Abstract
Sleep parasomnias have drawn the interest of sleep experts because they represent a valuable window to directly monitor dream activity and sleep mentation associated with nocturnal events. Indeed, parasomnias and their manifestations are helpful in investigating dream activity and features, overcoming methodological limits that affect dream study. Specifically, REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD) is a parasomnia characterized by enacted dream episodes during Rapid Eye Movements (REM) sleep, caused by the loss of physiological atonia. Patients suffering from RBD report a peculiar oneiric activity associated with motor episodes characterized by high Dream Recall Frequency (DRF) and vivid dreams. Additionally, isolated RBD (iRBD) represents a prodromal stage of neurodegeneration preceding the development of α-synucleinopathies. This narrative review aims to combine evidence describing dream activity in RBD and similarities and differences with other NREM parasomnias. Moreover, a special focus has been reserved for those conditions in which RBD is associated with α-synucleinopathies to clarify the potential role of dreams in neurodegenerative processes.
Keywords: parasomnias, REM sleep behavior disorder, dream recall frequency, dream contents, oneiric activity
1. Introduction
Sleep parasomnias are described in the third edition of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD-3) [1] as sleep disorders involving unusual motor and vocal behaviors accompanied by emotional or sensory perceptions and associated with dream mentation. These episodes appear during transition periods between sleep and wake or are concomitant to specific sleep stages. Hence, parasomnias can be classified into Non-Rapid Eye Movement (NREM) (i.e., confusional arousals, Sleep Walking (SW), Sleep Terrors (ST), and sleep-related eating disorder) and Rapid-Eye Movement (REM) related (i.e., REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD), recurrent isolated sleep paralysis, nightmare disorder, and sleep-related hallucinations) [1].
REM and NREM parasomnias have drawn the interest of sleep experts not only on the clinical characteristics reported by the patients but also because these conditions represent a precious window to directly monitor dream activity and sleep mentation associated with nocturnal events. Indeed, an intrinsic issue of dream study concerns their inaccessible nature: dream contents are not directly accessible, and knowledge on oneiric activity is collected through retrospective recall [2]. As a consequence, the retrospective nature of dream collection leads to several methodological problems due to distortions and omissions in the recall caused by memory reprocessing [3].
In this review, we focus on RBD as privileged parasomnia to explore the dream process. Indeed, this is a sleep disorder characterized by enacted dream episodes during REM sleep, caused by the loss of physiological atonia. Clinical observations have pointed out in RBD a peculiar oneiric activity associated with motor episodes characterized by high Dream Recall Frequency (DRF) and vivid dreams, containing elements of violence and attacks (by people and animals) [4]. Moreover, RBD may present itself as isolated (iRBD), namely the parasomnia is not due to other neurological conditions, and it represents a prodromal stage of neurodegeneration. Specifically, longitudinal studies showed in iRBD patients an elevated risk of developing α-synucleinopathies (i.e., Parkinson’s Disease (PD), Dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB), and Multiple System Atrophy (MSA)) that seems to increase over several years (i.e., 33.5% at five years after diagnosis, 82.4% at 10.5 years, and 96.6% at 14 years) [5]. In light of this strong association between iRBD and neurodegenerative diseases, in the last years, scientists have researched neuropsychological, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging biomarkers for a timely prediction of phenoconversion [6]. In this view, oneiric activity in RBD has been proposed as associated with biological processes leading to α-synucleinopathies. Indeed, evidence in PD patients, already in the early stages of the disease, shows a high prevalence of distressing and vivid dreams, dreams with violent content, and nightmares [7], supporting the notion that RBD patients share similar dream features with PD patients [8].
Within this theoretical background, this paper aims to combine evidence describing dream activity and its features in RBD, and similarities and differences with other NREM parasomnias, such as SW and ST. Moreover, a special focus has been reserved for those conditions in which RBD is associated with α-synucleinopathies to clarify the role of dreams in neurodegenerative processes.
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Search Strategy
We conducted a literature search from March 2022 to July 2022, following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement. The literature search was conducted using PubMed, Scopus, PsyArticles, and Web of Science, considering available studies up to July 2022. Search terms included: “Rem Sleep Behavior Disorder”, “dream enactment”, “dream”, “dream recall”, “dreaming”, “oneiric”, “nightmare”, and “dream report”. Search terms had to be contained in the title, abstract, and/or keywords.
2.2. Inclusion/Exclusion Criteria
Titles, abstracts, and keywords were inspected to meet the following inclusion criteria: (1) English language; (2) peer-reviewed article; (3) cross-sectional or longitudinal design; (4) main focus on at least one of the investigated phenomena (RBD sample, with or without comorbidities; dreams; nightmares); (5) quantitative/qualitative examination of at least one aspect of the investigated phenomena (frequency, qualitative features, content); (6) the method employed to diagnose RBD had to include at least one night of video polysomnographic (vPSG) registration. Books, abstracts, comments, reviews, meta-analyses, pre-prints, and letters to editors were excluded.
One expert researcher chose eligible articles through a multi-step process (title reading, abstract, and full-text assessment).
3. Results
Forty-one papers published between 1999 and 2022 met the inclusion criteria and were selected for our review. Thirty-two studies had a cross-sectional design, of which eight with a descriptive approach (i.e., between-group analyses were not performed), five papers adopted a longitudinal approach, two were retrospective studies, two adopted a multiple-awakenings protocol, and two papers selected were case series studies (see Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4).
In all included studies, a total of 1936 RBD are enrolled. Moreover, among studies that considered RBD secondary to other pathologies, a total of 174 RBD patients were secondary to PD, 24 PD patients presented probable RBD, 15 RBD secondary to a Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), and 13 to DLB. The examined studies also considered a total of 614 Healthy Controls (HCs), 104 PD patients, 113 SW and ST sufferers, 64 with Obstructive Sleep Apnea (OSA), 13 DLB, and seven with PTSD. Moreover, a single study considered RBD and REM Sleep Without Atonia (RSWA) in children and adolescents [9].
Among RBD, idiopathic and secondary, 87.2% were men, reflecting the male predominance of this parasomnia [10]. Moreover, considering all papers, the mean age was 63.6 years, ranging from three years [9] to 88 years [11].
From a methodological point of view, the studies selected can be divided into two categories according to the dream assessment, as shown in Figure 1: retrospective (n = 32) and prospective (n = 11). Studies in both categories extracted quantitative (dreams and nightmares recall frequency) and qualitative (dream contents and themes) dream features through different procedures.
Figure 1.
Studies categorization. Among 41 studies analyzed, 32 studies performed retrospective dreams assessment [9,11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40] extracting quantitative [4,12,13,14,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,36,37,38,39,40] and qualitative [9,11,13,15,16,17,19,20,27,28,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,40,41,42,43] dream features. 11 studies performed prospective dreams assessment [17,36,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52] extracting quantitative [17,36,46,47,48,49,51] and qualitative [17,36,44,45,46,48,49,50,52] dream features.
