Skip to main content
Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire) logoLink to Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire)
. 2021 Sep 7;57(4):437–444. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agab064

Irregular Autonomic Modulation Predicts Risky Drinking and Altered Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Response to Stress in Alcohol Use Disorder

Seungju Hwang 1,, Jorge S Martins 2, Ryan J Douglas 3, Justin J Choi 4, Rajita Sinha 5, Dongju Seo 6,
PMCID: PMC9270986  PMID: 34491306

Abstract

Aims

Autonomic dysfunction has been associated with risky drinking and alcohol use disorder (AUD). Although autonomic nervous system (ANS) activity has been attributed to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VmPFC)-limbic-striatal regions, the specific role of ANS disruption in AUD and its association with these regions remain unclear. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and concurrent electrocardiogram (ECG), the current study examined neural correlates of ANS activity in AUD and its role in AUD pathology.

Methods

Demographically matched 20 AUD patients and 20 social drinkers (SD) completed an fMRI task involving repeated exposure to stress, alcohol-cue and neutral-relaxing images in a block design. Based on the known VmPFC-limbic-striatal functions involved in emotions, reward and the ANS, we performed a regions of interest (ROI) analysis to examine the associations between ANS activity and neural responses in the VmPFC, amygdala, and ventral striatum.

Results

Across conditions, AUD patients showed significantly higher levels of overall heart rate (HR) and approximate entropy (ApEn) compared to SD (Ps < 0.05). In all participants, increased HR was associated with greater drinking volume (P < 0.05). In addition, higher ApEn levels were associated with greater drinking volume (P < 0.05) and decreased right VmPFC response to stress (P < 0.05).

Discussion

Our findings demonstrate ANS disruption in AUD indexed by high overall HR and ApEn. The association between ApEn and rVmPFC response suggests that ApEn may play a role in modulating drinking via interactions with neural regions of emotion regulation. These findings provide insight into patterns of ANS disruption and their relevance to AUD pathology.


Short Summary: Greater disruption in the autonomic nervous system was found in patients with alcohol use disorder, including higher overall approximate entropy (ApEn) and heart rate, compared to controls. Higher ApEn was associated with increased drinking and decreased stress response in brain regions of emotion regulation, such as the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

INTRODUCTION

Individuals with alcohol use disorder (AUD) display disruptions in the autonomic nervous system (ANS) including elevated heart rate (HR) and diminished heart rate variability (HRV), which may result from complex interacting factors at the level of the brain and body (Ralevski et al. 2019). Despite numerous studies on AUD-related ANS disruption, its role in alcohol pathology and underlying neural mechanisms remain unclear. In previous studies, we found basal group differences with higher HR in AUD patients than in controls (Sinha 2009; Seo et al. 2013). Unlike other response measures such as brain activity and emotion ratings (which differentially react to stress and alcohol-cue exposure), ANS dysfunction in AUD was characterized by overall, noncondition-specific HR dysregulation (Seo et al. 2013). This overall ANS disruption in AUD has been interpreted as long-term ANS hyperarousal resulting from repeated alcohol use (Cheng et al. 2019). This interpretation is also consistent with the basal state hyperactive physiology hypothesis in AUD (Breese et al. 2011). Although these concepts present viable explanations, further investigation into different components of ANS measures may provide useful insight into complex AUD pathology.

Emerging evidence indicates that nonlinear characteristics of HRV may uncover distinct aspects of pathology that are not fully understood by conventional linear metrics (Karmakar et al. 2017). Classic HRV indices, such as root mean square of successive differences (RMSSD) and high-frequency power (HF), characterize parasympathetic activation and vagal tone in healthy and AUD populations (Cheng et al. 2019), yet few studies have examined nonlinear HRV indices such as approximate entropy (ApEn). ApEn is known to detect complex ANS patterns by quantifying the influence of ANS and other physiological factors (e.g. thermoregulation, endocrine factors) on HRV, thereby providing an index of the complex network of nonlinear systems in ANS regulation (Young and Benton 2015). Therefore, examining ApEn may help capture autonomic complexity related to AUD pathology.

In addition, investigating neural systems underlying ANS modulation is necessary to further elucidate AUD pathology. Recent studies suggest that the central autonomic network plays a crucial role in addictive behaviors by regulating and receiving feedback from ANS via neurocardiovascular pathways (Eddie et al. 2020) and modulating neural responses via breathing (Bates et al. 2019). Although these studies highlight the importance of brain-ANS interactions, few studies have investigated autonomic and brain functions simultaneously (Napadow et al. 2008), limiting our comprehension of AUD pathology.

