Abstract
Purpose
Higher total fruit and vegetable (FV) intakes have been associated with lower perceived stress. The relationship between specific types of FV and perceived stress remains uncertain. The aims of this cross-sectional study were to explore the relationship between consumption of specific types of FV with perceived stress in a population-based cohort of men and women aged ≥ 25 years from the Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle (AusDiab) Study.
Methods
Dietary intake was assessed using a validated Food Frequency Questionnaire (n = 8,640). Perceived stress was evaluated using a validated Perceived Stress Questionnaire, with values ranging 0–1 (lowest to highest). High perceived stress cut-offs of ≥0.34 for men and ≥0.39 for women were obtained from the highest quartile of the perceived stress score for each sex. Multivariable-adjusted logistic regression was performed to investigate the associations.
Results
The mean age of participants (50.1% females) was 47.8 (SD 15) years. Persons in the highest, versus lowest, quartiles of apples and pears, orange and other citrus, and banana intakes had a significantly lower odds (24–31%) of having high perceived stress. Similarly, persons with higher intakes of cruciferous, yellow/orange/red, and legume vegetables had significantly lower odds (25–27%) of having high perceived stress.
Conclusion
In Australian adults, a higher consumption of apples and pears, oranges and other citrus, and bananas, as well as cruciferous, yellow/orange/red, and legume vegetables were associated with lower odds of having high perceived stress. The recommendations of “eating a rainbow” of colours may assist in preventing and/or reducing perceived stress.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00394-022-02848-5.
Keywords: AusDiab, Australian adults, Perceived stress, Types of fruit and vegetable intake
Introduction
Perceived stress can be defined as the thoughts and feelings an individual has about how much stress they experience in response to stressful life events [1]. For this manuscript, the term “stress” will be used to represent “perceived stress” for simplicity. Whereas some stress is part of humans life, long-term elevated stress can adversely affect health and lead to mental [2] and physical disorders [3], such as depression [4] and cardiovascular disease (CVD) [5, 6]. Mental health problems affect 1 in 5 people worldwide [7]. In the United States, mental health problems are estimated to cost the global economy over US$1 trillion each year [8]. Therefore, there is a need for public health strategies to prevent and/or reduce stress levels.
In addition to the benefits of an active lifestyle, growing evidence indicates that diet [9, 10], in particular high consumption of fruit and vegetables (FV), can positively impact mental health [11]. Findings from a systematic review [12] that included 61 observational studies showed a positive relationship between FV intake and several mental health-related outcomes, such as depression, anxiety, stress, mood, self-esteem, creativity, and quality of life. However, to date, only a few studies have investigated the association of specific types of FV [12] with mental health [13–17], with only one study considering stress [13]. Noteworthy, due to the varying nutrient composition of different types of FV, it is possible that specific fruits and/or vegetables may provide additional benefits. To date, however, this remains unknown. For example, specific fruit and/or vegetable types may be richer sources of some vitamins, minerals, carotenoids and flavonoids [18], which may potentially alleviate stress levels [19].
Further studies investigating whether specific FV types may offer additional benefits for perceived stress are warranted. It is noteworthy that studies in this area have typically focussed on depression and other mental health issues [12], and had a relatively small number of participants within specific demographics (e.g. middle-aged persons, students) [13–17]. Therefore, a broader and more representative sample of the adult lifespan should be explored. Previously, we demonstrated an association between higher total FV consumption and lower perceived stress in the Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study (AusDiab) cohort [20]. However, it remains unclear if specific types of fruits and/or vegetables may provide greater benefits when considering perceived stress. Therefore, the aims of this study were to further investigate the cross-sectional associations of FV types with perceived stress, in this cohort of Australian adults.
Materials and methods
Study population
Participants included in this study were part of the AusDiab study, a national population-based survey of men and women aged ≥ 25 years, recruited in 1999–2000 from 42 randomly selected census collector districts across Australia. Detailed methods have been published elsewhere [21]. A total of 20,347 Australian adults completed a household interview at baseline (1999–2000), and 11,247 of those attended a clinical examination (5,049 men; 6,198 women) in the original cohort study. For the present cross-sectional study, those with missing data for exposures, outcome and confounding variables were excluded. This comprised people who did not complete the baseline perceived stress questionnaire (n = 1,697) and the food frequency questionnaire (FFQ, n = 148), as well as individuals with at least one missing value for confounding factors (n = 436), those with implausible energy intake (<3300 or >17,500 kJ in men and <2,500 kJ or >14,500 kJ in women) (n = 275) [22], and pregnant women (n = 51). In total, 8,640 participants were included in the present study, after excluding missing data (n = 2,607). A flow diagram is shown in Online Resource 1.
Perceived stress assessment
A validated 30-item Perceived Stress Questionnaire (PSQ) [23] was used to assess perceived stress levels. Participants reported their feelings related to stressful situations (i.e., “you feel rested”, “you feel that too many demands are made on you”, “you are irritable or grouchy”) over the previous 12 months. Response options ranged from 1 to 4 on a four-point Likert scale. All positive questions (Questions 1, 7, 10, 13, 17, 21, 25, 29), such as “you feel rested”, were reverse scored (i.e., 1 [“usually”] to 4 [“almost never”]) and all other [negative] questions were scored from 1 (“almost never”) to 4 (“usually”). A perceived stress index was calculated from the raw scores which ranged from a minimum of 30 to a maximum of 120, using the formula: perceived stress index = (raw score – 30)/90 [23], with a higher index indicating greater perceived stress levels. As men are known to have lower perceived stress than women [24], individuals with high perceived stress were identified based on a specific cut-point derived from participants in the highest quartile of the perceived stress for each sex (e.g. high perceived stress; ≥0.34 and ≥0.39 for men and for women, respectively).
Dietary intake
Dietary intake was assessed using a validated 74 food item FFQ developed by the Cancer Council of Victoria [25], with participants recalling their usual intakes over the previous 12 months. Nutrient intakes were estimated based on frequency of consumption and an estimation of usual serving size. Energy intake (kcal/day) was calculated based on individuals total reported food consumption [25]. Alcohol consumption was assessed using the questions about consumption of alcoholic beverages [25], with frequency of alcohol intake ranging from ‘never’ to ‘every day’.