3.1. How RBD Patients Dream?
From the first observation and description [53], RBD drew the attention of sleep experts for its unique characteristics of dream-enactment during REM sleep.
Motor behaviors observed in this parasomnia appear to act out dream contents and settings, as demonstrated by the correspondence between the features of dream recall and the observed behaviors. Moreover, dream contents reported have recurrent elements and similar characteristics among patients. The analysis of dream contents (See Table 1) highlighted recurrent unpleasant dreams and nightmares reported by RBD patients [12,44]. Specifically, the main themes collected in the dream recall were attacks by people or animals [13,14,44], violence [11,12,15], and fright [12] (for detailed dream report examples, see Leclair-Visonneau et al. [45] in Table 1). Interestingly, these violent and aggressive oneiric themes are not due to and do not match personality features. Indeed, RBD patients did not show hostile and violent traits during wakefulness; on the contrary, they appeared quiet and calm [16,17,46]. Moreover, dream assessment proved that the more dreams with aggression, misfortune, and negative emotions occurred, the more the patients had lower traits of hostility, anger, and less tendency to be aggressive [16].
Table 1.
Sample, design, tools, and findings in studies investigating dreaming in RBD and RBD vs. HC. Legend: X = absence of HC sample.
Study | RBD Sample | HC Sample | Design | Dream Measures | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
[11] |
39 RBD < 50 y Mean age (SD): 32 ± 9 y Gender: 23 M/16 F 52 RBD ≥ 50 y Mean age (SD): 67 ± 8 y Gender: 39 M/13 F |
X | Cross-Sectional | Not specified |
Dream Content:
|
[12] |
20 early onset RBD Gender: 12 M/11 F 67 late-onset RBD Gender: 51 M/16 F |
90 HC Gender: 63 M/27 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-HK |
DRF: In total, 91% RBD reported dreams more than 3 times per week Dream-related scores (Factor1):
|
[44] |
4 iRBD Gender: 2 M/2 F |
X |
Cross-Sectional
Descriptive |
Not specified |
Dream content:
|
[13] |
93 RBD Mean age: 64.4 y Gender: 81 M/12 F |
X |
Retrospective Descriptive Case Series |
Not specified |
Dream Report (n 67):
|
[14] |
203 iRBD Gender: 162 M/41 F |
X |
Longitudinal
Descriptive |
Semi-structured interview |
Unpleasant dream recall:
|
[15] |
7 RBD Gender: 5 M/2 F |
X |
Cross-Sectional
Descriptive |
Telephone interview |
All RBD patients reported violent dreams |
[45] |
56 RBD Mean age (SD): 64.7 ± 8.2 y Gender: 43 M/13 F |
17 HC Mean age (SD): 62.2 ± 7.1 y Gender: 14 M/3 F |
Cross-Sectional
Descriptive |
Immediate dream recall through interview |
Detailed dream reports examples in the paper: |
[16] |
49 RBD Mean age (SD): 67.5 ± 7.5 y Gender: 36 M/5 F |
35 HC Mean age (SD): 69.1 ± 5.9 y Gender: 30 M/5 F |
Cross-Sectional | Free recall and semi-structured interview scored by HVdC |
DRF: >in RBD (p < 0.001) Dream content
|
[18] |
94 RBD Mean age (SD): 61.9 ± 12.7 y Gender: 66 M/28 F |
X | Cross-Sectional | Clinical interview |
DRF:
|
[19] |
141 M iRBD Mean age (SD): 66.7 ± 6.7 y 43 F iRBD Mean age (SD): 68.7 ± 7.3 y |
X | Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-JP | Dream-related scores (Factor1): Absence of significant difference between male and female iRBD |
[20] |
90 RBD Gender: 63 M/27 F |
X | Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-HK |
Dream-related scores (Factor1): Absence of significant difference between M and F RBD Vivid dreams: Absence of significant differences between M and F RBD Violent dreams: Absence of significant differences between M and F RBD Frightening dreams: Absence of significant differences between M and F RBD |
[21] |
68 RBD Mean age (SD): 63.7 ± 10.9 y Gender: 49 M/19 F |
44 HC Mean age (SD): 62.0 ± 12.2 y Gender: 28 M/16 F |
Cross-Sectional | TDQ DTD index |
DRF:
|
[22] |
8 non-recallers RBD Age range: 69.8–75.8 y Gender: 6 M/2 F 17 recallers RBD Age range: 66.0–75.0 y Gender: 12 M/5 F |
X | Cross-sectional | Dream interview |
DRF:
|
[23] |
53 RBD Mean age (SD): 69.0 ± 16.5 y Gender: 39 M/14 F |
X |
Cross-Sectional
Descriptive |
Dream questionnaire |
DRF: >in RBDs in which injury occurred compared to RBDs in which injury did not occur (p = 0.002) Dream content:
|
[24] |
6 RBD Mean age: 54 y Gender: 3 M/3 F |
X |
Longitudinal (6 weeks) to examine the effect of melatonin treatment |
Modification in dream activity after treatment |
None of the responders reported any frightening dreams during the treatment period |
[25] |
8 RBD Mean age: 54 y Gender: 8 M |
X |
Longitudinal
(4 weeks) to examine the effect of melatonin treatment |
Modification in dream activity after treatment |
Dream Content: None of the responders reported having frightening dreams after four days of treatment DRF: All patients were able to distinguish placebo from treatment based on a reduction in dream mentation |
[26] |
39 RBD Mean age (SD): 68.3 ± 7.8 y Gender: 29 M/10 F |
X |
Longitudinal (28.8 months) to examine the effect of clonazepam treatment |
RBDQ-3M |
DRF: Absence of significant differences pre/post-treatment Nightmare frequency: >before than pre-treatment (p < 0.01) Dream-related scores (Factor 1):
|
[27] |
32 RBD Mean age (SD): 61.5 ± 11.1 y Gender: 23 M/9 F |
30 HC Mean age (SD): 56.9 ± 16.6 y Gender: 19 M/11 F |
Cross-Sectional | Dream questionnaire |
|
[29] |
29 iRBD Mean age (SD): 62.9 ± 9.4 y Gender: 23 M/8 F 31 pRBD Mean age (SD): 44.4 ± 9.8 y Gender: 13 M/8 F |
31 HC Mean age (SD): 46.0 ± 12.5 y Gender: 11 M/20 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-HK |
Dream-related scores (Factor1):
|
[30] |
51 iRBD Gender: 41 M/10 F 29 sRBD Gender: 23 M/6 F 27 RBD like Gender: 11 M/16 F |
107 HC Mean age (SD): 55.3 ± 9 y Gender: 62 M/45 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-HK |
Dream-related scores (Factor1):
|
[31] |
105 RBD Mean age (SD): 67.3 ± 6.4 y Gender: 60 M/49 F |
105 HC Mean age (SD): 65.8 ± 5.7 y Gender: 49 M/56 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-KR | Dream related scores (Factor1): >in RBD compared to HC (p < 0.001) |
[32] |
13 iRBD Mean age (SD): 66.3 ± 6.5 y Gender: 11 M/2 F |
10 HC Mean age (SD): 62.3 ± 7.5 y Gender: 7 M/3 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-KR |
Dream-related scores (Factor1):
|
[33] |
94 RBD Mean age (SD): 67.6 ± 7.3 y Gender: 53 M/41 F |
50 HC Mean age (SD): 65.4 ± 6.0 y Gender: 24 M/26 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ-KR |
Dream related scores (Factor1): >in RBD than HC (p < 0.001) Correlations:
|
[48] |
12 RBD Mean age (SD): 65.6 ± 10.7 y Gender: 11 M/1 F |
12 HC Mean age (SD): 63.3 ± 12.9 y Gender: 8 M/4 F |
Cross-Sectional | Dream diary (3 weeks) |