To better understand autonomic dysfunction in AUD, the current study examined both nonlinear HRV (i.e. ApEn) and conventional HRV (i.e. HF, LF, RMSSD) metrics via electrocardiogram (ECG) signal data in conjunction with blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) responses to stress and alcohol cues obtained from a well-validated emotion provocation task using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Sinha et al. 2016). Using this fMRI-ECG method, we examined concurrent brain-ANS responses with baseline drinking measures in AUD patients and social drinkers (SD). We used a regions-of-interest (ROI) approach to specifically study associations between the ANS and the ventromedial-limbic-striatal region, a well-known neural system involved in emotion, reward, and ANS function (Hiser and Koenigs 2018). Evidence consistently points to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VmPFC) as an underlying neural correlate of emotional control (e.g. stress, reward), ANS modulation (Thayer et al. 2012), and alcohol-related pathology, such as high craving and early relapse (Seo et al. 2013; Blaine et al. 2020). In addition, limbic-striatal pathways involved in emotion and reward processing, especially the amygdala and ventral striatum (VS), are known to play important roles in ANS modulation (Rajmohan and Mohandas 2007). These studies suggest that ANS activity in AUD can be differentially modulated by activity in ventromedial-limbic-striatal regions represented by the VmPFC, amygdala, and VS. Accordingly, our ROI approach specifically examined the role of a priori regions (VmPFC, amygdala, VS) in modulating ANS activity in AUD. Based on prior literature, we expect AUD patients to exhibit greater overall ANS hyperactivity across conditions. We further hypothesize that nonlinear HRV metrics, such as ApEn, will additionally explain autonomic disruption in AUD patients and be associate with altered activity in these regions.

METHODS

Participants and study procedure

Forty adults (20 treatment-seeking AUD, 20 SD) were recruited from the Greater New Haven area. Given the high rates of comorbidity between AUD and other psychiatric disorders (Castillo-Carniglia et al. 2019), those with comorbid conditions were included to model real-world AUD pathology. Controls (SD) were regular, low-risk drinkers based on NIAAA guidelines (NIAAA 2009). The AUD group was demographically matched to the SD group on age, sex, intelligence, and comorbid conditions (Table 1). To remove the effects of physiological disorders, those with somatic and cardiovascular diseases were excluded. Blood pressure levels were at normal range in all participants. For further information on our sample, please refer to Supplemental Information (SI). Prior to the fMRI session, baseline demographic, psychiatric, and cognitive measures were collected from all participants. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 (First 2015) was administered to determine AUD and assess any additional psychiatric diagnoses. Participants also completed the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders et al. 1993) and Quantity-Frequency Variability (QFV) Index (Cahalan and Cisin 1968) as measures of problematic drinking and alcohol consumption patterns over time. For all appointments and fMRI sessions, breathalyzer and urine screenings confirmed drug and alcohol abstinence. All study procedures were approved by the Human Investigation Committee of the Yale University School of Medicine.

Table 1.

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Subject variable AUD patients (N = 20) Social drinkers (SD) (N = 20) P-values
Age (s.d.) 31.4 (10.4) 29.4 (9.3) n.s.
Sex (% female) 10 (50.0%) 10 (50.0%) n.s.
Race (N%)
Caucasian 10 (50.0%) 10 (50.0%) n.s.
Cognitive information, s.d.
Education, in years 14.2 (2.1) 15.4 (2.4) n.s.
Shipley, test score 110.2 (5.7) 111.5 (5.1) n.s.
Baseline alcohol assessment, s.d.
AUDIT total score 19.3 (5.4)* 3.6 (3.0) P < 0.001
Drinks per week, QFV Index 27.7 (16.9)* 3.3 (3.7) P < 0.001
Years of regular use 9.9 (9.8) 6.2 (8.0) n.s.
Monthly drink frequency 28.2 (32.2)* 5.7 (4.6) P < 0.001
Maximum drinks in one sitting 10.1 (6.4)* 4.2 (2.6) P = 0.001
Psychiatric history
PTSD (current) 2 1 n.s.
Depression (current) 1 1 n.s.