Fruit and vegetable types
Fruit types were grouped into three different categories [26]: apples and pears; oranges and other citrus fruits; and bananas. These are the most common fruit consumed in Australia [27]. Fruit juice, tinned and dried fruit were not included as these types of fruit should be consumed only occasionally as substitutes for fresh fruit, according to The Australian Dietary Guidelines [28]. The reason for this is the high sugar content of dried fruit and some fruit juices, as well as the absence of dietary fibre in juices [28]. Types of vegetables were classified into five groups according to their phytochemical properties [29–34] and based on the Australian Dietary Guidelines [28]: cruciferous vegetables (cabbage, Brussels sprouts, cauliflower, and broccoli); allium vegetables (onion, leek, and garlic); yellow/orange/red vegetables (tomato, capsicum, carrot, beetroot and pumpkin); leafy green vegetables (lettuce and other salad greens, celery, silver beet, and spinach); and legumes (peas, greens beans, bean sprouts and alfalfa sprouts, baked beans, soy beans, soy bean curd and tofu, and other beans). “Potato, roasted and fried, including hot chips” was excluded as these are not considered part of a healthy diet. This method of classifying vegetable types has been used extensively [35–38]. Intake for each FV type was calculated (g/day) and subsequently categorised into quartiles. Vegetable diversity (number of different vegetables consumed daily) was obtained from the question “How many different vegetables do you usually eat in a day?”, as described in previous studies [39–41].
Baseline demographic and clinical assessment
Demographic information was collected at a household interview and included age (date of birth), sex (male/female), relationship status (de facto, married, separated, divorced, widowed, never married), education level (never to some high school, completed University or equivalent) and average weekly income (according to six categories: $0–199, $200–399, $400–599, $600–799, $800–1499, and >$1500). Area-based socio-economic status was assessed based on the 5-yearly census [42] from 1999, using the socio-economic index for areas (SEIFA) which takes into consideration the social and economic conditions per geographic area.
Following the household interview, anthropometric assessments were performed at the clinic visits [21, 43] including: height, measured using a stadiometer to the nearest 0.5 cm without shoes; and weight, measured using a mechanical beam balance without shoes and excess clothing to the nearest 0.1 kg [44]. Body mass index (BMI) was obtained by dividing weight (kg) by height in squared metres.
The Active Australia Survey Questionnaire was used to assess physical activity in the previous 7 days (total minutes per week) [45]. Total physical activity (min/week) was obtained by summing the total walking time (if continuous for ≥10 min) and/or performing moderate-intensity exercise, plus the time performing vigorous-intensity exercise (this was multiplied by two) and then added to the time [41].
An interviewer-administered general questionnaire was used to assess smoking status [21], with participants being classified as current smokers (smoking at least daily), ex-smokers (smoked less than daily for at least the last 3 months), or never smokers (smoked < 100 cigarettes during life) [46].
Glucose tolerance status was based on self-report of diagnosed diabetes mellitus (DM) and on plasma glucose levels, and was categorised as known DM, undiagnosed DM, impaired fasting glucose, impaired glucose tolerance, normal glucose levels. Prevalent CVD was assessed using self-reported history of CVD (yes/no) [47]. These variables were included as confounding factors in the model due to the likelihood of individuals with diabetes and a history of CVD to have been counselled on diet and lifestyle, and therefore may have changed their dietary habits.
To further explore whether diet quality could be a confounding factor in the present study, a Dietary Guideline Index (DGI) [48] was included in the analyses. The DGI includes 15 components based on the Australian Dietary Guidelines [49, 50] as previously reported [48]. Each component was given a score ranging from 0 to 10 points (from lowest to optimal intakes), with total scores ranging from 0 to 150. Higher scores indicate greater adherence to the dietary guidelines and better diet quality [48].
Statistical analysis
Statistical significance was set at p ≤ 0.05 (two-tailed) for all tests. Statistical analyses were performed in Stata (Stata Statistical Software: Release 15. College Station, TX: StataCorp LLC) [51] using the suite of survey commands. The main exposures of this study were intakes of the eight types of FV (continuous [per SD] and categorical [quartiles]) and the primary outcome was the perceived stress index. Logistic regression commands for complex survey designs were performed to investigate the association between the exposures and high perceived stress (highest quartiles in men and women). Linear regression commands for complex survey designs were used to test for differences in perceived stress (continuous) with the exposures of interest. The complex survey commands adjusts standard errors and incorporates stratification weights thereby providing population-level estimates [21]. Two models of adjustment were used in the statistical analysis for each of the above analyses: (1) unadjusted; and (2) adjusted for age, sex, BMI, energy intake, relationship status, physical activity levels, level of education, SEIFA, smoking status, self-reported history of CVD, and diagnosis of diabetes based on plasma glucose levels. Participants with missing data for exposure, outcome and confounding variables were excluded from the analyses.
Sensitivity and interaction analyses
In the sensitivity analyses, DGI was added to the multivariable-adjusted model to further explore whether a healthy diet could be a confounding factor. When a specific fruit type was associated with lower odds of high perceived stress, we also considered a model where we adjusted for ‘all other fruit’ in the multivariable-adjusted logistic regression model. A similar analysis was performed for vegetable types. Interaction terms for FV types vs. age and FV types vs. sex were assessed to investigate potential effect modification for perceived stress in multivariable-adjusted models, with both FV types and perceived stress index entered as continuous variables.
Additional analysis
The Spearman rank correlation test (rs) for non-parametric variables was used to assess the correlation between vegetable diversity (number of different vegetables consumed per day) and intake (total amount in g/day). We sought to assess if vegetable diversity was associated with high perceived stress, independent of total vegetable intake. Variable inflation factors (VIF) were examined in the aforementioned multivariable-adjusted model to assess for collinearity.
Results
Baseline characteristics
Table 1 shows the baseline characteristics of the 8,640 participants in total and by stress levels (lower vs. high stress). A total of 50.1% (n = 4,747) were women and the mean (±SD) age of participants was 47.8 ± 15 years. Approximately, 16.3% were current smokers and 6.7% had a history of CVD. Normal glucose levels were present in 77.1% of the participants. Average perceived stress index was 0.28 ± 0.16 (mean ± SD). Perceived stress was 0.20 ± 0.10 in the three lowest stress quartiles, and 0.49 ± 0.10 in the high stress group. Participants with higher stress levels tended to be younger, less physically active with a higher energy intake and higher levels of education (all p < 0.05) compared to the lower stress groups. Relationship status, smoking habits, history of CVD and diabetes were also significantly different between the groups with high and lower stress levels (all p < 0.01).