Dream content:
Words number: Absence of significant difference between RBD and HC |
[49] |
13 clonazepam-treated iRBD Mean age (SD): 65.3 ± 10.9 y Gender: 12 M/1 F 11 untreated iRBD Mean age (SD): 68.9 ± 6.8 y Gender: 9 M/2 F |
12 HC Mean age (SD): 63.3 ± 12.8 y Gender: 8 M/4 F |
Cross-Sectional | Dream diary (3 weeks) scored by HVdC TSS |
Dream Reports (n 214):
|
[34] |
123 RBD divided in 96 with treatment- improvement Mean age (SD): 65.7 ± 8.5 y Gender: 61 M/35 F 27 without treatment- improvement Mean age (SD): 66.1 ± 7.5 y Gender: 15 M/12 F |
X |
Longitudinal (17.7 months) to examine the effect of clonazepam treatment |
RBDQ-KR | Dream-related scores (Factor1): Absence of significant difference between responding and no-respond groups |
Abbreviations: Aggression/Characters ratio (A/C); Aggression Questionnaire (AQ); Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP); Dream Enactment Behaviors (DEB); Dream Recall Frequency (DRF); Dream Theme Diversity (DTD); Female (F); Hall and Van De Castle method (HVdC); Healthy Controls (HC); idiopathic REM sleep Behavior Disorder (iRBD); Male (M); Modified RBD Questionnaire (RBDQ-3M); Periodic Limb Movement Index (PLMI); Psychiatric REM sleep Behavior Disorder (pRBD); Rapid Eye Movements (REM); REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD); RBD Questionnaire–Japanese version (RBDQ-JP); RBD Questionnaire—Hong Kong version (RBDQ-HK); RBD Questionnaire—Korean version (RBDQ—KR); secondary REM sleep Behavior Disorder (sRBD); Threat Simulation Scale (TSS); Typical Dreams Questionnaire (TDQ); Years (y).
Another key feature of this REM parasomnia is the prevalence of the disorder in the male population [10,54], reporting more severe symptoms and nocturnal behavioral episodes in men than women with RBD [41,55]. However, studies that explored gender differences in oneiric activity revealed the absence of significant differences between males and females in dreams and nightmares recall rates [14,18,19], vividness [20], and contents [19,20].
Moreover, elevated dream and nightmare recall frequency (from 98.6% to 75%) was reported in RBDs [12,14,18,21,22]. In 63% of dream reports, the recall was associated with behavioral episodes [13], and higher DRF was found in RBD patients causing injuries than in RBDs in which injury did not occur, although no between-groups differences were reported in the dream contents [23]. However, this peculiar framework of oneiric activity observed in RBD, characterized by high DRF and violent dream contents, has not always been confirmed by studies that examined the effect of treatment on RBD symptoms and studies that compared patients with HCs.
On the one hand, we can affirm that specific dream contents, characterized by violent and aggressive themes, are typical of RBD. Moreover, longitudinal studies showed that melatonin and clonazepam assumptions suspended frightening, violent dreams [24,25,26], and nightmares [26] during treatment. In the same line, compared to HCs, dreams in RBDs were characterized by a prevalence of violent and aggressive themes, also involving animal or people attacks [16,21,27,28,29], with a high incidence of negative emotions and nightmare distress [16,27]. Moreover, using the RBD Questionnaire (RBDQ) [30], patients showed higher scores in Factor 1 (which considers the dreams and nightmares frequency and the emotional, violent, and aggressive contents) than HCs [29,30,31,32,33]. This evidence has been explained in two ways. The first hypothesis regards the biological and evolutionistic role of dreaming in simulating dangers and threats that have to do with ancestral human fears to “prepare” the subject to rehearse threat perception and its avoidance during wakefulness [42]. The second hypothesis to explain aggressive features in the RBDs’ dream reports could be to account for cognitive dysfunctions due to impairments observed in the frontal cortex [56]. Thus, these results suggest that violent nightmares and dreams in RBD may have clinical importance in predicting the possible onset of neurodegeneration. Indeed, the aggressive dream contents reported by PD patients is suggestive to be related to frontal cognitive dysfunction [57]. In addition, the violent and aggressive dream contents experienced by RBDs could be also explained by the lack of inhibition due to frontal cortex dysfunctions, leading to archaic defense behaviors acted out in dreams [56]. However, findings that may help to clarify this relationship will be reported and discussed in the next paragraph.
On the other hand, studies reported the absence of more vivid dreams in RBDs [28], no higher DRF [27,28,47], and no differences in dream contents [28,48] when comparing RBDs to HCs and when comparing pre and post treatment [34,49] (See Table 1 and Table 3). Contrasting results between studies in RBDs can be explained by methodological limits that affect results. Indeed, retrospective studies assessing oneiric activity in the past and during the entire patient’s life reported high rates of dreams and nightmare recall [12,13,14,18,21,23]. Retrospective methodology to collect dreams leads to the so-called “recall bias”, which is the predisposition of patients suffering from RBD to recall more frequently vivid dreams with violent and frightening contents accompanied by motor behaviors [58].
Concluding, although literature findings confirm a predisposition of RBDs to report oneiric activity characterized by violent content, findings in dream frequency are not sufficient and are not solid enough to conclude a clear increase of DRF in RBDs. We recommend employing prospective experimental designs to collect dreams in future studies exploring dreaming in RBDs.
3.2. Dreaming in RBD: A Window into Neurodegenerative Mechanisms?
The intrinsic features of dream activity in iRBD patients described in the previous paragraph focus researchers’ attention on identifying potential markers able to predict the phenoconversion of parasomnia into α-synucleinopathies years before signs of neurodegeneration emerge [6]. Indeed, iRBD and PD conditions share similar dream features, such as a high prevalence of dreams with violent and distressing contents, and nightmares [59]. In this view, establishing a relationship between oneiric activity in iRBD and neuropathological mechanisms could allow early detection of neurodegeneration processes and make it possible to understand neural mechanisms underlying the generation and recall of dreams.
Our literature analysis reported 6 studies investigating dreaming in RBDs compared to PDs [35,36,46,50,51,52] and one study that compared dreaming between RBDs and DLBs [37] (See Table 2).
