Stress: PSS Score (possible range 0–40) 14.2 (3.6); 0–24
Anger: AX-Out Score (possible range 9–36) 15.7 (4.2); 9–35
Anger: AX-In Score (possible range 8–32) 15.6 (4.2); 8–32
Anger: AX-Control Score (possible range 3–12) 8.3 (2.1); 3–12
Cynicism: Cook-C (possible range 0–8) 3.7 (2.2); 0–8
Trauma: PTSD Score (possible range 0–9) 4.5 (2.5); 1–9
Depression: CESD Score (possible range 0–60) 13.1 (10.8); 0–56
Quality of life: SF-12 Score (possible range 12–47) 22.6 (6.1); 12–44
Social support-Emotional subscore (possible range 6–18) 15.2 (2.7); 6–18
Social support-Instrumental subscore (possible range 0–5) 4.4 (1.1); 0–5
Social support-Criticism subscore (possible range 6–18) 9.9 (2.6); 6–18
Social support-Isolation subscore (possible range 3–19) 4.5 (1.5); 3–9
Social support-Total score (possible range 15–50) 41.2 (6.0); 17–50

All numbers given as mean and standard deviation, unless otherwise specified. Observed range added for neuropsychological or sociocultural scales.

Psychosocial scale means, ranges, and distributions are shown in Table 1 and Fig. 1. Cohen perceived stress scale (PSS) was generally normally distributed, with observed range truncated to being somewhat lower than maximal possible range. Spielberger Anger Expression (AX) subscale scores were left-skewed with most scores corresponding to less extreme scores on anger expression. Cook-Medley Hostility scale cynicism subscale (Cook-C) scores were somewhat bimodal, with many scoring 0, but otherwise distributed normally. Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD, trauma) symptoms scores were somewhat uniformly distributed across the entire range of possible symptoms. CES-D depression scale scores and SF-12 quality of life scores were left-skewed, with most scores corresponding to fewer symptoms or better quality of life, respectively. Finally, social support subscales were skewed with the largest number of scores corresponding to stronger social support.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Distributions of neuropsychological and psychosocial measures among American Indian adults from the Strong Heart Family Study (2001–2003).

Legend: Histograms showing distributions of psychosocial measures, based on frequency. Measures include Cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), Spielberger anger expression (AX) subscales, Cook-Medley cynicism scale, PTSD scale, Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale, Short Form 12 (SF) a quality of life scale, and Social Support and Social Undermining (SS/U) scale. Higher PSS scores connote higher perceived degree of life stress; higher AX-Out scores more likelihood to express anger outward; higher AX-In scores more likelihood to hide anger; higher AX-Control scores greater ability to control anger; higher Cook-C scores greater degree of cynical views or perspectives; higher PTSD score more symptoms related to traumatic experience; higher CES-D scores more symptoms of depression; higher SF-12 scores worse quality of life; higher emotional, instrumental, and total support scores greater degree of social support; higher criticism and isolation scores lower degree of social support.

Especially strong (rho > 0.6) positive correlations between scales (Fig. 2) include stress-trauma, anger out-anger in, anger in-depression, anger in-instrumental social support, anger in-social isolation, cynicism-depression, trauma-social isolation, depression-poor quality of life, depression-social criticism, depression-social isolation, poor quality of life-social criticism, emotional-instrumental support, emotional-total social support, instrumental-total social support. Especially strong (rho < − 0.6) negative or inverse correlations include cynicism-total social support, depression-emotional support, depression-total social support, poor quality of life-emotional support, poor quality of life-total social support, social criticism-total social support.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Standardized Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient heatmap among neuropsychological and psychosocial measures among American Indian adults from the Strong Heart Family Study (2001–2003).

Legend: Heatmap of Pearson correlation coefficients among standardized neuropsychological and psychosocial measures. Red indicates high degree of positive correlation and blue indicates high degree of negative correlation (inverse); with strength of coloration corresponding to coefficient indicated in legend (right). Variables all standardized (z-scored) to allow cross-comparisons. Measures include Cohen Perceived Stress Scale (PSS), Spielberger anger expression (AX) subscales, Cook-Medley cynicism scale, PTSD scale, Centers for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CES-D) scale, Short Form 12 (SF) a quality of life scale, and Social Support and Social Undermining (SS/U) scale.

For time-to-event analyses, we followed N = 2786 participants with 480 failures for mortality over 40,106.1 person-years (12 deaths per 1000 person-years) and with 299 failures for CVD composite events over 42,522.4 person-years (7 CVD events per 1000 person-years). Cox regressions (Table 2) showed that higher CES-D score (more symptoms of depression) and higher SF-12 score (poorer quality of life) were associated with higher risk of all-cause mortality, both without (HR 1.2, 95% CI 1.1–1.4; HR 1.5, 95% CI 1.3–1.6, respectively) and with (HR 1.3, 95% CI 1.1–1.4; HR 1.4, 95% CI 1.3–1.6, respectively) adjustment for potentially mediating clinical features as described in Methods. Higher social criticism and social isolation were also significantly associated with higher risk of mortality (HR 1.1, 95% CI 1.0–1.2; HR 1.2, 95% CI 1.1–1.3, respectively) adjusted for all features. However, more emotional support, instrumental support, and total social support were inversely associated with mortality (HR 0.9, 95% CI 0.8–1.0; HR 0.9, 95% CI 0.8–1.0; HR 0.8, 95% CI 0.8–0.9, respectively). The degree of these associations, all standardized, suggest that the strongest degree of association is for SF-12, but the associations for CES-D and Social support features are of similar magnitude. Associations for composite CVD outcome followed similar patterns, although CES-D, SF-12, and instrumental support scales were not significantly associated after adjustment for confounding and/or mediating features.

