Skip to main content
. 2022 Jun 29;23(4):409–427. doi: 10.1080/26895269.2022.2085224

Table 3.

Antecedents of 1st social transitions.

Code No. of youth participants who mentioned code No. of parent participants who mentioned code
n = 10 youth interviews reporting on 10 initial transitions n = 21 parent interviews reporting on 21 initial transitions
n (%) n (%)
Child made a statement (e.g., “I am a girl”)/brought it up 7 (70.0%) 19 (90.5%)
Parent inquired about/suggested/explained the options 1 (10.0%) 11 (52.4%)
Child’s gender preferences/presentation (peers, toys, clothing, hairstyle) 4 (40.0%) 19 (90.5%)
Child role-played/identified as different gender in fantasy play (could be on the internet) 1 (10.0%) 6 (28.6%)
Someone else brought it up (e.g., therapist, support group, teacher, parent’s friend) 0 (0%) 2 (9.5%)
Went to a therapist/support group about gender (child or parent) 1 (10.0%) 9 (42.9%)
Child mental health/wellbeing/behavior issues that seemed to be related to gender 1 (10.0%) 11 (52.4%)
Gender segregation (e.g., boys’ and girls’ sports, boys’ line vs. girls’ line) in school or other activity brought it up 1 (10.0%) 2 (9.5%)
Issues related to body/puberty/hormones/blockers 0 (0%) 2 (9.5%)
Learned about a new identity (e.g., gay men, lesbians, nonbinary) 1 (10.0%) 3 (14.3%)
Met a role model of a particular gender / LGBT identity 0 (0%) 1 (4.8%)
Child’s internal gender identity experience 5 (50.0%) 1 (4.8%)
Child wanted to live authentically 2 (20.0%) 0 (0%)
Peers — acceptance or rejection from peers of one gender (e.g., being bullied by the boys; being accepted by the girls) 0 (0%) 3 (14.3%)
People were misgendering them/were transphobic toward them in previous gender 0 (0%) 1 (4.8%)
Believed gender expression had to align with gender identity (e.g., believed feminine people had to be girls) 1 (10.0%) 2 (9.5%)
Child thought it would be cool/edgy/make a political statement to transition 1 (10.0%) 0 (0%)