Table 2.
Sample, design, tools, and findings in studies investigating dreaming in RBD with and without neurodegenerative disorders.
Study | Sample | Design | Dream Measures | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|
[46] |
49 PD + RBD Mean age (SD): 68.3 ± 7.5 y Gender: 33 M/16 F 36 PD—RBD Mean age (SD): 69.9 ± 9.6 y Gender: 18 M/18 F 30 HC Mean age (SD): 66.8 ± 9.9 y Gender: 12 M/18 F |
Cross-Sectional | Dream Diary (1 month) scored by HVdC |
Total dreams collected: 106
DRF
|
[50] |
65 PD + RBD Mean age (SD): 65 ± 9 y Gender: 44 M/21 F 35 PD—RBD Mean age (SD): 61 ± 13 y Gender: 21 M/14 F |
Cross-Sectional | Interview |
Dream Content in PD + RBD
|
[51] |
6 PD + RBD Mean age (SD): 58.5 ± 8.4 y |
Multiple
Awakenings |
Dream Questionnaire |
DRF
|
[52] |
9 PD + RBD
1 PD 3 HC |
Cross-sectional
Descriptive |
Immediate Free Dream Recall |
|
[35] |
36 PD + RBD Mean age (SD): 67.2 ± 7.3 y Gender: 25 M/11 F 26 PD—RBD Mean age (SD): 68.3 ± 10 y Gender: 18 M/8 F 24 PD + probable RBD |
Cross-Sectional | NMSQuest—Item 24 |
|
[36] |
9 PD + RBD Mean age (SD): 61.2 ± 9.8 y Gender: 7 M/2 F 8 PD—RBD Mean age (SD): 64.0 ± 10.3 y Gender: 6 M/2 F |
Multiple
Awakenings |
Semi-Structured Interview Immediate Dream Recall |
DRF changes after the PD onset
Awakenings
|
[37] |
13 DLB Mean age (SD): 78.4 ± 6.2 y Gender: 6 M/7 F 13 DLB + RBD Mean age (SD): 77.4 ± 5.7 y Gender: 10 M/3 F |
Cross-Sectional Descriptive | Clinical Interview |
Unpleasant dream recall: 7 of the 13 (53.8%) patients with DLB + RBD |
Abbreviations: Dementia with Lewy Bodies (DLB); Female (F); Hall and Van de Castle method (HVdC); Healthy Controls (HC); Male (M); Non-Motor Symptoms Questionnaire (NMSQuest); Parkinson’s Disease (PD); Rapid Eye Movements (REM); REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD); Years (y).
These studies described high rates (from 50% to 70%) of DRF [37,51,52] and more vivid dreams [35,36,52] in RBD conditions in comorbidity with α-synucleinopathies (i.e., PD and DLB) than RBD without signs of neurodegeneration.
To clarify the association between dream-related features and neurodegenerative mechanisms that seem to underlie the RBD pathophysiology, interesting findings are those that consider specific RBD symptoms as RSWA. Results showed that in RBD the higher percentage of phasic muscle activity during REM sleep was related to more elements with natural disasters in dream recall. On the other hand, RBDs that showed higher tonic muscle activity percentage during REM sleep were less prone to recall dreams [21]. RSWA is one of the key criteria to diagnose RBD syndrome [1] and authors suggest it may reflect a progressive damage to the brainstem [43]. Tonic and phasic EMG activities during REM sleep have different neural mechanisms. Specifically, phasic EMG activity is regulated by locomotor nuclei in the ventromedial medulla, structure impaired yet in the early stages of PD [60]. On the other hand, increasing tonic muscle activity, depending on REM-on neurons of the sublaterodorsal tegmental nucleus, seems to be strictly associated with the phenoconversion to PD [61]. However, evidence from this review is not enough to confirm a strong relationship between dreams and RSWA features.
Moreover, a single study [36] retrospectively explored changes in RBDs converted in PD, reporting higher nightmares frequency after the onset of PD symptoms. However, a paper [46] investigating DRF through sleep diaries compiled for one month showed lower rates of dreams reported in PDs with and without RBD (<2 dreams per month) and no significant differences in DRF between the two groups. In the same direction, no between-group differences are revealed in the dream contents reported by iRBDs and RBDs with PD symptoms. Specifically, the absence of significant differences was found in vividness and intense emotional contents such as threat, aggression, or negativity [36,46]. Also in this case, these findings may be explained by the “recall bias” occurring when dreams are collected retrospectively. Indeed, studies reporting the absence of group differences in DRF and contents between RBD patients with and without PD adopted prospective designs (i.e., daily dream logs for one month [46] and systematic laboratory awakenings protocol [36]). Undoubtedly, these procedures reduce recall bias. These findings are consistent with the work by D’Agostino et al. [48] illustrated in the previous paragraph, which compared dream contents in RBDs and HCs employing immediate free recall through 3 weeks of daily dream diaries.
Overall, although the theory proposed is fascinating, the state of the art does not confirm the predictive value of dream features as markers able to track the neurodegenerative process in RBD.
3.3. Dream Features in RBD and NREM Parasomnias or Other Sleep Disorders
RBD nocturnal episodes appear as abnormal motor and vocal behaviors (i.e., punching, falling out of bed, and shouting) often associated with peculiar dream mentation [4]. These behavioral manifestations may also occur in other sleep disorders (e.g., OSA) and other NREM parasomnias (e.g., SW and ST). In severe OSA conditions, quite common among older adults, the respiratory effort and/or breathing resumption associated with sleep arousals lead to motor and vocal behaviors both during REM and NREM sleep [38]. In clinical settings, it is common to perform a differential diagnosis between RBD and OSA, based on the RSWA as a key feature for the RBD diagnosis [1].
Regarding SW and ST, these NREM parasomnias show complex, unaware, and aggressive or harmful motor and vocal behaviors, which might be mistaken for RBD episodes. However, SW/ST behaviors occur mostly during the first half of the night and always during Slow Wave Sleep (SWS) [1]. Consequently, the vPSG is the gold standard for a differential diagnosis between RBD and other sleep disorders.
Moreover, since the importance of operating a rapid and effective differential diagnosis to address different treatments and to indicate different prognoses, clinical information may also be useful. In this vein, the associated dream mentation features to the behavioral episodes may be suitable in terms of clinical implication to differentiate RBD from sleep disorders that mimic RBD symptoms.
In the literature, two studies [39,62] compared dreaming between RBD and OSA patients, and three studies [17,28,47] explored dreaming in RBDs and SWs or STs. As shown in Table 3, findings suggest little relevance of DRF as a signature of RBD.
Table 3.
Sample, design, tools, and findings in studies investigating dreaming in RBD and other sleep disorders.