Table 1.
Clinical and demographic characteristics of all participants and by perceived stress
All | Lower stress | High stress | |
---|---|---|---|
n = 8640 | n = 6456 | n = 2184 | |
Age (years) | 47.8 ± 15.0 | 49.7 ± 16.0 | 43.0 ± 11.1 |
Sex (women), n (%) | 4747 (50.1) | 3601 (51.7) | 1146 (46.3) |
BMI (kg/m2) | 26.6 ± 4.8 | 26.6 ± 4.9 | 26.6 ± 4.5 |
Energy intake (kcal/d) | 2042 ± 697 | 2002 ± 704 | 2143 ± 669 |
Physical activity (min/week) | 277 ± 337 | 287 ± 347 | 250 ± 311 |
Relationship status, n (%) | |||
Married | 6268 (72.8) | 4722 (73.2) | 1546 (71.7) |
De facto | 414 (4.4) | 286 (4.1) | 128 (5.1) |
Separated | 217 (2.4) | 132 (1.9) | 85 (3.7) |
Divorced | 515 (5.1) | 357 (4.7) | 158 (6.1) |
Widowed | 528 (5.2) | 468 (6.4) | 60 (2.2) |
Single | 698 (10.0) | 491 (9.6) | 207 (11.1) |
SEIFA | 1025 ± 87 | 1026 ± 87 | 1023 ± 86 |
Level of education, n (%) | |||
Never to some high school | 3445 (35.7) | 2688 (37.7) | 757 (30.7) |
Completed university/equivalent | 5195 (64.3) | 3768 (62.3) | 1427 (69.3) |
Smoking status, n (%) | |||
Current | 1338 (16.3) | 921 (15.1) | 417 (19.3) |
Ex-smoker | 2537 (25.9) | 1938 (26.4) | 599 (24.5) |
Non-smoker | 4765 (57.8) | 3597 (58.5) | 1168 (56.2) |
History of CVD, n (%) | 685 (6.7) | 552 (7.5) | 133 (4.5) |
Prevalence of diabetes, n (%) | |||
Known diabetes | 348 (3.3) | 284 (3.9) | 64 (2.0) |
Undiagnosed diabetes | 350 (3.5) | 284 (3.9) | 66 (2.4) |
Impaired fasting glucose | 512 (5.8) | 387 (5.7) | 125 (5.9) |
Impaired glucose tolerance | 1043 (10.3) | 818 (10.7) | 225 (9.3) |
Normal glucose levels | 6387 (77.1) | 4683 (75.8) | 1704 (80.4) |
Perceived stress index | 0.28 ± 0.16 | 0.20 ± 0.10 | 0.49 ± 0.10 |
Estimated using the survey command to apply the necessary weighting for selection bias. Values are mean ± SD unless otherwise stated. Lower stress represents Q1 to Q3; High stress represents Q4
BMI Body Mass Index, CVD Cardiovascular Disease, SEIFA Socio-Economical Index For Areas
Associations of FV type intakes with high perceived stress
Compared to participants with the lowest intakes, those with the highest intakes of apples and pears, orange and citrus fruits, and bananas had a statistically significant 31, 25, and 24% lower odds, respectively, of having high perceived stress (Table 2). Similarly, each SD increase in consumption of apples (78 g/d) and bananas (35 g/d) was associated with 11 and 8% lower odds for perceived stress, respectively (Table 2). Mean perceived stress index was significantly lower in the higher intake quartiles of apples and pears, oranges and other citrus, and bananas (Online Resource 2).
Table 2.
Odds ratios (OR) for high perceived stress by quartiles of specific types of fruit
Per SD | Fruit intake quartiles | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q1 | Q2 | Q3 | Q4 | ||
Apples and pears | 76 g increase | n = 2162 | n = 2165 | n = 2153 | n = 2160 |
Average intake (g/day) | 7 (7, 8) | 28 (27, 28) | 66 (65, 66) | 168 (162, 174) | |
High stress, n (%) | 599 (34%) | 582 (29%) | 510 (25%) | 453 (25%) | |
Model 11 | 0.86 (0.79, 0.94) | Ref | 0.81 (0.59, 1.13) | 0.64 (0.48, 0.86) | 0.64 (0.51, 0.81) |
Model 22 | 0.89 (0.81, 0.97) | Ref | 0.76 (0.52, 1.10) | 0.68 (0.49, 0.94) | 0.69 (0.53, 0.90) |
Orange and other citrus | 50 g increase | n = 2162 | n = 2165 | n = 2153 | n = 2160 |
Average intake (g/day) | 2 (2, 2) | 12 (12, 12) | 34 (33, 34) | 112 (108, 116) | |
High stress, n (%) | 600 (31%) | 610 (30%) | 549 (29%) | 425 (23%) | |
Model 11 | 0.90 (0.83, 0.97) | Ref | 0.98 (0.81, 1.19) | 0.93 (0.74, 1.18) | 0.68 (0.56, 0.82) |
Model 22 | 0.95 (0.89, 1.02) | Ref | 0.88 (0.71, 1.01) | 0.90 (0.71, 1.14) | 0.75 (0.64, 0.89) |
Bananas | 35 g increase | n = 2166 | n = 2161 | n = 2155 | n = 2158 |
Average intake (g/day) | 4 (4, 4) | 16 (16, 16) | 36 (36, 37) | 84 (82, 86) | |
High stress, n (%) | 620 (33%) | 604 (31%) | 530 (27%) | 430 (22%) | |
Model 11 | 0.83 (0.76, 0.90) | Ref | 0.89 (0.71, 1.13) | 0.74 (0.64, 0.85) | 0.59 (0.51, 0.68) |
Model 22 | 0.92 (0.85, 0.98) | Ref | 0.93 (0.74, 1.19) | 0.86 (0.72, 1.03) | 0.76 (0.64, 0.89) |
Odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) obtained using the survey command for logistic regression. Fruit intake (g/d) is shown as mean and 95% CI. Lower stress represents Q1 to Q3; High stress represents Q4. Analyses were adjusted according to the following models: model 11, unadjusted; and model 22, multivariable-adjusted (confounding factors included age, sex, BMI [body mass index], energy intake, relationship status, physical activity, level of education, SEIFA [Socio-economical index for areas], smoking status, diabetes and history of cardiovascular disease). Numbers in bold are significantly different from Q1 (p < 0.05)
The associations between types of vegetables and high perceived stress are shown in Table 3. Compared to participants with the lowest intakes, those with the highest intakes of cruciferous, yellow/orange/red vegetables, and higher consumption of legumes (Q3) had a statistically significant 25, 27 and 26% lower odds of high perceived stress, respectively. Likewise, each SD increase in intake of cruciferous vegetables (10 g/d) and yellow/orange/red vegetables (25 g/d) was associated with 12 and 11% lower odds for perceived stress, respectively (Table 3). The mean perceived stress index was significantly lower in those with higher intake of cruciferous, yellow/orange/red vegetables and legume vegetables (Online Resource 3).