Table 2:

Standardized Cox proportional hazards models for time to event among American Indians in the Strong Heart Family Study (2001–2003)

All-cause mortality Model A Model B
HR 95% CI P-value HR 95% CI P-value

PSS 1.06 (0.89, 1.26) 0.498 1.03 (0.86, 1.23) 0.783
AX-Out 0.98 (0.83, 1.16) 0.812 0.97 (0.80, 1.17) 0.737
AX-In 1.03 (0.88, 1.19) 0.740 1.05 (0.88, 1.25) 0.570
AX-Control 0.95 (0.81, 1.11) 0.507 0.94 (0.80, 1.11) 0.480
Cook-C 1.11 (0.97, 1.28) 0.111 1.09 (0.94, 1.27) 0.236
PTSD 1.10 (0.89, 1.36) 0.362 1.13 (0.90, 1.41) 0.292
CESD 1.24 (1.11, 1.39) <0.001 1.26 (1.11, 1.43) <0.001
SF12 1.45 (1.32, 1.59) <0.001 1.42 (1.29, 1.58) <0.001
Support-Emotional 0.91 (0.83, 0.99) 0.044 0.89 (0.81, 0.99) 0.030
Support-Instrumental 0.90 (0.83, 0.97) 0.008 0.89 (0.81, 0.97) 0.009
Support-Criticism 1.09 (0.99, 1.20) 0.076 1.11 (1.01, 1.23) 0.031
Support-Isolation 1.15 (1.05, 1.27) 0.004 1.17 (1.05, 1.29) 0.003
Social Support-Total 0.86 (0.78, 0.95) 0.002 0.84 (0.75, 0.93) 0.001

Composite CVD Model A Model B
HR 95% CI P-value HR 95% CI P-value

PSS 1.06 (0.84, 1.33) 0.617 1.06 (0.83, 1.37) 0.631
AX-Out 1.06 (0.87, 1.28) 0.568 1.08 (0.87, 1.33) 0.504
AX-In 1.07 (0.90, 1.28) 0.420 1.09 (0.90, 1.31) 0.382
AX-Control 1.07 (0.90, 1.28) 0.438 1.05 (0.87, 1.28) 0.583
Cook-C 1.10 (0.94, 1.30) 0.238 1.05 (0.88, 1.25) 0.595
PTSD 0.99 (0.82, 1.21) 0.944 1.06 (0.86, 1.31) 0.599
CESD 1.14 (0.99, 1.32) 0.064 1.17 (1.01, 1.36) 0.041
SF12 1.15 (1.03, 1.29) 0.014 1.11 (0.98, 1.26) 0.100
Support-Emotional 0.84 (0.75, 0.94) 0.003 0.83 (0.73, 0.93) 0.002
Support-Instrumental 0.89 (0.90, 0.99) 0.037 0.92 (0.82, 1.04) 0.181
Support-Criticism 1.16 (1.03, 1.31) 0.016 1.22 (1.07, 1.38) 0.002
Support-Isolation 1.18 (1.04, 1.33) 0.011 1.15 (1.01, 1.31) 0.040
Social Support-Total 0.80 (0.71, 0.90) <0.001 0.79 (0.70, 0.89) <0.001

Notes: Composite cardiovascular disease (CVD), including myocardial infarction, congestive heart failure, stroke. HR= hazard ratio; 95% CI=95% confidence interval. Variables all standardized (z-scored) to allow cross-comparisons. Model A adjustment: field center, age, sex, education, waist-hip ratio; Model B adjustment: Model A plus inflammatory-responsive mediators added, including systolic blood pressure, fasting glucose, C-reactive protein, interleukin 6, plasminogen activator inhibitor 1.

Causal mediation analyses of social support subscales (Table 3) suggest that both positive (emotional, instrumental) and negative (criticism, isolation) features partially arbitrate the associations of CES-D and SF-12 with the mortality outcome. The estimated percent of total effect mediated (arbitrated) by social support ranged from 1 to 15% for CES-D and from 0 to 4% for SF-12.