Study | Sample | Design | Dream Measures | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|
[17] |
24 RBD Mean age (SD): 68.6 ± 8.8 y Gender: 19 M/5 F 32 SW/ST Mean age (SD): 31.4 ± 8.4 y Gender: 16 M/16 F |
Cross-sectional | Immediate free dream recall scored by HVdC TSS Dream complexity |
No. of dreams:
Complexity:
|
[28] |
64 RBD
Mean age (SD): 68.6 ± 8.0 y Gender: 44 M/20 F 62 SW Mean age (SD): 31.7 ± 9.5 y Gender: 29 M/33 F 66 oHC Mean age (SD): 67 ± 7.8 y Gender: 43 M/23 F 59 yHC Mean age (SD): 31.9 ± 9.3 y Gender: 29 M/30 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDSQ |
|
[47] |
20 RBD Mean age (SD): 66.5 ± 6.5 y Gender: 16 M/4 F 19 SW Mean age (SD): 34.4 ± 15.4 y Gender: 6 M/13 F 18 HC Mean age (SD): 57.9 ± 5.3 y Gender: 14 M/4 F |
Cross-Sectional | Immediate free recall |
DRF: absence of significant between-group differences |
[39] |
16 iRBD Mean age (SD): 64.5 ± 5.1 y Gender: 13 M/3 F 16 OSA Mean age (SD): 59.6 ± 7.7 y Gender: 11 M/5 F 20 HC Mean age (SD): 63.0 ± 9.8 y Gender: 16 M/4 F |
Cross-Sectional | Not specified |
Unpleasant dream content:
|
[62] |
118 RBD
Mean age (SD): 66.5 ± 8.4 y Gender: 91 M/27 F 106 OSA Mean age (SD): 61.6 ± 8.4 y Gender: 57 M/49 F |
Cross-Sectional | RBDQ—Beijing | Dream related scores (Factor 1): >in RBD compared to OSA (p < 0.001) |
[6] |
15 RBD + PTSD
Mean age: 55.2 y 12 RBD Mean age: 57.6 y 7 PTSD Mean age: 56.7 y |
Cross-Sectional | Not specified |
Dream content/emotions:
|
Abbreviations: Dream Recall Frequency (DRF); Female (F); Hall and Van De Castle method (HVdC); Healthy Controls (HC); idiopathic REM sleep Behavior Disorder (iRBD); Male (M); old Healthy Controls (oHC); Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD); REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD); REM sleep Behavior Disorder Screening Questionnaire (RBDSQ); Sleep Terrors (ST); Sleep-Walkers (SW); Threat Simulation Scale (TSS); Years (y); young Healthy Controls (yHC).
However, these works indicate in RBDs a prevalence of unpleasant and complex dreams [17,28,39,62], specifically containing attacks and violent contents [17,39]. However, this evidence refers to all retrospective dream collection. Indeed, when dream recall was performed immediately after the awakening in the sleep laboratory, these findings were completely reversed, showing in SWs/STs more complex and long dream reports, without significant difference in violent and unpleasant dreams [17].
One particular case regards sleep disorders due to trauma or severe anxiety states, such as PTSD. Despite PTSD not being considered a sleep disorder by the ICSD-3 [1], sleep-related symptoms are common. Specifically, PTSD patients report sleep disturbances, hyperarousal, and sleep movements. Furthermore, intrusive thoughts and images are key features of the PTSD diagnosis, which occur as nightmares during the night [63]. As in RBD, an increased phasic and tonic electromyography (EMG) activity during REM sleep can be observed also in PTSD [64,65,66], caused by similar neuroanatomic abnormalities in both syndromes. Indeed, one of the hypotheses advanced points to a loss of neurons in the locus coeruleus in patients with RBD and with PTSD [40]. Despite RBD and PTSD sharing such clinical similarities, only one study in literature [67] described dream content between these two conditions. Although the authors provided only a descriptive overview not performing any statistical analysis, nevertheless the findings reported seeming relevant. Indeed, 100% of PTSDs with and without RBD recalled dreams containing frightening emotions and unpleasant dreams related to past trauma; on the other hand, RBD patients without PTSD symptoms reported lower rates of frightening dreams (67%) and dreams related to trauma (42%). Conversely, pleasant dreams were reported in 17% of RBDs and never reported by PTSDs (with and without RBD).
This preliminary evidence suggests that nightmares are a PTSD hallmark, beyond the presence of RBD symptoms. However, further works investigating the relationship between nightmare occurrence and EMG activity in these two disorders would be interesting in order to consider similar neuropathological mechanisms underlying RBD and PTSD.
3.4. RBD in Infants
For a long time, it was thought that RBD was a parasomnia affecting particularly elderly men. Nevertheless, a similar prevalence in women [68] and in all ages has been observed over time. In this regard, RBD was also found during childhood and adolescence.
Although most of the clinical features of RBD in older adults also occurred in children, in this last population, specific characteristics are found. Case reports showed that most children suffering from RBD also showed other neurological (i.e., cerebellar tumor [69,70], juvenile PD [71], narcolepsy [9,72,73,74]) and neuropsychological (i.e., autism [9,75], anxiety, depression, obsessive-compulsive disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [9]) disorders. However, many of the main clinical aspects of RBD in pediatrics are not fully known. Indeed, the outcome and the course of this parasomnia in children are unclear since follow-up studies aimed to trace its clinical evolution are still lacking.
Furthermore, assessing symptoms and general clinical features may be quite challenging in this population because of communication problems due to the early age or concomitant handicap conditions [76]. This limitation is particularly relevant in collecting subjective sleep symptoms or self-reported dream contents. In fact, a unique study in the literature [9] assessed dream activity in RBD children, declaring the failure in collecting dream contents in two subjects because they were unable to describe it (See Table 4). However, findings in children confirmed evidence reported in older RBDs, showing high rates of nightmares and vivid frightening dreams involving violence or chasing. Moreover, also in children, the clonazepam treatment leads to the resolution of RBD symptoms, including nightmares.
Table 4.
Sample, design, tools, and findings in the study investigating dreaming in children with RBD.
Study | Sample | Design | Dream Measures | Main Findings |
---|---|---|---|---|
[9] |
15 RBD Mean age: 9.5 y Gender: 11 M/15 F |
Retrospective Descriptive Case Series |
Not specified |
Dream Content
|
Abbreviations: Female (F); Male (M); REM sleep Behavior Disorder (RBD); Years (y).
Notably, we owe the current knowledge about oneiric activity in children with RBD to a single study [9]. Although infant RBD is a rare condition, further studies, especially those involving longitudinal design, will help understand the pathophysiology behind this condition and the long-term implications of childhood RBD.
4. Conclusions and Future Perspectives
To sum up, the results reported in our review suggest a double interpretation of dreaming in RBD, depending on the design adopted by the studies: retrospective or prospective. Indeed, retrospective studies mainly point to RBD as characterized by unpleasant dreams and nightmares, containing animal or people attacks, violence, and negative emotions. These features arise mostly when RBD patients are compared to patients with other parasomnias, such as SW and ST, and patients with neurodegenerative symptoms. This evidence suggests a potential clinical relevance of aggressive contents and high DRF in the pathophysiology of RBD and a potential role of oneiric activity as a marker to track neurodegenerative processes.
