Table 3.
First author | Country | Assessment of pet ownership | Assessment of loneliness or social isolation | Study type | Sample characteristics | Results | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Sample description | Sample size | Age | Females in total sample (%) | ||||||
Adult population prior to the pandemic | |||||||||
Antonacopoulos (2010) [50] | Canada | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3) (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Individuals who are living alone | n = 132 |
M: 39.4 SD: 14.4 18–78 |
73.3 |
According to hierarchical regression, there was no significant association between pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) and loneliness However, pet ownership × social support was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (ß = – 0.32, p < 0.05) |
Antonacopoulos (2017) [49] | Canada | Having acquired a dog (dichotomous) |
Feelings of loneliness during the last week, rated on a four-point scale UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3) (20 items) |
Longitudinal (two waves during 8 months) | Individuals who are living in a town and do not have a dog at the baseline | n = 139 |
M: 36.8 SD: 14.4 18–68 |
64.0 |
Regarding the UCLA Loneliness Scale, acquiring a dog (ref.: not acquiring a dog) was not related to diverging levels of loneliness With respect to the single item, ANOVA revealed that acquiring a dog (ref.: not acquiring a dog) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (p < 0.05) |
Bennett (2015) [38] | Australia |
Pet ownership (dichotomous) Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) |
UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Community-dwelling individuals | n = 68 |
M: 71.6 SD: 5.6 65–80 |
72.1 | T tests revealed no significant differences between pet owners and non-pet owners, dog owners and non-dog owners, and cat owners and non-cat owners |
Branson (2019) [43] | United States | Cat ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Community-dwelling individuals without a dog | n = 96 |
M: 76.6 SD: 9.5 60–100 |
74.0 | Logistic regression did not detect loneliness as a significant covariate of cat ownership (ref.: non-cat ownership) |
Carr (2020) [44] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) × social loss (dichotomous) | Composite measure (UCLA Loneliness Scale, Health and Retirement Study Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire) (three items) | Longitudinal (three waves during 8 years) | Health and Retirement Study | n = 437 |
M: 65.6 SD: 10.1 37–88 |
56 | Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) did not significantly affect changes in loneliness following a social loss |
Enmarker (2015) [19] | Norway | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | Loneliness: four-point scale | Cross-sectional | Nord-Trøndelag Health Study | n = 12,093 |
M: 74.8 SD: 6.5 65–101 |
54.3 | “There was a slight difference in pet ownership in relation to loneliness: 16.5% of participants who indicated that they were lonely owned a pet compared with 18% of participants who indicated that they were not lonely.” |
Gulick (2012) [52] | United States |
Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) |
UCLA Loneliness Scale (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Individuals who own a dog or cat, utilize services for older people and can communicate in English | n = 159 |
55–72: 50.9% 73–84: 49.1% |
100.0 | There were no significant differences among loneliness between cat and dog owners |
Hajek (2020) [21] | Germany |
Dog ownership (dichotomous) cat ownership (dichotomous) |
Loneliness: De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (11 items) Social isolation: scale from Bude & Lantermann, 2006 (four items) |
Cross-sectional | German Ageing Survey | n = 1,160 |
M: 75.1 SD: 6.4 65–95 |
65.4 | Linear regression showed that dog ownership (ref.: not owning a pet) was related to decreased levels of social isolation (ß = – 0.16, p < 0.05) and loneliness (ß = – 0.12, p < 0.1). Cat ownership (ref.: not owning a pet) remained insignificant |
McConnell (2011) [53] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Community sample | n = 217 |
M: 31 SD not specified Range not specified |
79 | Regarding t tests, pet owners (ref.: non-pet ownership) had lower loneliness scores (p < 0.08) |
Pikhartova (2014) [39] | United Kingdom | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) | Longitudinal (five waves during 9 years) | English Longitudinal Study of Ageing | n = 5,210 |
M: 61.4 SD not specified Range not specified |
55.8 |
According to logistic regression, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with increased odds of loneliness in the cross-sectional analysis (OR: 1.24, 95% CI: 1.06–1.47) In the longitudinal analysis, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was also related to higher chances of loneliness (e.g., wave 0 to wave 5: OR: 1.31, 95% CI: 1.03–1.68) |
Powell (2018) [20] | Australia | Dog ownership (current or past or not) | Expectation that dog ownership would result in a decrease among loneliness | Cross-sectional | Potential dog owners | n = 3,465 |
18–44: 52.0% 45–64: 39.0% ≥ 65: 9.0% |
85.0 | According to logistic regression, current dog ownership (ref.: never owned a dog) was significantly related to higher expectations that a dog benefits to a decrease in loneliness (OR: 1.61, 95% CI: 1.19–2.20). Past dog ownership remained insignificant |
Rijken (2011) [48] | Netherlands |
Pet ownership (dichotomous) Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) Dog and cat ownership (dichotomous) Other pet ownership (no cats or dogs) (dichotomous) |
UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (six items) | Cross-sectional | National Panel of People with Chronic Illness or Disability | n = 1,410 |
M: 74.6 SD: 6.4 Range not specified |
60.0 | With respect to ANOVA, there were no significant differences between the different types of pet ownership among loneliness |