There were no significant differences in age, sex ratio, race, or intelligence as measured by both years of education and by the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (range: 98–123) (Zachary et al. 1985). Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) scores were used to measure problematic alcohol use and drinking volume, and the Cahalan Quantity-Frequency Variability (QFV) Index was used to measure drinking volume and frequency. Compared to social drinkers (SD), AUD participants reported significantly higher drinking volume, drinking frequency, maximum number of drinks in one sitting, and alcohol-related problems. s.d. = standard deviation; n.s. = nonsignificant; *P ≤ 0.001.

fMRI session

Overview

All fMRI-ECG sessions were conducted in the morning (8–10 AM) to control for the potential effects of circadian rhythm and diurnal hormone variations. During the scan, participants underwent a block-designed, sustained emotion provocation (SEP) task (Sinha et al. 2016) (Fig. 1). There were three blocks, each displaying visual stimuli from one of three conditions (stress [S], alcohol-cue [A], neutral-relaxing [N]). As part of a larger published study (Blaine et al. 2020) reporting brain activity in AUD, the current study examined the subset of individuals with available ECG data and primarily focused on ANS activity associated with neural responses using concurrent ECG-fMRI data collection.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Task design. The sustained emotion provocation (SEP) task contained three blocks, each displaying visual stimuli from one of the three conditions (stress [S], alcohol-cue [A], neutral-relaxing [N]). Examples of pictures shown during each condition are depicted above. Each condition presented three baseline runs of blank gray images with a centered fixation cross followed by six provocation runs containing picture content representative of the condition. Each run consists of 11 visual stimuli. All visual stimuli were presented through a liquid crystal display panel using E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., PA, USA).

Visual stimuli

The visual stimuli were 66 highly stressful, 66 alcohol-cue, and 66 neutral-relaxing images; all images were either selected from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al. 2008) or developed by the Yale Stress Center as previously described (Sinha et al. 2016; Blaine et al. 2020). The stressful images consisted of photographs depicting scenes of terror, violence, disgust, threat, etc. The alcohol-cue pictures included images of various alcoholic beverages (e.g. beer, wine, liquor) by themselves or within a range of social scenarios (e.g. bars, social drinking). The neutral-relaxing images contained neutral-relaxing content (e.g. relaxing meditation poses, natural landscapes, etc.).

Each condition started with 3 baseline runs, consisting of blank gray images with a centered fixation cross, followed by 6 provocation runs containing picture content pertaining to the applied condition. Each run consisted of 11 discrete visual stimuli. To achieve equivalent levels of emotional intensity of pictures throughout the task, we ensured that there were no statistical differences in valence/arousal ratings or in picture content across the 6 provocation runs within each condition, as described previously (Sinha et al. 2016; Blaine et al. 2020). For detailed information on the valence and arousal ratings of the visual stimuli, refer to SI. All visual stimuli were presented with E-Prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pennsylvania, USA) on a liquid crystal display panel mounted on the fMRI head coil.

fMRI experimental task

On the fMRI scan day, all individuals received training on the fMRI experimental procedure. Participants completed a practice task consisting of 4 runs using neutral visual stimuli that were not used during the actual fMRI scan task. After a 15-minute adaptation period supplemented by a 4-minute guided relaxation exercise to create standardized baseline conditions across participants, the participant entered an MRI suite at approximately 8:00 AM and underwent the SEP task.

In this task, participants were asked to view a series of images in the stress [S], alcohol-cue [A], and neutral-relaxing [N] conditions. Participants were shown a total of 198 pictures (66 pictures per condition). The order of stimuli and conditions were randomized and counterbalanced across subjects. For each baseline/provocation run, the blank or visual image was presented for 5 seconds with a 1-second interstimulus interval, and each image was presented only once. At the end of each run, participants rated their stress and alcohol craving using a scale displayed on the screen, with the scales consisting of numbers 1 (not at all) through 9 (very much). The ‘stress’ rating asked participants to rate their level of tension, anxiety, and distress. The ‘craving’ rating asked participants to rate their level of desire to drink alcohol while viewing the visual stimuli. Participants selected their ratings using a nonferromagnetic button box in each hand.

Data acquisition

MRI data were acquired on a 3-T Siemens Prisma MRI system, equipped with a standard quadrature head coil, using a T2*-sensitive gradient-recalled single-shot echo-planar pulse sequence. For fMRI data, 75 axial slices parallel to the AC-PC line covering the whole brain were acquired with the following parameters: TR = 1000 ms, TE = 30 ms, bandwidth = 1894 Hz/pixel, flip angle = 55°, field of view = 220x220 mm, slice thickness = 2 mm with no gap. A high-resolution 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence was used to acquire sagittal anatomical images. fMRI data were acquired with a 64-channel head coil with multiband accelerated echo-planar imaging sequence. To address potential susceptibility artifact issues in gradient-echo-planar imaging, we used microscopic voxel size, high bandwidth, and advanced registration methods (see SI for further details). Throughout the fMRI session, ECG signals were concurrently acquired by Siemen’s MRI-compatible 4-lead EKG system.