However, prospective studies do not confirm this framework, suggesting a similar oneiric activity in idiopathic and secondary RBD, and between RBD and other parasomnias. The discussion of findings leads us to the “recall bias” phenomena, which could obscure the potential association between oneiric features and this REM parasomnia. Moreover, despite that there has been a surge in research about several aspects of RBD in recent years, from this review it can be noted that there are few studies in the literature aimed at exploring dream activity in RBD. Although oneiric activity is a central feature of RBD, most of the studies discussed in this paper assessed dreaming in patients without standardized protocols and only with a descriptive approach.
These methodological limitations bring out the need to deepen the issue of dreaming in RBD. Thus, we believe that further steps in this research area should be done in future studies considering (a) the application of robust experimental protocols and prospective tools to collect dreams; (b) the relationship between dream features in RBD and the motor manifestations or the EMG activity features during REM sleep; (c) how treatment for RBD symptoms affects oneiric activity; (d) dream features in other populations in which RBD is less frequent, but still presents, such as in female patients and children; (e) oneiric features as potential indexes to operate differential diagnosis between RBD and other disorder that mimic this REM parasomnia.
Overall, investigations in these directions, applying more controlled experimental designs, will offer relevant clinical insights. Indeed, in a translational view, dream research, which until now has been a niche of empirical research, could provide knowledge about RBD useful in clinical settings. Indeed, although available data are still not robust enough, in the future, dream features in RBD could help clinicians to monitor the severity of the disease and the possible conversion in synucleinopathies, but also to operate a differential diagnosis between RBD and other parasomnias. Our work suggests the relevance of considering dream features in clinical settings, supervised by general practitioners and sleep specialists. Indeed, monitoring the dream frequency and the dream contents with negative valence may be useful to track the presence of comorbidities between RBD and nightmare disorders for a first and low-cost screening. Moreover, based on the continuity hypothesis, which suggests a permanence between wake and sleep thoughts [77], considering the relationship between violent and frightening dreams and waking experiences could improve the patient’s well-being. Moreover, since changes in dream recall seem dependent on cognitive deterioration [78], monitoring alterations in the DRF in RBD patients may be useful to evaluate the relationship between the frequency of dream recalls and the neurodegenerative processes onset.
Despite the promising translational value of dreams features in clinical settings, these tips should be considered with caution, given these data’s novelty and weak points.
Author Contributions
E.F., S.S. and L.D.G. conceived the idea and the conceptualization of this idea. E.F. wrote the final manuscript under the supervision of S.S., M.G. and V.A. provided feedback to the manuscript, contributing to the final manuscript, data curation, and methodology. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Institutional Review Board Statement
Not applicable.
Informed Consent Statement
Not applicable.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.
Conflicts of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Funding Statement
This research received no external funding.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Sateia M.J. International classification of sleep disorders. Chest. 2014;146:1387–1394. doi: 10.1378/chest.14-0970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schredl M., Wittmann L. Dreaming: A psychological view. Dreaming. 2004;484:92. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Alfonsi V., D’Atri A., Scarpelli S., Mangiaruga A., De Gennaro L. Sleep talking: A viable access to mental processes during sleep. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019;44:12–22. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schenck C.H., Mahowald M.W. REM sleep behavior disorder: Clinical, developmental, and neuroscience perspectives 16 years after its formal identification in SLEEP. Sleep. 2002;25:120–138. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.2.120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Galbiati A., Verga L., Giora E., Zucconi M., Ferini-Strambi L. The risk of neurodegeneration in REM sleep behavior disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Sleep Med. Rev. 2019;43:37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2018.09.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Ferini-Strambi L., Fasiello E., Sforza M., Salsone M., Galbiati A. Neuropsychological, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging biomarkers for REM behavior disorder. Expert Rev. Neurother. 2019;19:1069–1087. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2019.1640603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.De Gennaro L., Lanteri O., Piras F., Scarpelli S., Assogna F., Ferrara M., Caltagirone C., Spalletta G. Dopaminergic system and dream recall: An MRI study in Parkinson’s disease patients. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2016;37:1136–1147. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23095. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Borek L.L., Kohn R., Friedman J.H. Phenomenology of dreams in Parkinson’s disease. Mov. Disord. 2007;22:198–202. doi: 10.1002/mds.21255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lloyd R., Tippmann-Peikert M., Slocumb N., Kotagal S. Characteristics of REM sleep behavior disorder in childhood. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2012;8:127–131. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.1760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Postuma R.B., Gagnon J.F., Vendette M., Fantini M.L., Massicotte-Marquez J., Montplaisir J. Quantifying the risk of neurodegenerative disease in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology. 2009;72:1296–1300. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000340980.19702.6e. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bonakis A., Howard R.S., Ebrahim I.O., Merritt S., Williams A. REM sleep behaviour disorder (RBD) and its associations in young patients. Sleep Med. 2009;10:641–645. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2008.07.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Zhou J., Zhang J., Du L., Li Z., Li Y., Lei F., Wing Y.K., Kushida C.A., Zhou D., Tang X. Characteristics of early-and late-onset rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in China: A case–control study. Sleep Med. 2014;15:654–660. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Olson E.J., Boeve B.F., Silber M.H. Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder: Demographic, clinical and laboratory findings in 93 cases. Brain. 2000;123:331–339. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.2.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Fernández-Arcos A., Iranzo A., Serradell M., Gaig C., Santamaria J. The clinical phenotype of idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder at presentation: A study in 203 consecutive patients. Sleep. 2016;39:121–132. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Kang S.H., Yoon I.Y., Lee S.D., Han J.W., Kim T.H., Kim K.W. REM sleep behavior disorder in the Korean elderly population: Prevalence and clinical characteristics. Sleep. 2013;36:1147–1152. doi: 10.5665/sleep.2874. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Fantini M.L., Corona A., Clerici S., Ferini-Strambi L. Aggressive dream content without daytime aggressiveness in REM sleep behavior disorder. Neurology. 2005;65:1010–1015. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000179346.39655.e0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Uguccioni G., Golmard J.L., de Fontréaux A.N., Leu-Semenescu S., Brion A., Arnulf I. Fight or flight? Dream content during sleepwalking/sleep terrors vs. rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2013;14:391–398. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2013.01.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lee H.G., Choi J.W., Lee Y.J., Jeong D.U. Depressed REM sleep behavior disorder patients are less likely to recall enacted dreams than non-depressed ones. Psychiatry Investig. 