Stanley (2014) [18] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | Felts of loneliness during the last 2 weeks (dichotomized) | Cross-sectional | Primary care patients | n = 830 |
M: 72.2 SD: 8.3 Range not specified |
57.8 |
According to logistic regression, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was not significantly associated with loneliness Though, living alone x pet ownership was significantly related to decreased odds of loneliness (OR: 0.20, 95% CI: 0.08–0.50) |
Taniguchi (2018) [7] | Japan | Pet ownership (current or past vs. not) | Social isolation: having contact with others less than once a week | Cross-sectional | Ota Genki Senior Project | n = 11,233 |
65–74: 47.7% 75–84: 52.3% |
51.6 | Referring to mixed-effects cumulative logistic regression models, social isolation was related to decreased chances of current or past pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) (OR: 0.74, 95% CI: 0.66–0.80) |
Zasloff (1994) [17] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (number of items not specified) | Cross-sectional | Single students who do not live with a mate, a significant other, or children under the age of 18 | n = 148 |
M: 28.4 SD: 8.3 21–53 |
100.0 | There were no significant differences in loneliness among pet owners and non-pet owners |
Children/adolescent prior to the pandemic | |||||||||
Black (2012) [36] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (20 items) | Cross-sectional | Rural adolescents who visit public high schools | n = 293 |
M: 15.8 SD: 1.3 13–19 |
54.1 | An ANOVA showed that individuals with pets (ref.: non-pet ownership) had significantly lower loneliness scores (p < .001) |
Charmaraman (2020) [51] | United States |
Pet ownership (dichotomous) dog ownership (dichotomous) |
Social isolation: two items | Cross-sectional | Middle school students | n = 700 |
M: 12.7 SD not specified 11–16 |
52 | Social isolation was negatively associated with dog ownership (ß = – 0.23, p < 0.05, ref.: pet, but non-dog ownership), but not with pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) in general, according to regression analysis |
Mueller (2021) [45] | United States |
Pet ownership (dichotomous) Dog ownership (dichotomous) |
Three-point scale | Longitudinal (two waves in 10 months) | Adolescents visiting Middle schools | n = 1,033 |
M: 12.69 SD: 1.21 |
50 | Dog ownership (ref.: non-dog pet ownership) was related to decreased levels of loneliness (ß = – 0.1, p < 0.05), according to regression analysis |
Rhoades (2015) [37] | United States | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) | Cross-sectional | Homeless youth who utilize drop-in centers | n = 398 |
M: 21.3 SD: 2.1 Range not specified |
27.4 | Regarding Chi-square tests, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (p < .05) |
Adult population during the pandemic | |||||||||
Carr (2021) [22] | United States |
Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) |
Composite measure (UCLA Loneliness Scale, Health and Retirement Study Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire) (three items) | Longitudinal (two waves during 2 years) | Community-based sample | n = 473 |
M: 69.4 SD: 6.1 60–92 |
66.0 | According to the fully adjusted regression model, neither dog ownership (ref.: non-dog ownership) nor cat ownership (ref.: non-cat ownership) was significantly associated with loneliness |
Kogan (2021) [23] | Mostly United States |
Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) |
Loneliness: five-point scale Social isolation: five-point scale |
Cross-sectional | Dog or cat owners who participated in an online survey | n = 5,061 |
≤ 39: 30% 40–59: 43% ≥ 60: 27% |
89 |
Most of the pet owners reported that their pet would decrease their loneliness (66%) and their feelings of isolation (64%) Regarding binary regression, cat owners were less likely to feel isolated than dog owners (OR: 0.74, 95% CI: 0.64–0.86). Concerning loneliness, there no significant differences between these groups were revealed |
Oliva (2021) [46] | Australia |
Dog ownership (dichotomous) Cat ownership (dichotomous) |
UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) Loneliness during COVID-19 lockdown, rated on a four-point scale |
Cross-sectional | Individuals living alone | n = 384 |
M: 50.9 SD: 15.1 23–89 |
85.4 | Referring to hierarchical logistic regression, dog ownership (ref.: non-dog ownership) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness among both measures (e.g., UCLA Loneliness Scale: ß = – 0.71, p < 0.05). Cat ownership (vs. non-cat ownership) remained insignificant |
Phillipou (2021) [24] | Australia | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (number of items not specified) | Cross-sectional | Covid-19 and you: mentaL heaLth in AusTralia now survEy | n = 263 |
M: 25.1 SD: 14.2 range not specified |
84.2 | Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was not significantly related to loneliness |
Ratschen (2020) [47] | United Kingdom | Pet ownership (dichotomous) | UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) | Cross-sectional | General population | n = 5,926 |
18–24: 7.1% 25–34: 17.5% 35–44: 16.8% 45–54: 23.8% 55–64: 22.2% 65–70: 7.1% ≥ 70: 5.6% |
78.6 | Looking at linear regression models, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with a decreased height of loneliness (p < 0.01) |
Children/adolescent during the pandemic | |||||||||
Mueller (2021) [45] | United States |
Pet ownership (dichotomous) Dog ownership (dichotomous) |
Three-point scale | Longitudinal (two waves in 10 months) | Adolescents visiting Middle schools | n = 357 |
M: 12.69 SD: 1.21 |
50 |
Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was significantly associated with increased loneliness during COVID-19 (ß = 0.12, p < 0.05) Pet owners (ref.: non-pet owners) reported significantly higher increases in loneliness during COVID-19 (ß = 0.14, p < 0.01) |