Skip to main content
. 2022 Jul 11;57(10):1935–1957. doi: 10.1007/s00127-022-02332-9

Table 3.

Key findings

First author Country Assessment of pet ownership Assessment of loneliness or social isolation Study type Sample characteristics Results
Sample description Sample size Age Females in total sample (%)
Adult population prior to the pandemic
 Antonacopoulos (2010) [50] Canada Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3) (20 items) Cross-sectional Individuals who are living alone n = 132

M: 39.4

SD: 14.4

18–78

73.3

According to hierarchical regression, there was no significant association between pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) and loneliness

However, pet ownership × social support was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (ß = – 0.32, p < 0.05)

 Antonacopoulos (2017) [49] Canada Having acquired a dog (dichotomous)

Feelings of loneliness during the last week, rated on a four-point scale

UCLA Loneliness Scale (Version 3) (20 items)

Longitudinal (two waves during 8 months) Individuals who are living in a town and do not have a dog at the baseline n = 139

M: 36.8

SD: 14.4

18–68

64.0

Regarding the UCLA Loneliness Scale, acquiring a dog (ref.: not acquiring a dog) was not related to diverging levels of loneliness

With respect to the single item, ANOVA revealed that acquiring a dog (ref.: not acquiring a dog) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (p < 0.05)

 Bennett (2015) [38] Australia

Pet ownership (dichotomous)

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (20 items) Cross-sectional Community-dwelling individuals n = 68

M: 71.6

SD: 5.6

65–80

72.1 T tests revealed no significant differences between pet owners and non-pet owners, dog owners and non-dog owners, and cat owners and non-cat owners
 Branson (2019) [43] United States Cat ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (20 items) Cross-sectional Community-dwelling individuals without a dog n = 96

M: 76.6

SD: 9.5

60–100

74.0 Logistic regression did not detect loneliness as a significant covariate of cat ownership (ref.: non-cat ownership)
 Carr (2020) [44] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) × social loss (dichotomous) Composite measure (UCLA Loneliness Scale, Health and Retirement Study Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire) (three items) Longitudinal (three waves during 8 years) Health and Retirement Study n = 437

M: 65.6

SD: 10.1

37–88

56 Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) did not significantly affect changes in loneliness following a social loss
 Enmarker (2015) [19] Norway Pet ownership (dichotomous) Loneliness: four-point scale Cross-sectional Nord-Trøndelag Health Study n = 12,093

M: 74.8

SD: 6.5

65–101

54.3 “There was a slight difference in pet ownership in relation to loneliness: 16.5% of participants who indicated that they were lonely owned a pet compared with 18% of participants who indicated that they were not lonely.”
 Gulick (2012) [52] United States

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

UCLA Loneliness Scale (20 items) Cross-sectional Individuals who own a dog or cat, utilize services for older people and can communicate in English n = 159

55–72: 50.9%

73–84: 49.1%

100.0 There were no significant differences among loneliness between cat and dog owners
 Hajek (2020) [21] Germany

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

cat ownership (dichotomous)

Loneliness: De Jong Gierveld Loneliness Scale (11 items)

Social isolation: scale from Bude & Lantermann, 2006 (four items)

Cross-sectional German Ageing Survey n = 1,160

M: 75.1

SD: 6.4

65–95

65.4 Linear regression showed that dog ownership (ref.: not owning a pet) was related to decreased levels of social isolation (ß = – 0.16, p < 0.05) and loneliness (ß = – 0.12, p < 0.1). Cat ownership (ref.: not owning a pet) remained insignificant
 McConnell (2011) [53] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale (20 items) Cross-sectional Community sample n = 217

M: 31

SD not specified

Range not specified

79 Regarding t tests, pet owners (ref.: non-pet ownership) had lower loneliness scores (p < 0.08)
 Pikhartova (2014) [39] United Kingdom Pet ownership (dichotomous) Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) Longitudinal (five waves during 9 years) English Longitudinal Study of Ageing n = 5,210

M: 61.4

SD not specified

Range not specified

55.8

According to logistic regression, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with increased odds of loneliness in the cross-sectional analysis (OR: 1.24, 95% CI: 1.06–1.47)

In the longitudinal analysis, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was also related to higher chances of loneliness (e.g., wave 0 to wave 5: OR: 1.31, 95% CI: 1.03–1.68)

 Powell (2018) [20] Australia Dog ownership (current or past or not) Expectation that dog ownership would result in a decrease among loneliness Cross-sectional Potential dog owners n = 3,465

18–44: 52.0%

45–64: 39.0%

 ≥ 65: 9.0%

85.0 According to logistic regression, current dog ownership (ref.: never owned a dog) was significantly related to higher expectations that a dog benefits to a decrease in loneliness (OR: 1.61, 95% CI: 1.19–2.20). Past dog ownership remained insignificant
 Rijken (2011) [48] Netherlands

Pet ownership (dichotomous)

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

Dog and cat ownership (dichotomous)

Other pet ownership (no cats or dogs) (dichotomous)

UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (six items) Cross-sectional National Panel of People with Chronic Illness or Disability n = 1,410

M: 74.6

SD: 6.4

Range not specified

60.0 With respect to ANOVA, there were no significant differences between the different types of pet ownership among loneliness
 Stanley (2014) [18] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) Felts of loneliness during the last 2 weeks (dichotomized) Cross-sectional Primary care patients n = 830