Table 3:

Non-standardized mediation analyses for mortality outcome with social support as mediation features, among American Indians in the Strong Heart Family Study (2001–2003)

Mortality-CESD Coefficients without medication Coefficients with mediation Estimate of % of total effect mediated
β 95% CI P-value β 95% CI P-value % 95% CI

Social-Emotional −0.09 (−0.11, −0.08) <0.001 0.02 (0.01, 0.04) 0.002 0.01 (0.01, 0.03)
Social-Instrumental −0.03 (−0.03, −0.03) <0.001 0.02 (0.01, 0.03) 0.006 0.09 (0.06, 0.21)
Social-Criticism 0.10 (0.09, 0.11) <0.001 0.02 (0.01, 0.03) 0.007 0.11 (0.07, 0.25)
Social-Isolation 0.07 (0.06, 0.08) <0.001 0.02 (0.00, 0.03) 0.014 0.15 (0.10, 0.32)

Mortality-SF12 Coefficients without medication Coefficients with mediation Estimate of % of total effect mediated
β 95% CI P-value β 95% CI P-value % 95% CI
Social-Emotional −0.12 (−0.14, −0.11) <0.001 0.07 (0.05, 0.09) <0.001 0.00 (0.00, 0.00)
Social-Instrumental −0.04 (−0.05, −0.03) <0.001 0.07 (0.05, 0.09) <0.001 0.04 (0.04, 0.05)
Social-Criticism 0.14 (0.12, 0.15) <0.001 0.07 (0.05, 0.09) <0.001 0.00 (0.00, 0.00)
Social-Isolation 0.09 (0.08, 0.10) <0.001 0.07 (0.05, 0.09) <0.001 0.03 (0.03, 0.03

Logistic regressions of CESD or SF12 on mortality; adjusted for field center, age, sex, education, waist-hip ratio; 1000 replications/simulations

Discussion

Overall, these findings suggest that, although symptoms of stress, expressed anger, cynicism, trauma, depression, poor quality of life, social criticism, and isolation were common; emotional control and social support were also common among middle-aged American Indians. Future research may examine population features associated with each scale and psychological domain, such as features that may predict or determine better emotional or instrumental support, or more extreme social isolation. Such findings may be useful in identifying subgroups at particularly high risk, or which have particularly keen characteristics related to higher resilience.

We also found that negative psychological features correlated strongly with each other, with depression especially commonly correlated with other features; and that social support negatively correlated with most of the negative psychological features, especially depression and (poor) quality of life. Social support also correlated inversely with cynicism; anger and trauma correlated positively with social isolation. All together these findings suggest that social support and social connectedness may be important to preventing, lowering, or improving depression, quality of life, and trauma, but that cynicism and anger may be psychological features that disrupt such social functioning. Future research identify specific subgroups at higher risk, sociological mechanisms, and potential targeted interventions.

In adjusted regressions, worse depression, poorer quality of life, social isolation, and social criticism were significantly associated with risk of mortality and CVD events, both with and without adjustment for clinical features that may mediate an inflammatory response. Furthermore, emotional and instrumental social support was associated with lower risk of these outcomes, and also partially mediated the effect for depression and quality of life scales. These findings suggest that improving social support may provide an effective opportunity to improve depression and health-related quality of life in middle-aged American Indians, with possible reduction of mortality and cardiovascular outcomes. Future research may be justified to examine the efficacy of social support programs on these and other outcomes.

The relationship between social support and CVD morbidity and mortality (particularly for coronary heart disease) has been well documented in previous studies in the general US population [9294]. In a cohort of 1,381 African–American adults, social support was associated with preventing or delaying CVD onset [95]. Another study showed that perceived social support was associated with lower mortality among women free of CVD at baseline [96]. However, the results from those studies may not be generalizable to American Indians. To the best of our knowledge, there are no studies directly assessing the relationship between social support, CVD events, and all-cause mortality in American Indians. Additionally, comparability in effect estimates across populations is not possible due to differences in measurement scales; future research should directly compare populations for such differences.

Furthermore, the evaluation of inter-feature correlations and mediation by inflammatory features and by social support features as potential determinants of resilience have been underexplored. Antidepressants may lower inflammation and anti-inflammatory agents may reduce symptoms of depression [97], with consequent effects on vascular risk, suggesting that depression and inflammation may be key factors in the causal chain between stress, vascular or neurodegenerative brain aging, and mortality. A nationally representative longitudinal survey of adults in the United States conducted by the Survey Research Center of the University of Michigan showed that social support as a resilience factor mediated the effect of depression on coronary heart disease [98]. In comparison, our study showed a mediating effect of social support on depression and poor quality of life on mortality outcome. However, social support was an independent negative correlate for anger, trauma, and isolation as well.

Possible explanations for our findings could be the strong cultural values of community connectedness [99, 100] observed in many American Indian communities and tribes. Although culturally heterogeneous, many members strongly value family, community, and heritage—with an emphasis on multiple generations. Previous studies have shown social support to be a protective risk factor for life satisfaction in older American Indians [70, 101, 102]. Social support, both emotional and instrumental, may reduce stress, promote cardiovascular resilience, and improve survival. Future strategies by public health and health care professionals may consider factors that promote social connectedness and reduce isolation.