ECG data processing and analysis

ECG signals were preprocessed with a bandpass Butterworth filter, and RR intervals were detected, artifact-corrected, and analyzed for linear (HR, HF, LF, RMSSD) and nonlinear (ApEn) HRV measures with Kubios HRV software. Before conducting analyses, raw ECG data were checked for normalcy and for outliers, and we removed univariate outliers for HR (±2 standard deviations from the group mean) as well as HRV data points associated with those HR outliers (3.4% of the data set) to ensure that data results were not impacted by ECG signal artifacts. Linear HRV indices were log-transformed. ApEn and HR were not log-transformed, as the data were normally distributed (see SI for details).

fMRI data processing and analysis

fMRI data were converted from DICOM to Analyze format and motion-corrected for three translational and three rotational directions. On the individual level, we performed a GLM analysis with a regressor specific to each fMRI task condition using BioImage Suite (Duncan et al. 2004). For each run, functional images were spatially smoothed, and individually normalized beta maps were generated. To account for individual variations in brain anatomy, we calculated three sequential registrations and applied them to the individually normalized beta maps.

To examine the associations of ANS activity and specific functions in the ventromedial-limbic-striatal regions, we used a regions of interest (ROI) approach to focus on the role of these hypothesized regions. Based on prior literature (Lane et al. 2009; Thayer et al. 2012; Hiser and Koenigs 2018), three structured brain regions related to emotion, reward, and ANS modulation were defined as a priori ROI, which included the VmPFC (BA 10), ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens) and amygdala. The structural ROIs for these regions were anatomically defined using the Yale-Brodmann atlas from BioImage Suite (www.bioimagesuite.org). For each region, beta weights were extracted from mean blood–oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal (averaged across all voxels within each ROI per participant) in Stress relative to Neutral-Relaxing (S–N) and Alcohol-Cue relative to Neutral-Relaxing (A–N) contrasts. Then, these signals were associated with both ANS and drinking measures in our sample using regression analyses to examine the functional associations of these ROIs. Outliers in regression analyses were evaluated by Cook’s distance and visual inspection of each scatterplot.

RESULTS

Demographic and clinical characteristics

Table 1 summarizes basic demographic and background information for AUD and SD groups. No group differences were found in sex, age, race, intelligence and comorbid psychiatric disorders, except for alcohol use (see Table 1).

Alcohol craving and stress ratings

Mixed factorial ANOVA analyses (2 × 3) were conducted on alcohol craving and stress ratings with Group (AUD, SD) as a between-subjects factor and Condition (A, N, S) as a within-subjects factor (Fig. 2). These models produced significant Group and Condition main effects, but no interaction (Group × Condition) effects. Group main effects were found for craving (F1,36 = 18.32, P < 0.05) and stress ratings (F1,36 = 5.37, P < 0.05), indicating that AUD patients reported higher overall craving (t = 4.57, P < 0.01) and higher stress levels (t = 2.38, P < 0.05) compared to SD. Condition main effects were also found for craving (F1,35 = 6.86, P < 0.05) and stress ratings (F1,35 = 38.44, P < 0.05). Craving ratings were higher during the alcohol-cue condition compared to stress and neutral-relaxing conditions (all Ps < 0.05). Stress ratings were higher during the stress condition compared to alcohol-cue and neutral-relaxing conditions (all Ps < 0.05).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Craving and stress ratings. Across groups, all participants reported (a) higher alcohol craving levels during the alcohol-cue condition than either the stress or neutral-relaxing conditions (P < 0.05) and (b) higher stress levels during the stress condition than either the alcohol-cue or neutral-relaxing conditions (P < 0.05). Group main effect was significant, such that AUD patients reported higher overall craving (P < 0.01) and higher overall stress levels (P < 0.05) than social drinkers (SD) across conditions. *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01.

Group differences in ANS activity

Mixed factorial ANOVA analyses were conducted on HR and HRV measures including Group (AUD, SD) as a between-subjects factor and Condition (A, N, S) as a within-subjects factor (Fig. 3). These models produced significant Group main effects for HR (F1,36 = 4.69, P < 0.05) and ApEn (F1,34 = 6.46, P < 0.05), indicating that AUD patients reported higher overall HR (t = 2.16, P < 0.05) and higher ApEn levels (t = 2.51, P < 0.05) during the provocation period. There were no significant effects of Condition or Group × Condition in HR and ApEn values. No significant effects were found for Group, Condition, or Group × Condition on linear HRV measures (HF, LF, RMSSD).