Table 3.
Odds ratios (OR) for high perceived stress by quartiles of specific types of vegetables
Per SD | Vegetable intake quartiles | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Q1 | Q2 | Q3 | Q4 | ||
Cruciferous vegetables | 10 g increase | n = 2166 | n = 2161 | n = 2158 | n = 2155 |
Average intake (g/day) | 5 (5, 5) | 15 (15, 15) | 26 (26, 27) | 51 (50, 52) | |
High stress, n (%) | 610 (31%) | 592 (31%) | 520 (27%) | 462 (23%) | |
Model 11 | 0.85 (0.77, 0.93) | Ref | 1.01 (0.84, 1.22) | 0.84 (0.69, 1.02) | 0.67 (0.52, 0.87) |
Model 22 | 0.88 (0.80, 0.97) | Ref | 1.10 (0.90, 1.36) | 0.91 (0.74, 1.13) | 0.75 (0.57, 0.99) |
Allium vegetables | 6 g increase | n = 2210 | n = 2114 | n = 2167 | n = 2149 |
Average intake (g/day) | 1 (1, 1) | 4 (4, 4) | 7 (7, 7) | 15 (14, 15) | |
High stress, n (%) | 577 (29%) | 511 (28%) | 544 (26%) | 552 (30%) | |
Model 11 | 1.00 (0.89, 1.13) | Ref | 0.95 (0.75, 1.20) | 0.86 (0.68, 1.10) | 1.05 (0.76, 1.47) |
Model 22 | 0.95 (0.84, 1.07) | Ref | 0.90 (0.71, 1.13) | 0.80 (0.62, 1.02) | 0.90 (0.64, 1.27) |
Yellow/orange/red vegetables | 25 g increase | n = 2161 | n = 2160 | n = 2159 | n = 2160 |
Average intake (g/day) | 15 (14, 16) | 32 (31, 32) | 46 (45, 46) | 76 (74, 77) | |
High stress, n (%) | 579 (31%) | 558 (29%) | 535 (28%) | 512 (25%) | |
Model 11 | 0.90 (0.83, 0.98) | Ref | 0.92 (0.75, 1.13) | 0.86 (0.65, 1.14) | 0.76 (0.63, 0.93) |
Model 22 | 0.89 (0.83, 0.96) | Ref | 0.95 (0.77, 1.17) | 0.86 (0.67, 1.11) | 0.73 (0.61, 0.88) |
Leafy green vegetables | 10 g increase | n = 2168 | n = 2165 | n = 2151 | n = 2156 |
Average intake (g/day) | 4 (3, 4) | 9 (9, 9) | 15 (15, 15) | 27 (27, 28) | |
High stress, n (%) | 606 (31%) | 569 (28%) | 542 (28%) | 467 (25%) | |
Model 11 | 0.92 (0.85, 1.00) | Ref | 0.85 (0.69, 1.04) | 0.87 (0.75, 1.01) | 0.73 (0.57, 0.92) |
Model 22 | 0.98 (0.90, 1.07) | Ref | 0.86 (0.70, 1.05) | 0.93 (0.78, 1.11) | 0.85 (0.67, 1.08) |
Legume vegetables | 21 g increase | n = 2176 | n = 2154 | n = 2155 | n = 2155 |
Average intake (g/day) | 9 (9, 9) | 21 (20, 21) | 33 (32, 33) | 59 (58, 60) | |
High stress, n (%) | 599 (31%) | 554 (29%) | 488 (25%) | 543 (29%) | |
Model 11 | 0.95 (0.88, 1.04) | Ref | 0.90 (0.70, 1.16) | 0.73 (0.56, 0.96) | 0.92 (0.75, 1.13) |
Model 22 | 0.93 (0.84, 1.02) | Ref | 0.94 (0.76, 1.16) | 0.74 (0.59, 0.92) | 0.86 (0.70, 1.05) |
Odds ratios (ORs) obtained using the survey command for logistic regression. Vegetable intake (g/d) is shown as mean and 95% confidence intervals. Lower stress represents Q1 to Q3; High stress represents Q4. Analyses were adjusted according to the following models: model 11, unadjusted; and model 22, multivariable-adjusted (confounding factors included age, sex, BMI [body mass index], energy intake, relationship status, physical activity, level of education, SEIFA [Socio-economical index for areas], smoking status, diabetes and history of cardiovascular disease). Numbers in bold are significantly different from Q1 (p < 0.05)
Sensitivity and interaction analyses
To explore whether a healthy diet could be a confounding factor, we further adjusted the analyses for the DGI. Similar relationships were observed for types of FV intake and lower odds of high perceived stress (Q4 for apples and pears: OR [95% CI]: 0.72 [0.54, 0.95], orange and other citrus fruits: 0.77 [0.65, 0.92], bananas: 0.78 [0.68, 0.91], cruciferous: 0.77 [0.59, 1.01], yellow/orange/red vegetables: 0.78 [0.62, 0.92], and Q3 for legume vegetables: 0.75 [0.60, 0.94], compared to Q1).