As in previous studies on depression and CVD [92, 93, 103], our study showed an increased risk of CVD events and mortality in participants with greater symptoms of depression and lower quality of life. Major depressive disorder is a well-documented risk factor for incident CVD in both healthy patients and those with established CVD [92, 93, 103, 104]. Postulated mechanisms involve an interplay between a behavioral and neuroendocrine system such as medication non-adherence, sedentary lifestyle due to depressive symptoms, hyperactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary axis, platelet activation, endothelial dysfunction, alterations in cardiac autonomic tone, increase in catecholamine and serotonin level [105]. This is the first study conducted in American Indians to show the interrelation between depressive symptoms, quality of life, social support, and CVD outcomes in healthy participants after adjusting for traditional cardiovascular risk factors. Depression and quality of life are directly related to each other, and studies have shown that treating depression could improve health-related quality of life. Future studies may focus on assessing the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral therapy and pharmacotherapy in combination with social programs on treating clinical depression, CVD, and mortality outcomes in this heavily burdened population.

In a theoretical or conceptual model of resilience or coping reserve, life stresses, conflict, trauma, and health disparities may accumulate, to cause an affected person to “run low” in resilience or coping reserve, with possible symptoms including feelings of stress, depression, anger, cynicism, and poor perceived quality of life. With the influence of positive inputs, such as social support and connectedness, such adverse effects may be counterbalanced, resulting in maintaining wellness and optimism. This framework may be used to understand the findings in this study, but may not fully explain the results. For example, we discovered that adjustment for potential mediators, including CVD comorbidities and inflammatory markers, did not change risk estimates. It is possible the salient mediators were not measured in this study, or there may be other, different health characteristics that mediate risk between social support, low quality of life, or depression, and mortality or CVD events.

Some of the findings in this study also warrant further consideration. For example, the Cook-Medley cynicism subscale was inversely correlated with many of the negative psychosocial measures. Also, the Anger-In scale was correlated with the Instrumental Social Support scale. Cynicism may vary independently, with unknown or latent determinants; and those needing or receiving tangible social support may be more likely to keep anger symptoms inside rather than expressed, to preserve their relationships. However, such suppositions are hypothetical and unexplored. Furthermore, resilience and other beneficial features, including cultural identity and participation have been unexplored. Future research efforts may benefit from more comprehensive, complete, or direct measures of such constructs.

Strengths and limitations

The strengths of this work include the comprehensive clinical and interview data collection protocols with the longitudinal evaluation of outcomes in a heterogeneous population-based recruitment setting across multiple regions, tribes, and communities. This is also the first attempt to assess psychosocial risk factors and CVD outcomes in a large longitudinal cohort of American Indians. Lastly, this is the first study in American Indians to describe social support as an independent mediator for inflammatory and stress-related exposures with significant risk for CVD and mortality outcomes.

There are also some limitations. First, although the construct of resilience was of interest, we did not directly measure individual resilience and so were limited to evaluating individual characteristics more distally related to resilience. Future research may benefit from direct measurement of resilience and other features in this and other similar populations. Also, the psychosocial exposures are measured only once; thus, these measures may not fully or accurately represent lifetime or usual exposure—which may contribute to measurement error. Further, many of these scales have not been validated in American Indians, so it is unknown to what degree they accurately represent the underlying factors that they have been developed to measure–possibly introducing errors of inference. Because American Indians comprise unique cultural, historical, educational, linguistic, psychological, and cognitive profiles, formal validations are critical to construct validity; furthermore, American Indian peoples are not homogeneous and so such validity may differ even among tribe, language group, or region. Initial assessments of the CES-D scale in this population suggest that 12 of the 20 items demonstrate reasonable measurement invariance, with further validation needed [106]. Third, without a direct measure of resilience, we implicitly assumed that the positive mediating factors evaluated are related to psychosocial resilience, based on previous reports in similar populations (in American Indians, these are mostly children). Fourth, features were measured by standardized self-report scale on questionnaire based instrument, which are common and practical tools for epidemiologic and clinic settings, but unable to replace the gold standard of full-day neuropsychological interview or direct functional testing. Finally, there may be unmeasured cultural or other resilience-related factors that independently contribute to these health outcomes, which, if true, could limit discoverability.

Future directions

Analyses examining scale validity, including measures of internal consistency and reliability, independent dimensionality, and appropriate measured and latent constructs that best capture stress and resilience-related factors, should follow. Such work requires multiple measures of a scale over time, and so follow-up measurement of these scales in this population are warranted. Future research may also benefit from direct measurement of resilience as an independent psychological construct, as well as intermediate biomarkers of stress or a larger inflammation panel that represents a larger set of systemic biological functions. Additionally, regional, cultural, or linguistic adaptation of some measurement items may be warranted. Finally, the effects of psychosocial factors on cognitive outcomes, including vascular and Alzheimer’s dementia, should be evaluated due to the strong overlap with cardiovascular, metabolic, and inflammatory pathological mediators.

In summary, our findings—that social support, depression, and quality of life may be key to risk and amelioration of mortality and CVD events in this heavily burdened population—have the potential to advance public health knowledge and programs on modifiable conditions strongly related to health disparities. If improving social support and connectedness can effectively reduce mortality and CVD risk caused by stress, trauma, then intervention programs aimed at these mediating factors may represent efficient, cost-effective risk reduction opportunities that may be enacted at the community level. Our findings should encourage researchers, public health practitioners, and communities to consider social support and companionship promoting programs, among other resilience-promoting interventions, as possible tools to investigate for addressing premature mortality, cardiovascular disease, and related conditions.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank Strong Heart Study staff and participants. The opinions included herein are those of the authors, and not necessarily those of the Indian Health Service (IHS). This work has been funded in whole or in part with federal funds from the National Institutes of Health by research grants K01AG057821, P50AG005136, R01HL109315, R01HL109284, and R01HL109319; and by the Health Equity Research Center, a Washington State University strategic research initiative.