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Group differences in ANS activity. Across conditions, AUD patients exhibited higher (a) mean heart rate (HR; bpm) and (b) approximate entropy measures (ApEn; m, r, N) during the provocation period compared to social drinkers (SD) (Ps < 0.05). Higher AUDIT scores were associated with (c) higher HR (Inline graphic=0.33, P < 0.05) and (d) higher ApEn scores (Inline graphic=0.38, P < 0.05) in all participants. *P < 0.05.

As only Group main effects were found for HR and ApEn, we averaged each ANS measure across conditions and used them as overall ANS measures. The overall HR and ApEn measures were computed by averaging values across provocation runs (6 per condition) and the three task conditions. Then, the overall HR and ApEn measures were associated with drinking measures and BOLD responses to stress and alcohol cues within the AUD group, SD group and both groups. In preliminary analysis, we also averaged the data per condition and examined their associations with drinking and BOLD responses. However, the results were consistent with the results from the overall measure.

ANS activity and drinking behaviors

Regression analyses were conducted including participants from both AUD and SD groups to determine whether overall HR and ApEn measures were associated with drinking behaviors (Fig. 3). HR was positively associated with AUDIT score (β = 0.33, P < 0.05) and alcohol volume defined as average drinks per week (β = 0.36, P < 0.05). Similarly, ApEn was positively associated with AUDIT score (β = 0.38, P < 0.05) and alcohol volume (β = 0.33, P < 0.05). No outliers were found in any of these associations. HR and ApEn levels were associated with weekly average drinking amount, but not with other types of drinking measures, such as maximum number of drinks, frequency or duration of drinking.

Neural correlates of ANS activity

Regression analyses were conducted to examine the associations between overall ApEn/HR measures, BOLD responses, and drinking behavior (Fig. 4). BOLD responses in the a priori brain regions (VmPFC, amygdala, VS) in S–N and A–N contrasts were used as predictors, and either ApEn, HR or AUDIT scores were used as the outcome variable. A significant negative association was found between right VmPFC response to stress (S–N) and ApEn values (Inline graphic= − 0.39, P < 0.05) among all participants. In addition, decreased rVmPFC response to stress was associated with total AUDIT scores (Inline graphic= − 0.37, P < 0.05) and drinking volume (Inline graphic= − 0.35, P < 0.05). No outliers were found in these associations. No other statistically significant association was found for other brain regions or with the A–N contrast.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Neural correlates of ANS activity. (a) ROI: right medial PFC (BA 10) from the Yale-Brodmann Atlas. Decreased activity in the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex (rVmPFC) during the stress condition, relative to neutral-relaxing, was associated with (b) higher ApEn levels (Inline graphic= − 0.39, P < 0.05) and (c) higher AUDIT scores (Inline graphic= − 0.37, P < 0.05) in all participants.

Controlling for psychiatric comorbidities

To evaluate the potential influence of comorbid conditions, we controlled for coexisting psychiatric disorders in all statistical analyses (see SI). Our findings remained significant, indicating that psychiatric comorbidity did not significantly impact the current findings.

DISCUSSION

The current study reports that AUD is characterized by high ANS arousal and irregular autonomic modulation indexed by high HR and ApEn values. Furthermore, increased ApEn was associated with greater alcohol consumption and a blunted rVmPFC response to stress, pointing to ApEn’s crucial role in modulating drinking behaviors through interactions with key brain regions of emotion regulation. These results highlight the importance of disrupted autonomic function in AUD and its role in increasing vulnerability to risky drinking.

As expected, when comparing AUD and SD, we found that AUD patients exhibited higher overall HR and ApEn across conditions. This noncondition-specific group difference in overall ANS function is a replication of our prior findings (Seo et al. 2013) using HR collected concurrently with fMRI during stress, alcohol-cue and neutral-relaxing imagery. This is also consistent with prior studies noting heightened ANS function in AUD (Cheng et al. 2019), including a recent study that found no difference in HR between alcohol-cue and neutral conditions (Wang et al. 2020). Consistent with the concept of hyperexcitable physiological states associated with AUD (Breese et al. 2011), increased HR across conditions may indicate autonomic disruption resulting from long-term ANS hyperarousal. In addition, we observed elevated overall ApEn in AUD patients. To our knowledge, this is the first report of high ApEn as an index of ANS disruption in the AUD literature.

