Skip to main content
. 2022 Oct 6;13:944815. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.944815

TABLE 2.

Characteristics of population-based longitudinal studies.

Population based studies
Longitudinal
Author/s Year Country N Age when SC measured Age when mental health measured SC measure/s Results
Rice et al. (26) 2019 England 7,543 7 years 10.64 (SD = 0.25) and 18.65 (SD = 0.49) CCC (stand.; par.) SCDC (stand.; par.) Significantly higher rates of childhood SC and pragmatic language problems were identified in individuals with early adolescence onset of depression, compared to those with later adolescence onset (SC: OR = 0.68; Pragmatic difficulties: OR = 1.31)
Sullivan et al. (28) 2017 England 7,058 7, 10, 13, 16 years 12 and 18 SCDC (stand.; par.) SC abilities at ages 7 and 10 were significantly associated with depressive symptoms at 12 years (OR = 1.22), and SC abilities at ages 13 and 16 were significantly associated with depressive symptoms at 18 years (OR = 1.21), even after controlling for sociodemographic variables (including IQ) and autism traits.
Sullivan et al. (27) 2016 England 7,659 9 years 12 and 18 CCC (stand.; par.) Poorer pragmatic language at age 9 was associated with depression at age 18 (OR = 1.10). Poorer expressive speech and language abilities were not associated with depression at either age.
Riglin et al. (29) 2016 England 6,664 7–9 years 3.9, 6.8, 8.1, 9.6, 11.7, 13.1, and 16.5 years CCC (stand.; par.)
SCDC (stand.; par.)
The persistent ADHD trajectory from 4 to 17 years was significantly associated with multimorbidity: within this group 53.1% had SC problems, 27.8% had pragmatic language impairments, 24.1% had low IQ, and 42% had conduct problems in childhood.
Law et al. (30) 2015 England 2,915 9 years 13 years CCC (stand.; par.) Pragmatic language skills partly mediated the association between social disadvantage and adolescent behavior (emotional and behavioral) in middle childhood, even after controlling for sex, age, and IQ.
Oliver et al. (31) 2011 England 6,074 9 years 4, 7, 8, 10, 12, and 13 years CCC (stand.; par.)
SCDC (stand.; par.)
SC difficulties were significantly greater in all conduct-problems groups, particularly in the early onset-persistent group. Controlling for demographic confounders and child verbal IQ did not change the associations.
Skuse et al. (32) 2009 England 8,094 7.8 years 8.4 years (SD = 3.7 months) SCDC (stand.; par.) SC difficulties were significantly associated with all SDQ behavior outcomes about 1 year later, after controlling for sex, IQ, and maternal education.
Askeland et al. (33) 2021 Norway 15,205 0.6, 1.5, 3, and 8 years 1.5, 3, 5, and 8 years Ages and stages SC (34) (stand.; par.)M-CHAT (35), (stand.; par.) SCQ (36) (stand.; par.), short version CCC-2 (stand.; par.) SC at age 5 correlated moderately with hyperactivity symptoms at age 8.
Beernink et al. (37) 2007 The Netherlands 1,803 14 and 19 months 14 and 19 months Self-constructed questionnaire (non-stand.; par.) The variable “communicative intent” at 14 months correlated significantly with oppositional behavior and attention at 19 months.

ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CCC, Children’s Communication Checklist; CCC-2, Children’s Communication Checklist 2nd Edition; M-CHAT, Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; non-stand., non-standardized; OR, odds ratio; par., parent reported; SC, Social Communication; SCDC, Social and Communication Disorders Checklist; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; stand., standardized; teach., teacher reported.