TABLE 4.
“Special populations” | ||||||||
Longitudinal | ||||||||
Author/s | Year | Country | N | Age when SC measured | Age when mental health measured | Special population | SC measure/s | Results |
Conti-Ramsden et al. (55) | 2019 | England | 168 | 11 years | 7, 8, 11, and 16 years | Language Impairment | CCC (stand.; teach) | Significantly poorer pragmatic language abilities were found in the group with childhood-onset-persistent emotional and peer-relationship problems, as compared to the resolving emotional group and low-level group. |
Mok et al. (56) | 2014 | England | 171 | 7 and 11 years | 7, 8, 11, and 16 years | History of language impairment | CCC (stand.; teach) | Pragmatic language difficulties at age 7 were 2.5 times more likely for children with childhood-onset persistent peer problems at age 11 than for those with low/no peer problems. |
Helland et al. (57) | 2014 | Norway | 40 | 13.47 (SD = 0.82) years | 7–9 and 13.47 (SD = 0.82) years | Behavioral difficulties | CCC-2 (stand.; par.) | Pragmatic language ability at age 12–15 years correlated significantly with the pragmatic language composite score (r = −0.52), with the SDQ subscales emotional problems (r = −0.40) and peer problems (r = −0.048), and with the SDQ total difficulties score (r = −0.05) at age 7–9 years. In a multiple regression model, language and peer problems at t1 significantly predicted pragmatics at t2 (R2 = 36%). |
CCC, Children’s Communication Checklist; CCC-2, Children’s Communication Checklist 2nd Edition; dir., direct assessment; non-stand., non-standardized; par., parent reported; SDQ, Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire; self, self-assessment; stand., standardized; teach., teacher reported.