Although ApEn has not been widely studied in AUD, it has gained recognition in cardiovascular and psychiatric research (Bar et al. 2007; Byun et al. 2019). Emerging trends point to nonlinear HRV metrics, such as ApEn, for new features of cardiac modulation that are not captured by conventional linear HRV patterns. These studies propose that intact nonlinear HRV (e.g. ApEn) patterns reflect coordinated cardiac modulation by integrating information from a vast feedback network of spontaneity (spontaneous depolarization and repolarization of the sinoatrial node) and adaptability (ANS modulation) (Shi et al. 2017). This coordination is critical for adaptive ANS responses and flexible coping during stressful situations. Thus, the high ApEn values observed in our AUD patients may reflect maladaptive coping responses resulting from erratic ANS modulation.

Although the current study found significance in HR and ApEn associated with AUD, we did not find significance using linear HRV measures (e.g. LF, HF, RMSSD). However, some AUD studies using linear HRV measures report altered vagal modulation and heightened ANS activity indexed by reductions in HF and RMSSD, indicating decreased parasympathetic control and subsequent sympathetic dominance (Cheng et al. 2019). These discrepant results may be explained by a study reporting no linear HRV (LF, HF) differences between AUD patients and controls in supine positions while finding altered HRV in standing positions (Sucharita et al. 2012). The supine position of our fMRI participants may have hindered optimal evaluation of changes in linear HRV metrics (Cheng et al. 2019). In addition, task differences may also account for the discrepancies with the literature due to our specific task with stress, alcohol-cue and neutral-relaxing pictures. Supporting this, our results on HR elevation across conditions replicate the findings from a prior task using stress, alcohol-cue and neutral-relaxing imagery (Sinha et al. 2009; Seo et al. 2013).

Like its linear HRV counterparts, nonlinear HRV has been associated with vagal dysregulation in AUD (Bar et al. 2008). However, ApEn may further explain complex ANS irregularity resulting from compromised autonomic function, which can adversely impact brain–heart interactions (Shaffer and Ginsberg 2017). For example, ApEn has been applied to electroencephalography (EEG), which shows significantly higher (more irregular) basal EEG ApEn values in AUD patients than controls (Shri et al. 2014). The higher ApEn observed in both ECG and EEG studies suggests that AUD is characterized by hyperactive, irregular electrical activity in the cardiovascular and central nervous systems (CNS), resulting in disrupted CNS–ANS interactions.

This is consistent with our result showing an association between increased ApEn and blunted VmPFC response to stress. Decreased VmPFC function has been associated with emotion dysregulation and alcohol-related, maladaptive coping behaviors including high craving and early relapse (Seo et al. 2013; Sinha et al. 2016). Specifically, our VmPFC finding was localized in the right, but not the left VmPFC. The right VmPFC is known to be more involved in the affective and social aspects (Tranel et al. 2002) of emotion regulation, whereas the left VmPFC assumes a greater role in the cognitive aspects (Leopold et al. 2012). This suggests that the inverse association between the rVmPFC and ApEn reflects emotional and autonomic dysregulation in AUD patients. Consistent with this, the rVmPFC has been shown to be involved in regulating emotion via ANS modulation (Hansel and von Kanel 2008), and right VmPFC lesions led to disruption of sympathetic inhibition during processing of stressful stimuli (Hilz et al. 2006), highlighting the role of rVmPFC in emotion- and stress-related ANS dysregulation in AUD patients.

Supporting this, our finding of the neural-ANS relationship was more stress-specific, as we did not detect significance in the alcohol-cue condition. This is discrepant with a study showing that altered RMSSD HRV response to alcohol cues is associated with decreased VmPFC activity in SD (Wang et al. 2020). This discrepancy may be explained by the nature of our sample, which is predominantly composed by AUD patients. Addictive drinking is accompanied by a transition from positive to negative reinforcement (Koob and Volkow 2010), where negative affect and stress-induced craving play more prominent roles (Ramchandani et al. 2018). It has been suggested that negative reinforcement results from an altered stress system, which is closely related to the central autonomic network that integrates autonomic information (Koob and Volkow 2010; Eddie et al. 2020). This may explain the association between ANS dysregulation and VmPFC response to stress found in our study. Consistent with this idea, our prior studies showed that blunted VmPFC response to stress in AUD patients predicts more frequent heavy drinking and faster alcohol relapse (Seo et al. 2013; Blaine et al. 2020), highlighting the importance of stress-related neural response in AUD severity. The current study extends these findings by showing an association between stress-related rVmPFC hypoactivity and disrupted ANS function in AUD patients. Hypoactivity in the ANS-governing rVmPFC may reflect an insufficient recruitment of neural resources during stress, leading to irregular ANS modulation and subsequent dysregulation of drinking behaviors. Supporting this, both blunted rVmPFC and increased ApEn were associated with increased alcohol consumption in our data, suggesting the importance of ANS regulation in an adaptive stress response and drinking behaviors.