We also further adjusted each fruit groups for other fruit groups (for example, the association of apples and pear with perceived stress was further adjusted for other non-apple and pears fruit). The relationship of all fruit types with lower odds of high perceived stress remained significant (Q4 for apples and pears: OR [95% CI]: 0.68 [0.48, 0.96], orange and other citrus fruit: 0.78 [0.64, 0.95], and bananas: 0.78 [0.65, 0.92]). Similarly, for vegetable types, we added other vegetables to the multivariable-adjusted model. Whereas the relationship of legumes and perceived stress remained significant (Q3 for legume vegetables: 0.80 [0.65, 0.98]), compared to Q1, the association of cruciferous and yellow/orange/red vegetables became non-significant (Q4: 0.85 [0.62, 1.16] and 0.89 [0.75, 1.05]), respectively, compared to Q1.
In separate interaction testing, sex and age did not influence the relationship between types of FV and perceived stress (sex: all pinteraction > 0.1, except pinteraction > 0.06 for orange and other citrus fruit and allium vegetables; age: all pinteraction > 0.1).
Further analyses
There was a high correlation between vegetable diversity and total vegetable intake (rho = 0.8, p = 0.001), and when both variables were included in multivariable-adjusted models, high variable inflation factors (2.8–3.0) were recorded suggesting evidence for collinearity indicating that any association of vegetable diversity with lower odds of high perceived stress was largely related to the association resulting from total vegetable intake.
Discussion
We have previously reported that higher FV intake is associated with lower perceived stress in adults aged ≥ 25 years participating in the AusDiab study [20]. We have now extended these findings and demonstrate that consuming specific types of fruit and vegetables are associated with lower odds of having high perceived stress. The groups with the highest consumption of apples and pears, oranges and other citrus, and bananas had 31, 25, and 24% lower risk of having high perceived stress, respectively, compared to those with the lowest intakes. For vegetables, cruciferous, yellow/orange/red and legumes were the main types driving the associations, with greater consumption being associated with 25, 27, and 26% lower odds of having high perceived stress. These relationships were robust and independent of a range of confounding lifestyle factors including a healthy diet and physical activity.
Prolonged stress is considered a risk factor for mental health problems such as anxiety and depression [1, 52], and modifiable risk factors, such as a healthy diet appear to be beneficial for mental wellbeing [12]. Although increasing evidence supports the existence of an inverse link between consumption of FV and mental health problems [12], the mechanisms to explain this relationship remain unclear. The protective role of fruit and vegetables on stress levels could be due to a range of their constituents, including minerals, vitamins, and other phytochemicals. Various phytochemicals found in specific FV have anti-oxidative and anti-inflammatory properties (i.e. carotenoids, vitamin K1 and flavonoids) that could potentially alleviate stress levels [19], contributing to the overall health benefits [53], including perceived stress. Apples and pears, and oranges and other citrus fruit, for example, are rich sources of flavonoids, dietary fibre, vitamins, carotenoids, and other minerals that seem to reduce inflammation and oxidative stress [54]. Likewise, the health benefits of bananas may be linked to their high content of tryptophan [55]; an essential amino acid (3) and precursor of serotonin, a neurotransmitter in the brain modulating mood [56].
For vegetable intake, similar associations were observed, where higher consumption of cruciferous, yellow/orange/red and legumes were associated with less perceived stress. These types of vegetables are rich in fibre, polyphenols and carotenoids which are linked to improvements in oxidative stress and inflammation [57]. Considering those with mental health problems have been shown to have higher oxidative stress and systemic inflammation [58], having a diverse intake of FV is likely to provide greater benefits due to a broader range of nutrients [59].
Of interest, the relationship of both cruciferous and yellow/orange/red vegetables with lower odds of high perceived stress was no longer evident after controlling for other vegetables (i.e. for cruciferous vegetables when other non-cruciferous vegetables were added to the model). These results add weight to the proposition that consuming a diverse range of vegetables is likely to have a positive influence on perceived stress. In contrast, for fruit, the associations of all fruit groups with lower odds of high perceived stress were independent of other fruit. This suggest that specific fruit such as apples and pears, oranges and other citrus, and bananas might be most beneficial. Collectively, such findings are similar to previous research considering other mental health outcomes [13].
A diverse consumption of FV has also been associated with better mental health in previous studies [13, 14]. Young women consuming lower amounts of citrus (OR [95%CI]: 3.14 [1.34–7.38] and green leafy (OR [95%CI]: 3.84 [2.05–7.19]) vegetables were more likely to present depressive symptoms [13]. In young adults, consumption of raw FV predicted lower depressive symptoms and better positive mood, life satisfaction, and flourishing [14]. This study identified the top 10 FV linked with improved mental health were bananas, apples, citrus, berries, grapefruit, kiwifruit, carrots, lettuce, cucumber, and green leafy vegetables (mainly spinach) [14]. Hence, the current daily recommendation for FV intake, whilst including a range of different types of FV (all colours of the rainbow) is likely to be most beneficial to alleviate stress, and consequently stress-related mental health problems. Future studies investigating the potential association between FV constituents (i.e., flavonoids) and perceived stress are needed to clarify the likely mechanism(s) for future interventions.
Limitations and strengths of the study
Due to the cross-sectional nature of the study, we were unable to demonstrate direct causality or to make firm conclusions about the direction of the relationship. Since we also did not measure biomarkers of stress, such as cortisol, we were unable to confirm a relationship of FV types with such biomarkers. In addition, since vegetable diversity and total vegetable intake were very highly correlated, we were unable to determine if the beneficial associations of higher vegetable intake and greater vegetable variety with lower perceived stress were independent of one another.
Strengths of this study include the availability of information on intake of specific types of fruit and vegetables, the use of validated questionnaires collect data on dietary intake and perceived stress, and the adjustment for numerous demographic and lifestyle covariates to limit the influence of potential confounders. Future longitudinal studies are required to explore the causal directions of the observed associations, and to clarify their potential mechanisms.
Conclusion
In Australian adults, higher consumption of specific types of FV was associated with lower odds for high perceived stress. These findings were independent of important potential confounding factors such as diet quality and physical activity. Specifically, we provide evidence for the benefits of specific types of FV (i.e. apples and pears, oranges and citrus, bananas, and cruciferous, yellow/orange/red and legume vegetables) when considering perceived stress. Based on these results, public health messages should continue to promote higher FV intake whilst also highlighting the importance of including ‘a rainbow of colours’ as part of a healthy diet.