Footnotes

Declarations

Conflict of interest The authors have no conflicts to report.

Data availability

The data used in these analyses are part of a data repository for the Strong Heart Study cohort. These data are the intellectual property of the tribes from which the data were collected, and their use for analysis is contingent on formal approval procedures. For more information, please visit strongheartstudy.org.

References

  • 1.Pool LR, Ning H, Lloyd-Jones DM, Allen NB. Trends in racial/ethnic disparities in cardiovascular health among US adults from 1999–2012. J Am Heart Assoc 2017;6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hutchinson RN, Shin S (2014) Systematic review of health disparities for cardiovascular diseases and associated factors among American Indian and Alaska native populations. PLoS ONE 9:e80973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Breathett K, Sims M, Gross M et al. (2020) Cardiovascular health in American Indians and Alaska natives: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation 141:e948–e959 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hagstrom E, Norlund F, Stebbins A et al. (2018) Psychosocial stress and major cardiovascular events in patients with stable coronary heart disease. J Intern Med 283:83–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kim JH, Islam SJ, Topel ML et al. (2020) Individual psychosocial resilience, neighborhood context, and cardiovascular health in black adults: a multilevel investigation from the morehouse-emory cardiovascular center for health equity study. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes 13:6638. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Pedersen SS, von Kanel R, Tully PJ, Denollet J (2017) Psychosocial perspectives in cardiovascular disease. Eur J Prev Cardiol 24:108–115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Dimsdale JE (2008) Psychological stress and cardiovascular disease. J Am Coll Cardiol 51:1237–1246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Harwell TS, Oser CS, Okon NJ, Fogle CC, Helgerson SD, Gohdes D (2005) Defining disparities in cardiovascular disease for American Indians: trends in heart disease and stroke mortality among American Indians and whites in Montana, 1991 to 2000. Circulation 112:2263–2267 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kim G, Bryant AN, Goins RT, Worley CB, Chiriboga DA (2012) Disparities in health status and health care access and use among older American Indians and Alaska Natives and non-Hispanic Whites in California. J Aging Health 24:799–811 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Chin AL, Negash S, Hamilton R (2011) Diversity and disparity in dementia: the impact of ethnoracial differences in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 25:187–195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Jervis LL, Manson SM (2002) American Indians/Alaska natives and dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 16(Suppl 2):S89–95 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Weiner MF, Rosenberg RN, Svetlik D et al. (2003) Comparison of Alzheimer’s disease in Native Americans and Whites. Int Psychogeriatr 15:367–375 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goins RT, Innes K, Dong L (2012) Lower body functioning prevalence and correlates in older American Indians in a southeastern tribe: the Native Elder Care Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 60:577–582 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Goins RT, Moss M, Buchwald D, Guralnik JM (2007) Disability among older American Indians and Alaska Natives: an analysis of the 2000 Census Public Use Microdata Sample. Gerontologist 47:690–696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Okoro CA, Denny CH, McGuire LC, Balluz LS, Goins RT, Mokdad AH (2007) Disability among older American Indians and Alaska Natives: disparities in prevalence, health-risk behaviors, obesity, and chronic conditions. Ethn Dis 17:686–692 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Schure MB, Goins RT (2016) An examination of the disablement process among older American Indians: the native elder care study. Gerontologist 56:948–955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dillard DA, Muller CJ, Smith JJ, Hiratsuka VY, Manson SM (2012) The impact of patient and provider factors on depression screening of american Indian and alaska native people in primary care. J Prim Care Community Health 3:120–124 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Garrett MD, Baldridge D, Benson W, Crowder J, Aldrich N (2015) Mental health disorders among an invisible minority: depression and dementia among american Indian and alaska native elders. Gerontologist 55:227–236 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hiratsuka VY, Smith JJ, Norman SM, Manson SM, Dillard DA (2015) Guideline concordant detection and management of depression among Alaska Native and American Indian people in primary care. Int J Circumpolar Health 74:28315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Beals J, Welty TK, Mitchell CM et al. (2006) Different factor loadings for SF36: the Strong Heart Study and the National Survey of Functional Health Status. J Clin Epidemiol 59:208–215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Schure MB, Odden M, Goins RT (2013) The association of resilience with mental and physical health among older American Indians: the Native Elder Care Study. Am Indian Alsk Native Ment Health Res 20:27–41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Burnette CE, Kaapu K, Scarnato JM, Liddell J (2020) Cardiovascular health among U.S. indigenous peoples: a holistic and sex-specific systematic review. J Evid Based Soc Work 17:24–48 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Osimo EF, Baxter LJ, Lewis G, Jones PB, Khandaker GM (2019) Prevalence of low-grade inflammation in depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis of CRP levels. Psychol Med 49:1958–1970 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Burkett JP, Young LJ (2012) The behavioral, anatomical and pharmacological parallels between social attachment, love and addiction. Psychopharmacology 224:1–26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Wang Q, Phillips NE, Small ML, Sampson RJ (2018) Urban mobility and neighborhood isolation in America’s 50 largest cities. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:7735–7740 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Radley DC, Baumgartner JC, Collins SR, Zephyrin L, Schneider EC (2021) Achieving racial and ethnic equity in U.S. health care: a scorecard of state performance. The Commonwealth Fund, New York [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Jian Y, Neas L, Messer LC et al. (2019) Divergent trends in life expectancy across the rural-urban gradient and association with specific racial proportions in the contiguous USA 2000–2005. Int J Public Health 64:1367–1374 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Franck L, Molyneux N, Parkinson L (2016) Systematic review of interventions addressing social isolation and depression in aged care clients. Qual Life Res 25:1395–1407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Zuckerman S, Haley J, Roubideaux Y, Lillie-Blanton M (2004) Health service access, use, and insurance coverage among American Indians/Alaska Natives and Whites: what role does the Indian Health Service play? Am J Public Health 94:53–59 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Barnes PM, Adams PF, Powell-Griner E. Health characteristics of the American Indian and Alaska Native adult population: United States, 2004–2008: US Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Health Statistics, Division of Health Interview Statistics; 2010. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Frieden TR, Centers for Disease C, PRevention (2011) Forward: CDC health disparities and inequalities report—United States, 2011. MMWR Suppl 60:1–2 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kim C (2000) Recruitment and retention in the Navajo Area Indian Health Service. West J Med 173:240–243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fact Sheets and Tool Kits. Seattle Indian Health Board. http://www.uihi.org/Resources/Fact-Sheets-Tool-Kits/. Accessed 30 Nov 2016
  • 34.Defining the Rural Population. 2015. http://www.hrsa.gov/ruralhealth/policy/definition_of_rural.html
  • 35.McOliver CA, Camper AK, Doyle JT et al. (2015) Community-based research as a mechanism to reduce environmental health disparities in american Indian and alaska native communities. Int J Environ Res Public Health 12:4076–4100 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Navas-Acien A, Umans JG, Howard BV et al. (2009) Urine arsenic concentrations and species excretion patterns in American Indian communities over a 10-year period: the Strong Heart Study. Environ Health Perspect 117:1428–1433 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Suchy-Dicey A, Noonan C, Burduli E et al. (2020) Urinary arsenic and cadmium associations with findings from cranial MRI in American Indians: data from the Strong Heart Study. Environ Health Perspect 128:127009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Liao Y, Bang D, Cosgrove S et al. (2011) Surveillance of health status in minority communities—racial and ethnic approaches to community health across the US (REACH US) risk factor survey, United States, 2009. Morbidity Mortal Wkly Rep Surveill Summ 60:1–44 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Shibata D, Suchy-Dicey A, Carty CL et al. (2019) Vascular Risk Factors and Findings on Brain MRI of Elderly Adult American Indians: the Strong Heart Study. Neuroepidemiology 52:173–180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Zhang Y, Galloway JM, Welty TK et al. (2008) Incidence and risk factors for stroke in American Indians: the Strong Heart Study. Circulation 118:1577–1584 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Howard BV, Lee ET, Fabsitz RR et al. (1996) Diabetes and coronary heart disease in American Indians: The Strong Heart Study. Diabetes 45(Suppl 3):S6–13 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Ellis JL, Campos-Outcalt D (1994) Cardiovascular disease risk factors in native Americans: a literature review. Am J Prev Med 10:295–307 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Campos-Outcalt D, Ellis J, Aickin M, Valencia J, Wunsch M, Steele L (1995) Prevalence of cardiovascular disease risk factors in a southwestern Native American tribe. Public Health Rep 110:742–748 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Gohdes D Diabetes in North American Indians and Alaska Natives. Diabetes in America. 2nd ed: National Institutes of Health/NIDDK; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Lee ET, Howard BV, Savage PJ et al. (1995) Diabetes and impaired glucose tolerance in three American Indian populations aged 45–74 years. The Strong Heart Study. Diabetes Care 18:599–610 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Centers for Disease C Prevention (2004) Declining prevalence of no known major risk factors for heart disease and stroke among adults–United States, 1991–2001. MMWR Morbidity Mortal Wkly Rep 53:4–7 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Jernigan VB, Duran B, Ahn D, Winkleby M (2010) Changing patterns in health behaviors and risk factors related to cardiovascular disease among American Indians and Alaska Natives. Am J Public Health 100:677–683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Suchy-Dicey A, Shibata D, Cholerton B et al. (2020) Cognitive correlates of MRI-defined cerebral vascular injury and atrophy in elderly American Indians: the Strong Heart Study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 26:263–275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Alzheimer’s Disease and Healthy Aging. Subjective cognitive decline among American Indian/Alaska Native Adults. 2019. https://www.cdc.gov/aging/data/infographic/2017/american-Indian-alaska-native-cognitive-decline.html. Accessed 10 Feb 2020