M: 72.2

SD: 8.3

Range not specified

57.8

According to logistic regression, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was not significantly associated with loneliness

Though, living alone x pet ownership was significantly related to decreased odds of loneliness (OR: 0.20, 95% CI: 0.08–0.50)

 Taniguchi (2018) [7] Japan Pet ownership (current or past vs. not) Social isolation: having contact with others less than once a week Cross-sectional Ota Genki Senior Project n = 11,233

65–74: 47.7%

75–84: 52.3%

51.6 Referring to mixed-effects cumulative logistic regression models, social isolation was related to decreased chances of current or past pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) (OR: 0.74, 95% CI: 0.66–0.80)
 Zasloff (1994) [17] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (number of items not specified) Cross-sectional Single students who do not live with a mate, a significant other, or children under the age of 18 n = 148

M: 28.4

SD: 8.3

21–53

100.0 There were no significant differences in loneliness among pet owners and non-pet owners
Children/adolescent prior to the pandemic
 Black (2012) [36] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale Revised (20 items) Cross-sectional Rural adolescents who visit public high schools n = 293

M: 15.8

SD: 1.3

13–19

54.1 An ANOVA showed that individuals with pets (ref.: non-pet ownership) had significantly lower loneliness scores (p < .001)
 Charmaraman (2020) [51] United States

Pet ownership (dichotomous)

dog ownership (dichotomous)

Social isolation: two items Cross-sectional Middle school students n = 700

M: 12.7

SD not specified

11–16

52 Social isolation was negatively associated with dog ownership (ß = – 0.23, p < 0.05, ref.: pet, but non-dog ownership), but not with pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) in general, according to regression analysis
 Mueller (2021) [45] United States

Pet ownership (dichotomous)

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Three-point scale Longitudinal (two waves in 10 months) Adolescents visiting Middle schools n = 1,033

M: 12.69

SD: 1.21

50 Dog ownership (ref.: non-dog pet ownership) was related to decreased levels of loneliness (ß = – 0.1, p < 0.05), according to regression analysis
 Rhoades (2015) [37] United States Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) Cross-sectional Homeless youth who utilize drop-in centers n = 398

M: 21.3

SD: 2.1

Range not specified

27.4 Regarding Chi-square tests, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness (p < .05)
Adult population during the pandemic
 Carr (2021) [22] United States

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

Composite measure (UCLA Loneliness Scale, Health and Retirement Study Psychosocial and Lifestyle Questionnaire) (three items) Longitudinal (two waves during 2 years) Community-based sample n = 473

M: 69.4

SD: 6.1

60–92

66.0 According to the fully adjusted regression model, neither dog ownership (ref.: non-dog ownership) nor cat ownership (ref.: non-cat ownership) was significantly associated with loneliness
 Kogan (2021) [23] Mostly United States

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

Loneliness: five-point scale

Social isolation: five-point scale

Cross-sectional Dog or cat owners who participated in an online survey n = 5,061

 ≤ 39: 30%

40–59: 43%

 ≥ 60: 27%

89

Most of the pet owners reported that their pet would decrease their loneliness (66%) and their feelings of isolation (64%)

Regarding binary regression, cat owners were less likely to feel isolated than dog owners (OR: 0.74, 95% CI: 0.64–0.86). Concerning loneliness, there no significant differences between these groups were revealed

 Oliva (2021) [46] Australia

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Cat ownership (dichotomous)

UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items)

Loneliness during COVID-19 lockdown, rated on a four-point scale

Cross-sectional Individuals living alone n = 384

M: 50.9

SD: 15.1

23–89

85.4 Referring to hierarchical logistic regression, dog ownership (ref.: non-dog ownership) was associated with decreased levels of loneliness among both measures (e.g., UCLA Loneliness Scale: ß = – 0.71, p < 0.05). Cat ownership (vs. non-cat ownership) remained insignificant
 Phillipou (2021) [24] Australia Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale-Revised (number of items not specified) Cross-sectional Covid-19 and you: mentaL heaLth in AusTralia now survEy n = 263

M: 25.1

SD: 14.2

range not specified

84.2 Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was not significantly related to loneliness
 Ratschen (2020) [47] United Kingdom Pet ownership (dichotomous) UCLA Loneliness Scale (three items) Cross-sectional General population n = 5,926

18–24: 7.1%

25–34: 17.5%

35–44: 16.8%

45–54: 23.8%

55–64: 22.2%

65–70: 7.1%

 ≥ 70: 5.6%

78.6 Looking at linear regression models, pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was associated with a decreased height of loneliness (p < 0.01)
Children/adolescent during the pandemic
 Mueller (2021) [45] United States

Pet ownership (dichotomous)

Dog ownership (dichotomous)

Three-point scale Longitudinal (two waves in 10 months) Adolescents visiting Middle schools n = 357

M: 12.69

SD: 1.21

50

Pet ownership (ref.: non-pet ownership) was significantly associated with increased loneliness during COVID-19 (ß = 0.12, p < 0.05)

Pet owners (ref.: non-pet owners) reported significantly higher increases in loneliness during COVID-19 (ß = 0.14, p < 0.01)