Supplementary Information
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The AusDiab study, initiated and coordinated by the International Diabetes Institute, and subsequently coordinated by the Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute, gratefully acknowledges the support and assistance given by: A Allman, B Atkins, S Bennett, S Chadban, S Colagiuri, M de Courten, M Dalton, M D’Emden, T Dwyer, D Jolley, I Kemp, P Magnus, J Mathews, D McCarty, A Meehan, K O’Dea, P Phillips, P Popplewell, C Reid, A Stewart, R Tapp, H Taylor, T Welborn, F Wilson and P Zimmet. Also, for funding or logistical support, we are grateful to: The Commonwealth Dept of Health and Aged Care, Abbott Australasia Pty Ltd, Alphapharm Pty Ltd, AstraZeneca, Aventis Pharmaceutical, Bristol-Myers Squibb Pharmaceuticals, Eli Lilly (Aust) Pty Ltd, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen-Cilag (Aust) Pty Ltd, Merck Lipha s.a., Merck Sharp & Dohme (Aust), Novartis Pharmaceutical (Aust) Pty Ltd., Novo Nordisk Pharmaceutical Pty Ltd, Pharmacia and Upjohn Pty Ltd, Pfizer Pty Ltd, Roche Diagnostics, Sanofi Synthelabo (Aust) Pty Ltd., Servier Laboratories (Aust) Pty Ltd, BioRad Laboratories Pty Ltd, HITECH Pathology Pty Ltd, the Australian Kidney Foundation, Diabetes Australia, Diabetes Australia (Northern Territory), Queensland Health, South Australian Department of Human Services, Tasmanian Department of Health and Human Services, Territory Health Services, Victorian Department of Human Services, the Victorian OIS program and Health Department of Western Australia.
Authors’ contributions
SRB and JRL designed this cross-sectional study; DJM, JES and RMD provided data collected in the original study. SRB, MS and NPB performed the statistical analysis; SRB drafted and had primary responsibility for final content of the manuscript. SRB, MS, LCB, NPB, CPB, RW, JMD, DJM, JES, RMD, JMH, JRL interpreted the results, had significant input, reviewed, and approved the final manuscript.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions. This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial or not-for-profit sectors. The salary of MS and CPB is supported by a Royal Perth Hospital Research Foundation Career Advancement Fellowship. The salary of LCB is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) of Australia Emerging Leadership Investigator Grant (ID: 1172987) and a National Heart Foundation of Australia Post-Doctoral Research Fellowship (ID: 102498). The salary of JRL is supported by a National Heart Foundation of Australia Future Leader Fellowship (ID: 102817). The salary of JMH is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia Senior Research Fellowship (ID: 1116973). The salary of JES is supported by a National Health and Medical Research Council Investigator Grant (ID: 1173952). None of the funding agencies had any role in the conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
Data availability
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study. Raw data are not publicly available. However, data described in the manuscript, codebook, and analytic code will be made available upon request and approval of the AusDiab Steering Committee.
Code availability
Not applicable.
Declarations
Conflict of interest
CPB and JMH report grants from FruitWest, and from Department of Agriculture and Food WA, outside the submitted work. DJM and JES report grants from Abbott Australasia Pty Ltd, Alphapharm Pty Ltd, AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly Australia, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen-Cilag, Merck Sharp & Dohme, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Novo Nordisk Pharmaceuticals, Roche Diagnostics Australia, Sanofi Aventis and Sanofi-synthelabo, during the conduct of the study. RMD received a Primary Growth Partnership grant via the Ministry of Primary Industries in New Zealand with Fonterra Co-operative Group Ltd, outside the submitted work. All other authors have no potential conflicts of interest to report.
Ethics approval
The study was approved (on March 2nd, 1999) by The Human Research Ethics Committees of the International Diabetes Institute (Melbourne, Australia).
Consent to participate
A written informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to their inclusion in the study in 1999/2000.
Consent for publication
All participants provided informed consent regarding publishing the results of this study.
References
- 1.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. Journal of health and social behavior. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24:385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schiavone S, Colaianna M, Curtis L. Impact of early life stress on the pathogenesis of mental disorders: relation to brain oxidative stress. Curr Pharm Des. 2015;21(11):1404–1412. doi: 10.2174/1381612821666150105143358. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Rueggeberg R, Wrosch C, Miller GE. The different roles of perceived stress in the association between older adults' physical activity and physical health. Health Psychol. 2012;31(2):164. doi: 10.1037/a0025242. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Hayward RD, Taylor WD, Smoski MJ, Steffens DC, Payne ME. Association of five-factor model personality domains and facets with presence, onset, and treatment outcomes of major depression in older adults. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2013;21(1):88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2012.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Pope BS, Wood SK. Advances in understanding mechanisms and therapeutic targets to treat comorbid depression and cardiovascular disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2020;116:337–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Surtees PG, Wainwright NW, Luben RN, Wareham NJ, Bingham SA, Khaw K-T. Depression and ischemic heart disease mortality: evidence from the EPIC-Norfolk United Kingdom prospective cohort study. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(4):515–523. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07061018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.WHO (2021) Any mental illness among adults. National Instittute of Mental Health. World Health Organization. https://www.nimh.nih.gov/health/statistics/mental-illness.shtml
- 8.WHO (2018) Mental health. World Health Organization. https://www.who.int/health-topics/mental-health#tab=tab_2