2016;13:227. doi: 10.4306/pi.2016.13.2.227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Takeuchi N., Sasai-Sakuma T., Inoue Y. Gender differences in clinical findings and α-synucleiopathy-related markers in patients with idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2020;66:216–219. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2019.11.1261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Zhou J., Zhang J., Li Y., Du L., Li Z., Lei F., Wing Y.K., Kushida C.A., Zhou D., Tang X. Gender differences in REM sleep behavior disorder: A clinical and polysomnographic study in China. Sleep Med. 2015;16:414–418. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Godin I., Montplaisir J., Nielsen T. Dreaming and nightmares in REM sleep behavior disorder. Dreaming. 2015;25:257. doi: 10.1037/drm0000011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Herlin B., Leu-Semenescu S., Chaumereuil C., Arnulf I. Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: A REM sleep behaviour disorder model. J. Sleep Res. 2015;24:602–609. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.McCarter S.J., Louis E.K.S., Boswell C.L., Dueffert L.G., Slocumb N., Boeve B.F., Silber M.H., Olson E.J., Morgenthaler T.I., Tippmann-Peikert M. Factors associated with injury in REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2014;15:1332–1338. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.06.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kunz D., Bes F. Melatonin as a therapy in REM sleep behavior disorder patients: An open-labeled pilot study on the possible influence of melatonin on REM-sleep regulation. Mov. Disord. 1999;14:507–511. doi: 10.1002/1531-8257(199905)14:3<507::AID-MDS1021>3.0.CO;2-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kunz D., Mahlberg R. A two-part, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial of exogenous melatonin in REM sleep behaviour disorder. J. Sleep Res. 2010;19:591–596. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00848.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Li S.X., Lam S.P., Zhang J., Yu M.W.M., Chan J.W.Y., Liu Y., Lam V.K.H., Ho C.K.W., Zhou J., Wing Y.K. A prospective, naturalistic follow-up study of treatment outcomes with clonazepam in rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2016;21:114–120. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.12.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Godin I., Montplaisir J., Gagnon J.F., Nielsen T. Alexithymia associated with nightmare distress in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep. 2013;36:1957–1962. doi: 10.5665/sleep.3238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Haridi M., Weyn Banningh S., Clé M., Leu-Semenescu S., Vidailhet M., Arnulf I. Is there a common motor dysregulation in sleepwalking and REM sleep behaviour disorder? J. Sleep Res. 2017;26:614–662. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12544. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Lam S.P., Li S.X., Chan J.W., Mok V., Tsoh J., Chan A., Yu M.W.M., Lau C.Y., Zhang J., Lam V., et al. Does rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder exist in psychiatric populations? A clinical and polysomnographic case–control study. Sleep Med. 2013;14:788–794. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Li S.X., Wing Y.K., Lam S.P., Zhang J., Yu M.W.M., Ho C.K.W., Tsoh J., Mok V. Validation of a new REM sleep behavior disorder questionnaire (RBDQ-HK) Sleep Med. 2010;11:43–48. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2009.06.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.You S., Moon H.J., Do S.Y., Wing Y.K., Sunwoo J.S., Jung K.Y., Cho Y.W. The REM sleep behavior disorder screening questionnaire: Validation study of the Korean version (RBDQ-KR) J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2017;13:1429–1433. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.6840. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Sunwoo J.S., Cha K.S., Byun J.I., Kim T.J., Jun J.S., Lim J.A., Lee S.T., Jung K.H., Park K.I., Chu K., et al. Abnormal activation of motor cortical network during phasic REM sleep in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep. 2019;42:zsy227. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jun J.S., Kim R., Jung H.M., Byun J.I., Seok J.M., Kim T.J., Lim J.A., Sunwoo J.S., Kim H.J., Schenck C.H., et al. Emotion dysregulation in idiopathic rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep. 2020;43:zsz224. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Sunwoo J.S., Kim Y.J., Byun J.I., Kim T.J., Jun J.S., Lee S.T., Jung K.H., Park K.I., Chu K., Kim M. Comorbid depression is associated with a negative treatment response in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. J. Clin. Neurol. 2020;16:261. doi: 10.3988/jcn.2020.16.2.261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Neikrug A.B., Avanzino J.A., Liu L., Maglione J.E., Natarajan L., Corey-Bloom J., Palmer B.W., Loredo J.S., Ancoli-Israel S. Parkinson’s disease and REM sleep behavior disorder result in increased non-motor symptoms. Sleep Med. 2014;15:959–966. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.04.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Valli K., Frauscher B., Peltomaa T., Gschliesser V., Revonsuo A., Högl B. Dreaming furiously? A sleep laboratory study on the dream content of people with Parkinson’s disease and with or without rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2015;16:419–427. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.10.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Fernández-Arcos A., Morenas-Rodríguez E., Santamaria J., Sánchez-Valle R., Lladó A., Gaig C., Lleó A., Iranzo A. Clinical and video-polysomnographic analysis of rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder and other sleep disturbances in dementia with Lewy bodies. Sleep. 2019;42:zsz086. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsz086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Antelmi E., Lippolis M., Biscarini F., Tinazzi M., Plazzi G. REM sleep behavior disorder: Mimics and variants. Sleep Med. Rev. 2021;60:101515. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2021.101515. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Iranzo A., Santamaría J. Severe obstructive sleep apnea/hypopnea mimicking REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep. 2005;28:203–206. doi: 10.1093/sleep/28.2.203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Garcia-Rill E. Disorders of the reticular activating system. Med. Hypotheses. 1997;49:379–387. doi: 10.1016/S0306-9877(97)90083-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Bodkin C.L., Schenck C.H. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in women: Relevance to general and specialty medical practice. J. Women’s Health. 2009;18:1955–1963. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2008.1348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Revonsuo A. The reinterpretation of dreams: An evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming. Behav. Brain Sci. 2000;23:793–1121. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00004015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Iranzo A., Ratti P.L., Casanova-Molla J., Serradell M., Vilaseca I., Santamaria J. Excessive muscle activity increases over time in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep. 2009;32:1149–1153. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.9.1149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Pujol M., Pujol J., Alonso T., Fuentes A., Pallerola M., Freixenet J., Barbé F., Salamero M., Santamaría J., Iranzo A. Idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder in the elderly Spanish community: A primary care center study with a two-stage design using video-polysomnography. Sleep Med. 2017;40:116–121. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Leclair-Visonneau L., Oudiette D., Gaymard B., Leu-Semenescu S., Arnulf I. Do the eyes scan dream images during rapid eye movement sleep? Evidence from the rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder model. Brain. 2010;133:1737–1746. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mariotti P., Quaranta D., Di Giacopo R., Bentivoglio A.R., Mazza M., Martini A., Canestri J., Della Marca G. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: A window on the emotional world of Parkinson disease. Sleep. 2015;38:287–294. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Oudiette D., Constantinescu I., Leclair-Visonneau L., Vidailhet M., Schwartz S., Arnulf I. Evidence for the re-enactment of a recently learned behavior during sleepwalking. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e18056. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.D’Agostino A., Manni R., Limosani I., Terzaghi M., Cavallotti S., Scarone S. Challenging the myth of REM sleep behavior disorder: No evidence of heightened aggressiveness in dreams. Sleep Med. 2012;13:714–719. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2012.01.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Cavallotti S., Stein H.C., Savarese M., Terzaghi M., D’Agostino A. Aggressiveness in the dreams of drug-naïve and clonazepam-treated patients with isolated REM sleep behavior disorder. Sleep Med. 2022;92:19–23. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2022.02.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.De Cock V.C., Vidailhet M., Leu S., Texeira A., Apartis E., Elbaz A., Roze E., Willer J.C., Agid Y., Arnulf I. Restoration of normal motor control in Parkinson’s disease during REM sleep. Brain. 2007;130:450–456. doi: 10.1093/brain/awl363. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Valli K., Frauscher B., Gschliesser V., Wolf E., Falkenstetter T., Schoenwald S.V., Ehrmann L., Zangerl A., Marti I., Boesch S.M., et al. Can observers link dream content to behaviours in rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder? A cross-sectional experimental pilot study. J. Sleep Res. 2012;21:21–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00938.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Muntean M.L., Trenkwalder C., Walters A.S., Mollenhauer B., Sixel-Döring F. REM sleep behavioral events and dreaming. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2015;11:537–541. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.4698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Schenck C.H., Bundlie S.R., Patterson A.L., Mahowald M.W. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder: A treatable parasomnia affecting older adults. JAMA. 1987;257:1786–1789. doi: 10.1001/jama.1987.03390130104038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Iranzo A., Molinuevo J.L., Santamaría J., Serradell M., Martí M.J., Valldeoriola F., Tolosa E. Rapid-eye-movement sleep behaviour disorder as an early marker for a neurodegenerative disorder: A descriptive study. Lancet Neurol. 2006;5:572–577. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70476-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Oksenberg A., Radwan H., Arons E., Hoffenbach D., Behroozi B. Rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder: A sleep disturbance affecting mainly older men. Isr. J. Psychiatry Relat. Sci. 2002;39:28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tassinari C.A., Rubboli G., Gardella E., Cantalupo G., Calandra-Buonaura G., Vedovello M., Alessandria M., Gandini G., Cinotti S., Zamponi N., et al. Central pattern generators for a common semiology in fronto-limbic seizures and in parasomnias. A neuroethologic approach. Neurol. Sci. 2005;26((Suppl. S3)):S225–S232. doi: 10.1007/s10072-005-0492-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Bugalho P., Paiva T. Dream features in the early stages of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neural Transm. 2011;118:1613–1619. doi: 10.1007/s00702-011-0679-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Arnulf I. Rapid-Eye-Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder. Springer; Cham, Switzerland: 2019. RBD: A window into the dreaming process; pp. 223–242. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Manni R., Terzaghi M., Ratti P.L., Repetto A., Zangaglia R., Pacchetti C. Hallucinations and REM sleep behaviour disorder in Parkinson’s disease: Dream imagery intrusions and other hypotheses. Conscious. Cognit. 2011;20:1021–1026. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Boeve B.F., Silber M.H., Saper C.B., Ferman T.J., Dickson D.W., Parisi J.E., Benarroch E.E., Ahlskog J.E., Smith G.E., Caselli R.C., et al. Pathophysiology of REM sleep behaviour disorder and relevance to neurodegenerative disease. Brain. 2007;130:2770–2788. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Postuma R.B., Gagnon J.F., Rompré S., Montplaisir J.Y. Severity of REM atonia loss in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder predicts Parkinson disease. Neurology. 2010;74:239–244. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181ca0166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Chang Y., Gu Z., Zhan S., Zhang H., Li Y., An J., Ding Y., Nie X., Chan P. Validation of the Beijing version of the REM sleep behavior disorder questionnaire (RBDQ-Beijing) in a mainland Chinese cohort. Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 2014;234:7–15. doi: 10.1620/tjem.234.7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing; Arlington, VA, USA: 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Hefez A., Metz L., Lavie P. Long-term effects of extreme situational stress on sleep and dreaming. Am. J. Psychiatry. 1987;144:344–347. doi: 10.1176/ajp.144.3.344. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Ross R.J., Ball W.A., Dinges D.F., Kribbs N.B., Morrison A.R., Silver S.M., Mulvaney F.D. Motor dysfunction during sleep in posttraumatic stress disorder. Sleep. 1994;17:723–732. doi: 10.1093/sleep/17.8.723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mysliwiec V., O’Reilly B., Polchinski J., Kwon H.P., Germain A., Roth B.J. Trauma associated sleep disorder: A proposed parasomnia encompassing disruptive nocturnal behaviors, nightmares, and REM without atonia in trauma survivors. J. Clin. Sleep Med. 2014;10:1143–1148. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.4120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Husain A.M., Miller P.P., Carwile S.T. REM sleep behavior disorder: Potential relationship to post-traumatic stress disorder. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2001;18:148–157. doi: 10.1097/00004691-200103000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Haba-Rubio J., Frauscher B., Marques-Vidal P., Toriel J., Tobback N., Andries D., Preisig M., Vollenweider P., Postuma R., Heinzer R. Prevalence and determinants of rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in the general population. Sleep. 2018;41:zsx197. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx197. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Barros-Ferreira M., Chodkiewicz J.P., Lairy G.C., Salzarulo P. Disorganized relations of tonic and phasic events of REM sleep in a case of brain-stem tumor. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1975;38:202–207. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(75)90232-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Schenck C.H., Bundlie S.R., Smith S.A., Ettinger M.G., Mahowald M.W. REM behavior disorder in a 10-year-old girl and aperiodic REM and NREM sleep movements in an 8-year-old brother. Sleep Res. 1986;15:162. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rye D.B., Johnston L.H., Watts R.L., Bliwise D.L. Juvenile Parkinson’s disease with REM sleep behavior disorder, sleepiness, and daytime REM onset. Neurology. 1999;53:1868–1870. doi: 10.1212/WNL.53.8.1868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Turner R., Allen W.T. REM sleep behavior disorder associated with narcolepsy in an adolescent: A case report. Sleep Res. 1990;19:302. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Schenck C.H., Mahowald M.W. Motor dyscontrol in narcolepsy: Rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep without atonia and REM sleep behavior disorder. Ann. Neurol. 1992;32:3–10. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Nevsimalova S., Prihodova I., Kemlink D., Lin L., Mignot E. REM behavior disorder (RBD) can be one of the first symptoms of childhood narcolepsy. Sleep Med. 2007;8:784–786. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2006.11.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Thirumalai S.S., Shubin R.A., Robinson R. Rapid eye movement sleep behavior disorder in children with autism. J. Child Neurol. 2002;17:173–178. doi: 10.1177/088307380201700304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Stores G. Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder in children and adolescents. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 2008;50:728–732. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Domhoff G.W. The invasion of the concept snatchers: The origins, distortions, and future of the continuity hypothesis. Dreaming. 2017;27:14. doi: 10.1037/drm0000047. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Scarpelli S., Bartolacci C., D’Atri A., Gorgoni M., De Gennaro L. Mental sleep activity and disturbing dreams in the lifespan. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health. 2019;16:3658. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16193658. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.