  • 50.Verney SP, Suchy-Dicey AM, Cholerton B et al. (2019) The associations among sociocultural factors and neuropsychological functioning in older American Indians: The Strong Heart Study. Neuropsychology 33:1078–1088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Daugherty J, Waltzman D, Sarmiento K, Xu L (2019) Traumatic brain injury-related deaths by race/ethnicity, sex, intent, and mechanism of injury—United States, 2000–2017. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep 68:1050–1056 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Peterson AB, Sarmiento K, Xu L, Haileyesus T (2019) Traumatic brain injury-related hospitalizations and deaths among American Indians and Alaska natives—United States, 2008–2014. J Saf Res 71:315–318 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Sarmiento K, Kennedy J, Daugherty J et al. (2020) Traumatic brain injury-related emergency department visits among American Indian and Alaska native persons-national patient information reporting system, 2005–2014. J Head Trauma Rehabil 35:E441–E449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Muller CJ, Alonso A, Forster J et al. (2019) Stroke incidence and survival in american indians, blacks, and whites: the strong heart study and atherosclerosis risk in communities study. J Am Heart Assoc 8:10229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Suchy-Dicey A, Muller C, Shibata D et al. (2021) Comparing vascular brain injury and stroke by cranial magnetic resonance imaging, physician-adjudication, and self-report: data from the strong heart study. Neuroepidemiology 55:398–406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Wang W, Zhang Y, Lee ET et al. (2017) Risk factors and prediction of stroke in a population with high prevalence of diabetes: the strong heart study. World J Cardiovasc Dis 7:145–162 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Enoch MA, Albaugh BJ (2017) Review: genetic and environmental risk factors for alcohol use disorders in American Indians and Alaskan Natives. Am J Addict 26:461–468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Lynch PDC (1990) The emergence of American Indian leadership in education. J Am Indian Educ 29:1–10 [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lomawaima T, McCarty TL (2006) To remain Indian: Lessons in democracy from a century of Native American education. Teachers College Press, New York [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Jack AS (2000) Secwepemc cultural education society. Behind closed doors stories from the Kamloops Indian Residential School. Secwepemc Cultural Education Society, Kamloops, B.C. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Zephier Olson MD, Dombrowski K. A systematic review of Indian boarding schools and attachment in the context of substance use studies of native Americans. J Racial Ethn Health Disparities 2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Running Bear U, Thayer ZM, Croy CD, Kaufman CE, Manson SM (2019) The impact of individual and parental American Indian boarding school attendance on chronic physical health of northern plains tribes. Fam Community Health 42:1–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Denham AR (2008) Rethinking historical trauma: narratives of resilience. Transcult Psychiatry 45:391–414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Roh S, Burnette CE, Lee KH, Lee YS, Easton SD, Lawler MJ (2015) Risk and protective factors for depressive symptoms among American Indian older adults: adverse childhood experiences and social support. Aging Ment Health 19:371–380 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Kelley A, Small C (2016) Establishing the reliability and validity of the sources of strength in one American Indian community. Am Indian Alsk Native Ment Health Res 23:84–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Kahn-John Dine M, Koithan M (2015) Living in health, harmony, and beauty: the dine (navajo) hozho wellness philosophy. Glob Adv Health Med 4:24–30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Kahn-John M (2016) The path to development of the Hozho Resilience Model for nursing research and practice. Appl Nurs Res 29:144–147 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Wexler L (2014) Looking across three generations of Alaska Natives to explore how culture fosters indigenous resilience. Transcult Psychiatry 51:73–92 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Cummins JR, Ireland M, Resnick MD, Blum RW (1999) Correlates of physical and emotional health among Native American adolescents. J Adolesc Health 24:38–44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Conte KP, Schure MB, Goins RT (2015) Correlates of social support in older American Indians: the Native Elder Care Study. Aging Ment Health 19:835–843 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Lee ET, Welty TK, Fabsitz R et al. (1990) The Strong Heart Study A study of cardiovascular disease in American Indians: design and methods. Am J Epidemiol 132:1141–1155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.North KE, Howard BV, Welty TK et al. (2003) Genetic and environmental contributions to cardiovascular disease risk in American Indians: the strong heart family study. Am J Epidemiol 157:303–314 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R (1983) A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 24:385–396 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Cohen SJDD (2012) Who’s stressed? Distributions of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples from 1983, 2006 and 2009. J Appl Soc Psychol 42:1320–1334 [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Knight RG, Chisholm BJ, Paulin JM, Waal-Manning HJ (1988) The Spielberger anger expression scale: some psychometric data. Br J Clin Psychol 27(Pt 3):279–281 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Chesney MA, Rosenman RH (1985) Anger and hostility in cardiovascular and behavioral disorders. Hemisphere Pub. Corp, Washington [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Miller TQJC, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT (1995) Are all hostility scales alike? Factor structure and covariation among measures of hostility. J Appl Soc Psychol 25:1142–1168 [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Cook WWMD (1954) Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI. J Appl Soc Psychol 38:414–418 [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Barefoot JC, Dodge KA, Peterson BL, Dahlstrom WG, Williams RB (1989) The Cook-Medley Hostility Scale: Item content and ability to predict survival. Psychosom Med 51:46–57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Radloff R (1977) The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1:385–401 [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Murphy JM (2002) Symptom scales and diagnostic schedules in adult psychiatry. In: Tsuan MT, Tohen M (eds) Textbook in psychiatric epidemiology. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 273–332 [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Naughton MJ, Wiklund I (1993) A critical review of dimension-specific measures of health-related quality of life in cross-cultural research. Qual Life Res Int J Qual Life Asp Treat Care Rehab 2:397–432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Eaton WW, Muntaner C, Smith C, Tien A, Ybarra M (2004) Center for epidemiologic studies depression scale: review and revision (CESD and CESD-R). In: Maruish ME (ed) The use of psychological testing for treatment planning and outcome assessment, 3rd edn. Lawrence Erlbaum, Mahwah, pp 363–377 [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Suchy-Dicey A, Verney SP, Nelson LA et al. (2020) Depression symptoms and cognitive test performance in older American Indians: the strong heart study. J Am Geriatr Soc 68:1739–1747 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Ware J Jr, Kosinski M, Keller SD (1996) A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care 34:220–233 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Ware JE Jr (2000) SF-36 health survey update. Spine (Phila Pa 1976) 25:3130–3139 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Oetzel J, Duran B, Jiang Y, Lucero J (2007) Social support and social undermining as correlates for alcohol, drug, and mental disorders in American Indian women presenting for primary care at an Indian Health Service hospital. J Health Commun 12:187–206 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S et al. (1994) Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States. Results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry 51:8–19 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Beals J, Manson SM, Mitchell CM, Spicer P (2003) Cultural specificity and comparison in psychiatric epidemiology: walking the tightrope in American Indian research. Cult Med Psychiatry 27:259–289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Dhamoon MS, Elkind MS (2010) Inclusion of stroke as an outcome and risk equivalent in risk scores for primary and secondary prevention of vascular disease. Circulation 121:2071–2078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Imai K, Keele L, Tingley D (2010) A general approach to causal mediation analysis. Psychol Methods 15:309–334 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Barth J, Schumacher M, Herrmann-Lingen C (2004) Depression as a risk factor for mortality in patients with coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis. Psychosom Med 66:802–813 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Lett HS, Blumenthal JA, Babyak MA et al. (2004) Depression as a risk factor for coronary artery disease: evidence, mechanisms, and treatment. Psychosom Med 66:305–315 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Ikeda A, Iso H, Kawachi I et al. (2008) Social support and stroke and coronary heart disease: the JPHC study cohorts II. Stroke 39:768–775 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Hernandez DC, Reitzel LR, Wetter DW, McNeill LH (2014) Social support and cardiovascular risk factors among black adults. Ethn Dis 24:444–450 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Freeborne N, Simmens SJ, Manson JE et al. (2019) Perceived social support and the risk of cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality in the Women’s Health Initiative Observational Study. Menopause 26:698–707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Zuzarte P, Duong A, Figueira ML, Costa-Vitali A, Scola G (2018) Current therapeutic approaches for targeting inflammation in depression and cardiovascular disease. Curr Drug Metab 19:674–687 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Liu RT, Hernandez EM, Trout ZM, Kleiman EM, Bozzay ML (2017) Depression, social support, and long-term risk for coronary heart disease in a 13-year longitudinal epidemiological study. Psychiatry Res 251:36–40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Hill DL (2009) Relationship between sense of belonging as connectedness and suicide in American Indians. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 23:65–74 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Schultz K, Cattaneo LB, Sabina C, Brunner L, Jackson S, Serrata JV (2016) Key roles of community connectedness in healing from trauma. Psychol Violence 6:42–48 [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Roh S, Kim Y, Lee KH, Lee Y-S, Burnette CE, Lawler MJ (2015) Religion, social support, and life satisfaction among american indian older adults. J Relig Spiritual Soc Work Soc Thought 34:414–434 [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Tehee M, Buchwald D, Booth-LaForce C, Omidpanah A, Manson SM, Goins RT (2019) Traumatic stress, social support, and health among older American Indians: the native elder care study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 74:908–917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Williams SA, Kasl SV, Heiat A, Abramson JL, Krumholz HM, Vaccarino V (2002) Depression and risk of heart failure among the elderly: a prospective community-based study. Psychosom Med 64:6–12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.van Melle JP, de Jonge P, Spijkerman TA et al. (2004) Prognostic association of depression following myocardial infarction with mortality and cardiovascular events: a meta-analysis. Psychosom Med 66:814–822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Bradley SM, Rumsfeld JS (2015) Depression and cardiovascular disease. Trends Cardiovasc Med 25:614–622 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Barbosa-Leiker C, Burduli E, Arias-Losado R et al. (2021) Gender differences in the assessment of depression in American Indian older adults: The Strong Heart Study. Psychol Assess 33:574–579 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The data used in these analyses are part of a data repository for the Strong Heart Study cohort. These data are the intellectual property of the tribes from which the data were collected, and their use for analysis is contingent on formal approval procedures. For more information, please visit strongheartstudy.org.

RESOURCES