The limitations for the current study include a small sample size. However, our AUD and SD groups are well-matched on demographics and intelligence. Thus, despite this limitation, our findings add to the AUD–ANS literature by reporting AUD-related differences in ApEn that may account for risky drinking and an altered neural response during stress. Given the preliminarry nature of our findings it is worthwhile to extend current findings in future studies with a larger sample size and further explore the utility of ApEn as an autonomic marker of AUD.

It should be noted that ApEn can be a difficult index to interpret, given the lack of literature regarding its exact role in HRV-related contexts and definition of healthy range. Signal complexity (e.g. ApEn) sometimes indicates a functional response, and both increased and decreased ApEn values have been associated with pathological states including decreased ApEn in schizophrenia (Bar et al. 2007) and increased basal ApEn in anxiety disorder (Caldirola et al. 2004). Given that ApEn is associated with other mental disorders (Caldirola et al. 2004; Bar et al. 2007; Byun et al. 2019), global ANS irregularity may be a transdiagnostic ANS signature also present in AUD. However, in our study, high ApEn was specifically associated with drinking behaviors and stress-related vmPFC response, suggesting that high ApEn may constitute an autonomic marker associated with stress-related drinking in AUD. Future studies, especially neuroimaging studies, should further clarify the role of ApEn in AUD pathology and its underlying neural mechanisms. Neuroimaging research should also explore other neural regions, such as the anterior cingulate cortex and other medial-prefrontal cortical regions, that are known to be modulated by breathing regulation (Bates et al. 2019).

Taken together, our findings suggest that high ApEn activity may serve as a novel index of ANS dysfunction associated with AUD, signifying irregular autonomic modulations, stress-related dysregulation, and increased susceptibility to hazardous drinking behavior. Our study also highlights the potential clinical utility of ApEn as a noninvasive, cost- and time-efficient ANS measure of AUD. ApEn may serve as an affordable index of AUD by detecting early AUD risk and alcohol-related cardiovascular problems in clinical care.

DATA AVAILABILITY

Data is available upon request by directly contacting the authors of this work.

Supplementary Material

Manuscript_ALC-21-0017_Supplemental_Information_agab064

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was supported by the following grants from National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism [R01-AA-13892, R01-AA026844, and K08AA023545] and Peter F. McManus Charitable Trust.

Contributor Information

Seungju Hwang, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

Jorge S Martins, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

Ryan J Douglas, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

Justin J Choi, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

Rajita Sinha, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

Dongju Seo, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519, USA.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT

No conflict of interest has been declared by any of our authors for this work.