- 9.Marx W, Moseley G, Berk M, Jacka F. Nutritional psychiatry: the present state of the evidence. Proc Nutr Soc. 2017;76(4):427–436. doi: 10.1017/S0029665117002026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Jacka FN, O’Neil A, Opie R, Itsiopoulos C, Cotton S, Mohebbi M, Castle D, Dash S, Mihalopoulos C, Chatterton ML. A randomised controlled trial of dietary improvement for adults with major depression (the ‘SMILES’trial) BMC Med. 2017;15(1):23. doi: 10.1186/s12916-017-0791-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Saghafian F, Malmir H, Saneei P, Milajerdi A, Larijani B, Esmaillzadeh A. Fruit and vegetable consumption and risk of depression: accumulative evidence from an updated systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological studies. Br J Nutr. 2018;119(10):1087–1101. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518000697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Głąbska D, Guzek D, Groele B, Gutkowska K. Fruit and vegetable intake and mental health in adults: a systematic review. Nutrients. 2020;12(1):115. doi: 10.3390/nu12010115. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Baharzadeh E, Siassi F, Qorbani M, Koohdani F, Pak N, Sotoudeh G. Fruits and vegetables intake and its subgroups are related to depression: a cross-sectional study from a developing country. Ann Gen Psychiatry. 2018;17(1):1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12991-018-0216-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Brookie KL, Best GI, Conner TS. Intake of raw fruits and vegetables is associated with better mental health than intake of processed fruits and vegetables. Front Psychol. 2018;9:487. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kwon SC, Wyatt LC, Kranick JA, Islam NS, Devia C, Horowitz C, Trinh-Shevrin C. Physical activity, fruit and vegetable intake, and health-related quality of life among older Chinese, Hispanics, and Blacks in New York City. Am J Public Health. 2015;105(S3):S544–S552. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2015.302653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Niu K, Guo H, Kakizaki M, Cui Y, Ohmori-Matsuda K, Guan L, Hozawa A, Kuriyama S, Tsuboya T, Ohrui T. A tomato-rich diet is related to depressive symptoms among an elderly population aged 70 years and over: a population-based, cross-sectional analysis. J Affect Disord. 2013;144(1–2):165–170. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Kelloniemi H, Ek E, Laitinen J. Optimism, dietary habits, body mass index and smoking among young Finnish adults. Appetite. 2005;45(2):169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2005.05.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.McMartin SE, Jacka FN, Colman I. The association between fruit and vegetable consumption and mental health disorders: evidence from five waves of a national survey of Canadians. Prev Med. 2013;56(3–4):225–230. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2012.12.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Godos J, Currenti W, Angelino D, Mena P, Castellano S, Caraci F, Galvano F, Del Rio D, Ferri R, Grosso G. Diet and mental health: Review of the recent updates on molecular mechanisms. Antioxidants. 2020;9(4):346. doi: 10.3390/antiox9040346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Radavelli-Bagatini S, Blekkenhorst LC, Sim M, Prince RL, Bondonno NP, Bondonno CP, Woodman R, Anokye R, Dimmock J, Jackson B. Fruit and vegetable intake is inversely associated with perceived stress across the adult lifespan. Clin Nutr. 2021;40:2860–2867. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2021.03.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dunstan DW, Zimmet PZ, Welborn TA, Cameron AJ, Shaw J, de Courten M, Jolley D, McCarty DJ. The Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study (AusDiab)—methods and response rates. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2002;57(2):119–129. doi: 10.1016/S0168-8227(02)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Subar AF, Thompson FE, Kipnis V, Midthune D, Hurwitz P, McNutt S, McIntosh A, Rosenfeld S. Comparative validation of the Block, Willett, and National Cancer Institute food frequency questionnaires: the Eating at America's Table Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154(12):1089–1099. doi: 10.1093/aje/154.12.1089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Levenstein S, Prantera C, Varvo V, Scribano ML, Berto E, Luzi C, Andreoli A. Development of the Perceived Stress Questionnaire: a new tool for psychosomatic research. J Psychosom Res. 1993;37(1):19–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(93)90120-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Liu RT, Alloy LB. Stress generation in depression: a systematic review of the empirical literature and recommendations for future study. Clin Psychol Rev. 2010;30(5):582–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.04.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Ireland P, Jolley D, Giles G, O’Dea K, Powles J, Rutishauser I, Wahlqvist ML, Williams J. Development of the Melbourne FFQ: a food frequency questionnaire for use in an Australian prospective study involving an ethnically diverse cohort. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 1994;3(1):19–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Blekkenhorst LC, Hodgson JM, Lewis JR, Devine A, Woodman RJ, Lim WH, Wong G, Zhu K, Bondonno CP, Ward NC. Vegetable and fruit intake and fracture-related hospitalisations: a prospective study of older women. Nutrients. 2017;9(5):511. doi: 10.3390/nu9050511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.(ABS) ABoS (2014) 4364.0.55.007 - Australian Health Survey: Nutrition First Results - Foods and Nutrients, 2011–12 Australian Bureau of Statistics (ABS)
- 28.NHMRC . Australian Dietary Guidelines. Canberra: Australian Government; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Blekkenhorst L, Sim M, Bondonno C, Bondonno N, Ward N, Prince R, Devine A, Lewis J, Hodgson J. Cardiovascular health benefits of specific vegetable types: a narrative review. Nutrients. 2018;10(5):595. doi: 10.3390/nu10050595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Blekkenhorst LC, Bondonno CP, Lewis JR, Devine A, Zhu K, Lim WH, Woodman RJ, Beilin LJ, Prince RL, Hodgson JM. Cruciferous and allium vegetable intakes are inversely associated with 15-year atherosclerotic vascular disease deaths in older adult women. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017;6(10):1–15. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.117.006558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Quiñones M, Miguel M, Aleixandre A. Beneficial effects of polyphenols on cardiovascular disease. Pharmacol Res. 2013;68(1):125–131. doi: 10.1016/j.phrs.2012.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Vazquez-Prieto MA, Miatello RM. Organosulfur compounds and cardiovascular disease. Mol Aspects Med. 2010;31(6):540–545. doi: 10.1016/j.mam.2010.09.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Voutilainen S, Nurmi T, Mursu J, Rissanen TH. Carotenoids and cardiovascular health. Am J Clin Nutr. 2006;83(6):1265–1271. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/83.6.1265. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Weitzberg E, Lundberg JO. Novel aspects of dietary nitrate and human health. Annu Rev Nutr. 2013;33:129–159. doi: 10.1146/annurev-nutr-071812-161159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Blekkenhorst LC, Bondonno CP, Lewis JR, Devine A, Zhu K, Lim WH, Woodman RJ, Beilin LJ, Prince RL, Hodgson JM. Cruciferous and allium vegetable intakes are inversely associated with 15-year atherosclerotic vascular disease deaths in older adult women. J Am Heart Assoc. 2017;6(10):e006558. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.117.006558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Blekkenhorst LC, Bondonno CP, Lewis JR, Woodman RJ, Devine A, Bondonno NP, Lim WH, Zhu K, Beilin LJ, Thompson PL. Cruciferous and total vegetable intakes are inversely associated with subclinical atherosclerosis in older adult women. J Am Heart Assoc. 2018;7(8):e008391. doi: 10.1161/JAHA.117.008391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Blekkenhorst LC, Sim M, Radavelli-Bagatini S, Bondonno NP, Bondonno CP, Devine A, Schousboe JT, Lim WH, Kiel DP, Woodman RJ. Cruciferous vegetable intake is inversely associated with extensive abdominal aortic calcification in elderly women: a cross-sectional study. Br J Nutr. 2021;125(3):337–345. doi: 10.1017/S0007114520002706. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Sim M, Blekkenhorst LC, Lewis JR, Bondonno CP, Devine A, Zhu K, Woodman RJ, Prince RL, Hodgson JM. Vegetable and fruit intake and injurious falls risk in older women: a prospective cohort study. Br J Nutr. 2018;120(8):925–934. doi: 10.1017/S0007114518002155. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Sim M, Blekkenhorst LC, Lewis JR, Bondonno CP, Devine A, Zhu K, Woodman RJ, Prince RL, Hodgson JM. Vegetable diversity, injurious falls, and fracture risk in older women: a prospective cohort study. Nutrients. 2018;10(8):1081. doi: 10.3390/nu10081081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Radavelli-Bagatini S, Anokye R, Bondonno NP, Sim M, Bondonno CP, Stanley MJ, Harms C, Woodman R, Magliano DJ, Shaw JE. Association of habitual intake of fruits and vegetables with depressive symptoms: the AusDiab study. Eur J Nutr. 2021;60(7):3743–3755. doi: 10.1007/s00394-021-02532-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Armstrong T, Bauman AE, Davies J. Physical activity patterns of Australian adults: results of the 1999 National Physical Activity Survey. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pink B. Socio-economic indexes for areas (SEIFA) Canberra: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Dalton M, Cameron AJ, Zimmet PZ, Shaw JE, Jolley D, Dunstan DW, Welborn TA, Committee AS. Waist circumference, waist–hip ratio and body mass index and their correlation with cardiovascular disease risk factors in Australian adults. J Intern Med. 2003;254(6):555–563. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2003.01229.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Barr ELM, Magliano DJ, Zimmet PZ, Polkinghorne KR, Atkins RC, Dunstan DW, Murray SG, Shaw JE. AusDiab 2005: The Australian Diabetes, Obesity and Lifestyle Study - Tracking the accelerating epidemic: its causes and outcomes. Melbourne, Australia: International Diabetes Institute; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Backholer K, Spencer E, Gearon E, Magliano DJ, McNaughton SA, Shaw JE, Peeters A. The association between socio-economic position and diet quality in Australian adults. Public Health Nutr. 2015;19(3):477–485. doi: 10.1017/S1368980015001470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Diabetes A, Dunstan DW. Diabesity & Associated Disorders in Australia-2000: the accelerating epidemic. Melbourne: International Diabetes Institute; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dunstan DW, Zimmet PZ, Welborn TA, Cameron AJ, Shaw J, De Courten M, Jolley D, McCarty DJ, Committee AS. The Australian diabetes, obesity and lifestyle study (AusDiab)—methods and response rates. Diabetes Res Clin Pract. 2002;57(2):119–129. doi: 10.1016/S0168-8227(02)00025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.McNaughton SA, Ball K, Crawford D, Mishra GD. An index of diet and eating patterns is a valid measure of diet quality in an Australian population. J Nutr. 2008;138(1):86–93. doi: 10.1093/jn/138.1.86. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.AGHE (2015) Australian guide to healthy eating. Eat for health. https://www.eatforhealth.gov.au
- 50.NHMRC . Australian dietary guidelines. Australian Government, Canberra: National Health and Medical Research Council; 2020. [Google Scholar]
- 51.StataCorp L. Stata statistical software (version release 14) College Station, TX: Author; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Besharat M-A, Khadem H, Zarei V, Momtaz A. Mediating role of perceived stress in the relationship between facing existential issues and symptoms of depression and anxiety. Iran J Psychiatry. 2020;15(1):80. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Alissa EM, Ferns GA. Dietary fruits and vegetables and cardiovascular diseases risk. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2017;57(9):1950–1962. doi: 10.1080/10408398.2015.1040487. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Boyer J, Liu RH. Apple phytochemicals and their health benefits. Nutr J. 2004;3(1):1–15. doi: 10.1186/1475-2891-3-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hulsken S, Märtin A, Mohajeri MH, Homberg JR. Food-derived serotonergic modulators: effects on mood and cognition. Nutr Res Rev. 2013;26(2):223–234. doi: 10.1017/S0954422413000164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Silber B, Schmitt J. Effects of tryptophan loading on human cognition, mood, and sleep. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2010;34(3):387–407. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Maiani G, Periago Castón MJ, Catasta G, Toti E, Cambrodón IG, Bysted A, Granado-Lorencio F, Olmedilla-Alonso B, Knuthsen P, Valoti M. Carotenoids: actual knowledge on food sources, intakes, stability and bioavailability and their protective role in humans. Mol Nutr Food Res. 2009;53(S2):S194–S218. doi: 10.1002/mnfr.200800053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Gehlich KH, Beller J, Lange-Asschenfeldt B, Köcher W, Meinke MC, Lademann J. Fruit and vegetable consumption is associated with improved mental and cognitive health in older adults from non-Western developing countries. Public Health Nutr. 2019;22(4):689–696. doi: 10.1017/S1368980018002525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Slavin JL, Lloyd B. Health benefits of fruits and vegetables. Adv Nutr. 2012;3(4):506–516. doi: 10.3945/an.112.002154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available from the Baker Heart and Diabetes Institute. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for the current study. Raw data are not publicly available. However, data described in the manuscript, codebook, and analytic code will be made available upon request and approval of the AusDiab Steering Committee.
Not applicable.