References

  1. Bar KJ, Boettger MK, Koschke Met al. (2007) Non-linear complexity measures of heart rate variability in acute schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 118:2009–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bar KJ, Boettger MK, Schulz Set al. (2008) Reduced cardio-respiratory coupling in acute alcohol withdrawal. Drug Alcohol Depend 98:210–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bates ME, Lesnewich LM, Uhouse SGet al. (2019) Resonance-paced breathing alters neural response to visual cues: proof-of-concept for a neuroscience-informed adjunct to addiction treatments. Front Psych 10:624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blaine SK, Wemm S, Fogelman Net al. (2020) Association of prefrontal-striatal functional pathology with alcohol abstinence days at treatment initiation and heavy drinking after treatment initiation. Am J Psychiatry 177:1048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Breese GR, Sinha R, Heilig M. (2011) Chronic alcohol neuroadaptation and stress contribute to susceptibility for alcohol craving and relapse. Pharmacol Ther 129:149–71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Byun S, Kim AY, Jang EHet al. (2019) Entropy analysis of heart rate variability and its application to recognize major depressive disorder: a pilot study. Technol Health Care 27:407–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cahalan D, Cisin IH. (1968) American drinking practices: summary of findings from a national probability sample. II. Measurement of massed versus spaced drinking. Q J Stud Alcohol 29:642–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Caldirola D, Bellodi L, Cammino Set al. (2004) Smoking and respiratory irregularity in panic disorder. Biol Psychiatry 56:393–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Castillo-Carniglia A, Keyes KM, Hasin DSet al. (2019) Psychiatric comorbidities in alcohol use disorder. Lancet Psychiatry 6:1068–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cheng YC, Huang YC, Huang WL. (2019) Heart rate variability as a potential biomarker for alcohol use disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend 204:107502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Duncan JS, Papademetris X, Yang Jet al. (2004) Geometric strategies for neuroanatomic analysis from MRI. Neuroimage 23:S34–45. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Eddie D, Bates ME, Buckman JF. (2020) Closing the brain-heart loop: towards more holistic models of addiction and addiction recovery. Addict Biol e12958. Online ahead of print. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. First MB. (2015) Structured clinical interview for the DSM (SCID). Encycl Clin Psychology 1:3–6. [Google Scholar]
  14. Hansel A, Kanel R. (2008) The ventro-medial prefrontal cortex: a major link between the autonomic nervous system, regulation of emotion, and stress reactivity? Biopsychosoc Med 2:21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Hilz MJ, Devinsky O, Szczepanska Het al. (2006) Right ventromedial prefrontal lesions result in paradoxical cardiovascular activation with emotional stimuli. Brain 129:3343–55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Hiser J, Koenigs M. (2018) The multifaceted role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in emotion, decision making, social cognition, and psychopathology. Biol Psychiatry 83:638–47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Karmakar C, Udhayakumar RK, Li Pet al. (2017) Stability, consistency and performance of distribution entropy in analysing short length heart rate variability (HRV) signal. Front Physiol 8:720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Koob GF, Volkow ND. (2010) Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:217–38. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lane RD, McRae K, Reiman EMet al. (2009) Neural correlates of heart rate variability during emotion. Neuroimage 44:213–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. (2008) International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida. [Google Scholar]
  21. Leopold A, Krueger F, dal Monte Oet al. (2012) Damage to the left ventromedial prefrontal cortex impacts affective theory of mind. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7:871–80. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Napadow V, Dhond R, Conti Get al. (2008) Brain correlates of autonomic modulation: Combining heart rate variability with fMRI. Neuroimage 42:169–77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. NIAAA NIAAA. (2009) Rethinking Drinking. In Services, USDoHaHs. In Rethinking Drinking. Rockville, MD: NIH Publication #09–3770. [Google Scholar]
  24. Rajmohan V, Mohandas E. (2007) The limbic system. Indian J Psychiatry 49:132–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Ralevski E, Petrakis I, Altemus M. (2019) Heart rate variability in alcohol use: a review. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 176:83–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ramchandani VA, Stangl BL, Blaine SKet al. (2018) Stress vulnerability and alcohol use and consequences: from human laboratory studies to clinical outcomes. Alcohol 72:75–88. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TFet al. (1993) Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption--II. Addiction 88:791–804. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Seo D, Lacadie CM, Tuit Ket al. (2013) Disrupted ventromedial prefrontal function, alcohol craving, and subsequent relapse risk. JAMA Psychiat 70:727–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. (2017) An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health 5:258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Shi BZ, Yuan C, Wang Set al. (2017) Entropy analysis of short-term heartbeat interval time series during regular walking. Entropy 19:568. [Google Scholar]
  31. Shri, TKP, Sriraam, N., Bhat, V. (2014) Characterization of EEG Signals for Identification of Alcoholics Using ANOVA Ranked Approximate Entropy and Classifiers. International Conference on Circuits, Communication, Control and Computing. IEEE, New York, NY, 109–12.
  32. Sinha R. (2009) Modeling stress and drug craving in the laboratory: implications for addiction treatment development. Addict Biol 14:84–98. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Sinha R, Fox HC, Hong KAet al. (2009) Enhanced negative emotion and alcohol craving, and altered physiological responses following stress and cue exposure in alcohol dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology 34:1198–208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Sinha R, Lacadie CM, Constable RTet al. (2016) Dynamic neural activity during stress signals resilient coping. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 113:8837–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Sucharita S, Pradeep J, Vincent Aet al. (2012) Alcohol-dependence syndrome: postural challenge on heart rate variability. Indian J Psychiatry 54:253–6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Thayer JF, Ahs F, Fredrikson Met al. (2012) A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36:747–56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Tranel D, Bechara A, Denburg NL. (2002) Asymmetric functional roles of right and left ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional processing. Cortex 38:589–612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Wang W, Zhornitsky S, Le TMet al. (2020) Heart rate variability, cue-evoked ventromedial prefrontal cortical response, and problem alcohol use in adult drinkers. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging 5:619–28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Young H, Benton D. (2015) We should be using nonlinear indices when relating heart-rate dynamics to cognition and mood. Sci Rep 5:16619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Zachary RA, Paulson MJ, Gorsuch RL. (1985) Estimating WAIS IQ from the Shipley Institute of Living Scale using continuously adjusted age norms. J Clin Psychol 41:820–31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Manuscript_ALC-21-0017_Supplemental_Information_agab064

Data Availability Statement

Data is available upon request by directly contacting the authors of this work.


Articles from Alcohol and Alcoholism (Oxford, Oxfordshire) are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES