Abstract
Child maltreatment has many well-documented lasting effects on children. Among its consequences, it affects children’s recognition of emotions. More and more studies are recognizing the lasting effect that a history of maltreatment can have on emotion recognition. A systematic literature review was conducted to better understand this relationship. The Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) protocol was used and four databases were searched, MEDLINE/PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE, and FRANCIS, using three cross-referenced key words: child abuse, emotion recognition, and adults. The search process identified 23 studies that met the inclusion criteria. The review highlights the wide variety of measures used to assess child maltreatment as well as the different protocols used to measure emotion recognition. The results indicate that adults with a history of childhood maltreatment show a differentiated reaction to happiness, anger, and fear. Happiness is less detected, whereas negative emotions are recognized more rapidly and at a lower intensity compared to adults not exposed to such traumatic events. Emotion recognition is also related to greater brain activation for the maltreated group. However, the results are less consistent for adults who also have a diagnosis of mental health problems. The systematic review found that maltreatment affects the perception of emotions expressed on both adult and child faces. However, more research is needed to better understand how a history of maltreatment is related to adults’ perception of children’s emotions.
Keywords: childhood maltreatment, emotion recognition, perception, cycle of maltreatment, systematic review
The intergenerational cycle of maltreatment is a complex phenomenon. Maltreated children are twice as likely to grow up in families where their mothers have been maltreated during childhood (Bartlett et al., 2017). According to an umbrella review of meta-analyses, the factor with the largest predictive effect size for child maltreatment is having a parent with a history of maltreatment (van IJzendoorn et al., 2020). However, there appears to be considerable variation in the intergenerational trajectory of maltreatment. A scoping review by Madigan et al. (2019) found that prevalence rates of intergenerational transmission range from 7% to 88%. Furthermore, the relationship between the form of maltreatment to which a parent was exposed as a child and the form that they may perpetuate is not necessarily direct. A history of maltreatment increases, by the same odds, the risk of homotypical (e.g., abuse to abuse) and hetereotypical (e.g., abuse to neglect) intergenerational transmission. Using a multigenerational multi-informant approach, Buisman et al. (2020) found that transmission appears to be more systematic for abuse than for neglect.
Many factors could contribute to the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment. A recent meta-analysis established that once they become parents, adults with a history of child maltreatment demonstrate more negative parenting and a lower quality in the parent–child relationship than adults without such past experiences (Savage et al., 2019). Similarly, a systematic review by Greene et al. (2020) found that parents with a history of maltreatment make greater usage of negative parenting practices and show a diminished propensity to use positive ones. These characteristics refer to sensitive parenting behavior. Ainsworth (1979) defined sensitivity by dividing it into three components, namely, the parents’ ability to (1) perceive their child signals, (2) interpret them properly, and (3) respond in an appropriate and quick manner. Few studies have examined the relationship between a history of maltreatment and the first component of Ainsworth’s model, namely how parents perceive the emotions displayed by children.
Studies investigating the effect of maltreatment on emotional development have primarily examined children’s perceptual abilities. In a series of experiments published in the early 2000s, Pollak and his colleagues demonstrated that maltreated children can identify anger and fear with less sensory information than nonmaltreated children. Numerous studies have followed to better understand this phenomenon. These studies have revealed the complexity of the effect of maltreatment on children’s perception of emotions. For instance, physical abuse is the form of maltreatment that has shown the most consistent results across studies. Children who have experienced this form of abuse tend to show hypersensitivity to anger and fear. They require less perceptual information to recognize facial expressions of anger compared to nonabused children (Pollak & Sinha, 2002) and show more attentional biases and shorter reaction times to both emotions (Assed et al., 2020; da Silva Ferreira et al., 2014; Harms et al., 2019; Masten et al., 2008). While this hypervigilance to fear and anger may help abused children assess their environment and adapt their behaviors in order to prevent further abuse, it may be inappropriate in nonthreatening contexts (Assed et al., 2020; Pollak, 2008).
A recent meta-analysis concluded that children exposed to neglect also have differentiated emotion recognition skills, evidenced by shorter reaction times when asked to identify negative expressions (Assed et al., 2020). They demonstrate a lower understanding of the negative emotions of anger and sadness (Shipman et al., 2005). For instance, they show a tendency to confuse all negative emotions with sadness (Pollak et al., 2000). Researchers suggest that growing up in less stimulating environments may impair children’s ability to recognize the emotional facial expressions of others (Pollak et al., 2000).
It is important to understand how childhood maltreatment can affect emotional recognition in adulthood. A growing number of studies are being conducted to verify the effect of childhood maltreatment on adults’ perception of emotions. In addition, recent techniques used to understand emotion recognition may offer new insights on the long-term effects of childhood maltreatment. These advances in the field warrant a review to examine the effects specifically associated with childhood maltreatment, examining whether different methodologies provide new insights, and whether perceptual abilities differ in relation to adult vs. child face stimuli.
Objective
The main objective is to explore the link between a history of childhood maltreatment and adults’ perception of emotions. To do so, a systematic review of research published on the topic was conducted. To our knowledge, this is the first systematic review to examine this relationship among adult subjects specifically. Particular attention was given to reviewing studies conducted with adult victims of childhood maltreatment, which is a specific form of early adverse experience (Atzl et al., 2019; Mersky et al., 2017). A distinction was also made between responses to emotions expressed on adult and child faces.
Method
Search Strategy
A systematic review of the literature was conducted according to the PRISMA protocol (Moher et al., 2015). Three cross-reference key words (childhood maltreatment, emotion recognition, and adults) were searched in four databases: MEDLINE/PubMed, PsycINFO, EMBASE, and FRANCIS. All related terms for each of the three categories of interest were identified and verified by two independent judges. Agreements were included in the search. A full list of key words used is shown in Table 1. The search returned a total of 372 articles.
Table 1.
Key Words Searched in Databases.
Main Categories (Combined With AND) | Synonyms (Combined With OR) |
---|---|
Childhood maltreatment |
|
Emotion recognition |
|
Adults |
|
Study Selection
Research findings were reviewed in three stages. First, duplicates were removed, leading to a total of 285 articles. Second, an initial selection of articles was conducted based on titles and abstracts. Two reviewers independently assessed the eligibility of these articles for use in the current paper. Of the 285 articles, 44 were retained based on title and abstract screening (see Figure 1 for PRISMA diagram). Excluded publications included non-English papers (n = 2), books (n = 2), thesis papers (n = 12), conference papers (n = 24), and articles unrelated to the current study’s research question (n = 201), such as studies examining the link between maltreatment and emotion processing in children and/or teenagers. Discrepancies (13 of 285 articles) were discussed until a consensus was reached (K = 0.80). The remaining 44 articles were subjected to in-depth evaluation by both independent reviewers to verify that they met predefined inclusion criteria: (a) peer-reviewed papers published in 2019 or before, (b) empirical studies and literature reviews focused on the facial emotion processing of child and/or adult stimuli, and (c) inclusion of a sample comprised of adults with a history of childhood maltreatment. Both auto-revealed and substantiated cases of maltreatment were accepted. Maltreatment experiences consisted of at least one form of abuse and/or neglect. We excluded studies that assessed the broader concept of early stress, such as experiencing bullying, the death of a parent or an illness. Disagreements (6 of 44 articles) were resolved through discussion and consensus was reached in all cases.
Figure 1.
PRISMA diagram for systematic review.
Among the 44 studies assessed for eligibility, 21 were excluded in total, due to unavailability (n = 1), absence of a task with emotions or use of nonfacial stimuli (n = 5), wrong population (e.g., veterans, maltreating parents; n = 4), and measure of early stress unrelated to abuse or neglect (n = 7). Moreover, three reviews of the literature were excluded. One did not find any studies on maltreatment and emotional processing in adults (Hart & Rubia, 2012); the two other reviews examined the link between childhood maltreatment and psychopathology (Jaffee, 2017; Teicher & Samson, 2016). Finally, a meta-analysis (Hein & Monk, 2017) was excluded given the variability of the studies in terms of participants and measurements. This search process yielded 23 studies that met inclusion criteria (K = 0.72). One additional article (i.e., English et al., 2018) was identified through manual search by both reviewers, leading to a total of 24 studies for the review (see Figure 1).
Data Extraction
Data were extracted to a standardized data collection Excel sheet by the author responsible for the study selection and verified by one of the other authors. This covered research objectives, details about participants, measures, analyses, relevant findings, and study limitations (see Table 2 for a brief description of the articles included in synthesis).
Table 2.
Measures and Tasks Used in Studies Included in Synthesis.
Author (Year) | Study Subject | Maltreatment Measure | Emotions | Database | Stimuli | Task |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Brüne et al. (2013) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Happiness Neutral |
Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces database | Adult faces | Dot-probe task |
Dannlowski et al. (2012) | Neurological | CTQ | Fear Anger |
Ekman and Friesen stimulus set. | Adult faces | Matching task (faces and shapes) |
Davis et al. (2014) | Neurological | CTQ | Threat-neutral Happy-neutral Neutral-neutral |
Adult faces | Dot-probe task | |
Dayton et al. (2016) | Attentional bias | CTQ | Anger Fear Distress |
IFEEL task (Emde et al., 1993) | Infant faces | Emotion discrimination |
Demers et al. (2018) | Perception Behavioral |
CM records | Anger Fear |
Standardized set of emotional faces (Ekman & Friesen, 1978) | Adult faces | Matching task (faces and shapes) |
English et al. (2018) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness |
Cohn-Kanade Facial Expression Database | Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
Fang et al. (2019) | Neurological | CTQ | Anger Disgust Fear Happiness |
Chinese Facial Affective Picture System | Adult faces | Dot-probe task |
Fani et al. (2011) | Perception | CTQ | Threat-neutral Happy-neutral Neutral-neutral |
Adult faces | Dot-probe task | |
Gibb et al. (2009) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Sadness Happiness Neutral |
NimStim Face Stimulus Set Standardized stimulus set (Matsumoto & Ekman, 1988) |
Adult faces | Dot-probe task |
Gong et al. (2019) | Neurological | CTQ | Anger Disgust Fear Happiness Neutral |
The Chinese picture collection of emotional faces from the Institute of Psychology of the Chinese Academy of Sciences. | Adult faces | Dot-probe task |
Hartling et al. (2019) | Genetics Perception |
CTQ | Disgust Fear Surprise Happiness + 20 Complex emotions |
Face Puzzle task—explicit (Kliemann et al., 2013) | Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
Jedd et al. (2015) | Neurological | CM records | Anger Fear |
Standardized set of emotional faces (Ekman & Friesen, 1976). | Adults faces | Matching task (faces and shapes) |
Johnson et al. (2010) | Genetics Perception |
CTQ | Anger-neutral Happy-neutral Sad-neutral |
NimStim set of facial expressions (Tottenham et al. 2011) | Adult faces | Dot-probe task |
Kim et al. (2014) | Neurological | AAI | Sadness Happiness |
Still face images were captured from the videos recorded at laboratory | Infant faces | Passive viewing |
Kirkham & Levita (2019) | Perception | CATS | Three (happy, sad, and neutral) | FACES database (Ebner et al., 2010) | Adult faces | Answer questions on a scale (approach or avoid the person/level of anger and happiness perceived) |
Neukel et al. (2019) | Neurological | CECA interview | Three (sad, happy, and neutral) | Still face images were captured from the videos recorded at laboratory | Infant faces | Emotion discrimination |
Olsavsky et al. (2019) | Neurological Perception |
RFQ | Anger Fear Distress Happiness Neutral |
Standardized photographic stimuli (Kim et al., 2014;
Strathearn & Kim, 2013; Strathearn et al., 2008) Standardized photographs (Ekman & Friesen, 1976) |
Child faces Adult faces |
Gender identification Answer questions on a scale |
Peters et al. (2018) | Neurological | CTQ | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness |
Faces were selected from the Gur emotional faces set (Gur et al., 2002). | Adult faces | Matching task (faces and shapes) |
Redlich et al. (2015) | Genetics Neurological |
CTQ | Anger Fear |
Adult faces | Matching task (faces and shapes) | |
Russo et al. (2015) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Disgust Fear Sadness Surprise Happiness |
Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Robbins et al., 1994) | Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
Schwaiger et al. (2019) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness |
Emotion recognition task by Chen & Jin
(2015) The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) |
Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
Suzuki et al. (2015) | Perception | CTQ | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness Neutral |
Ekman faces (Ekman & Friesen, 1976) | Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
van Harmelen et al. (2013) | Neurological | NEMESIS trauma interview | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness Neutral |
Karolinska Directed Emotional Faces System (Lundqvist et al., 1998) | Adult faces | Gender identification |
Veague & Hooley (2014) | Perception | CMIS | Anger Fear Happiness |
Ekman and Friesen (1976) | Adult faces | Emotion discrimination |
Note. LEC = Life-Events Checklist; CTQ = Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; ELSQ = Early Life Stress Questionnaire; CAPS = Clinician Administrated PTSD Scale; PCL-S = Posttraumatic Symptom Checklist–Short Form; APES = Adolescent Perceived Events Scale; CATS = Child Abuse and Trauma Scale; RFQ = Risky Families Questionnaire; CECA = childhood experience of care and abuse; BBTS = Brief Betrayal-Trauma Survey; AAI = Adult Attachment Interview; PTSD = Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder.
Results
Description of Study and Subjects’ Characteristics
All studies included in our systematic review were empirical studies. Among the 24 studies selected, almost half (11 of 24, 46%) examined associations between childhood maltreatment and adults’ responses to emotions, namely their attentional or interpretation biases or their abilities to discriminate between emotions. Ten articles (42%) described research documenting how childhood maltreatment affects adults’ brain response to facial emotional expressions of emotion. Finally, three studies (12%) examined the relationship between genetic factors and early adversity on emotion processing.
Study samples ranged from 26 to 395 participants. Approximately 21% of studies (5 of 24 had samples of 50 or fewer people, eight publications (33%) had between 51 and 100 participants, 11 studies (46%) had more than 100 participants. Six of the 24 (25%) studies were conducted with samples of predominantly male adults. A few publications were conducted with samples of parents (4 of 24, 17%) or undergraduate students (4 of 24, 17%). Finally, 50% of articles (12 of 24) included in our review compared adults with a history of childhood maltreatment to adults without such a history.
Participants’ mean age ranged between 18 and 52 years old. Only 12% of studies (3 of 24) had participants between the ages of 18 and 20 years. Most samples reported a mean age between either 20 and 30 years old (8 of 24, 33%) or 30 and 45 years old (9 of 24, 37%). Three publications (12%) were conducted with participants over 45 years old, and one (4%) simply indicated that participants ranged between 18 and 68 years old. Only 13 studies (54%) reported information about the ethnic background of their participants. Of these, six of the samples (25%) were predominantly White, Caucasian, or from European descent, while seven (29%) were predominantly African, Asian, Hispanic, Black, or Multiracial. Five studies (21%) were conducted with participants of low-socioeconomic status.
Among the 24 studies, five (21%) screened participants for current or past mental illness problems and excluded those who met this criterion. Other studies (8 of 24, 33%) examined the combined effect of childhood maltreatment and mental illness (two borderline personality disorder, one bipolar disorder, two depression, one schizotypy, and two other psychopathology) on emotion processing. Eleven (46%) studies did not report information about participants’ mental health.
Maltreatment
Two thirds of the studies (16 of 24, 67%) in our systematic review used the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (Bernstein et al., 2003) which measures five types of maltreatment experiences. Three types are related to abuse, namely, physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, whereas the two others are forms of neglect, physical and emotional. In their study, van Harmelen et al. (2013) used the NEMESIS interview (De Graff et al., 2002) to measure the same dimensions, apart from physical neglect, which is not documented. Veague & Hooley (2014) also used an interview protocol composed of 11 questions tapping on physical, sexual, and emotional abuse as well as witnessing domestic violence. Neukel and colleagues (2019) administered the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse Interview (CECA; Bifulco et al., 1994) to participants to assess childhood abuse experiences (physical and/or sexual). Two studies relied on child protection reports as indicators of child maltreatment (Demers et al., 2018; Jedd et al., 2015). One study used the Child Abuse and Trauma Scale (CATS; Sanders & Becker-Lausen, 1995) which measures physical and emotional abuse and neglect (Kirkham & Levita, 2019). Olsavsky et al. (2019) used three different questions, one for emotional abuse (being verbally insulted or threatened), one for physical abuse (being pushed, grabbed, slapped, or shoved), and one concerning the exposure to domestic violence. Finally, Kim et al. (2014) used the Adult Attachment Interview (AAI; George et al., 1996) to screen for adults with unresolved traumas, such as experiences of loss and/or abuse.
Stimuli
Adults or child faces
Most studies investigating the link between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition have exposed participants to adult faces. Among the 24 studies included in our review, only four used child faces as stimuli. Within these four studies, two different types of stimuli were used. On one hand, participants were exposed to a set of pictures representing different expressions of emotion performed by children in laboratory settings (Dayton et al., 2016; Olsavsky et al., 2019). On the other hand, mothers were exposed to pictures of their own child. These pictures were extracted from videos of their child by independent judges who assigned each one to a specific emotion category, namely, happy, sad, or neutral (Kim et al., 2014; Neukel et al., 2019).
Emotions
Face database
Most studies used a validated face database depicting different emotions. Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) database of adult faces was used in some studies, either as is (e.g., Dannlowski et al., 2012; Demers et al., 2018) or to create morphed emotions (e.g., Suzuki et al., 2015). Other standardized validated databases were also used, such as FACES (e.g., Kirkham & Levita, 2019), which presents a set of faces showing different emotions created by Ebner et al. (2010). Certain studies used the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (Robbins et al., 1994), a computer-generated paradigm for the recognition of six basic facial emotional expressions (Russo et al., 2015). The Karolinska Directed Emotional Face database developed by Lundqvist et al. (1998) was also used in van Harmelen et al. (2013). Few research studies (Demers et al., 2018; Jedd et al., 2015; Peters et al., 2018) also focused on an emotion-matching task proposed by Hariri and colleagues (2000) which uses the Gur emotional faces set (Gur et al., 2002). A series of studies was realized using the Dote-probe task from Mogg and Bradley (1999) which is a database of happy and sad faces (Brüne et al., 2013; Davis et al., 2014; Fani et al., 2011). In English et al. (2018), images were obtained from the Cohn–Kanade Facial Expression Database (Kanade et al., 2000), a sample of video sequences created by Pollak et al. (2009), where actors portray different emotions ranging from neutral to apex. Ten images per emotion were taken to reflect different levels of intensity. Hartling et al. (2019) used videos instead of pictures. They exposed participants to 24 short video clips of positive and negative emotions portrayed by 15 professional actors. Finally, for tasks with infant faces, Dayton et al. (2016) used the Infant Facial Expressions of Emotion from Looking at Pictures (IFEEL) task (Emde et al., 1993) in which the participant is exposed to infant faces, many depicting ambiguous emotional expressions. Olsavsky et al. (2019) and Kim et al. (2014) used a task composed of 10 infant faces depicting happy, neutral, and distressed faces. The task was first used in Strathearn et al. (2008). Neukel et al. (2019) created their own database of happy, sad, and neutral emotion pictures from videos of participants’ school-aged children.
Number of emotions
Some researchers exposed participants to Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) set of six basic emotions, namely, happiness, anger, disgust, fear, sadness, and surprise (Russo et al., 2015), whereas other studies focused on specific emotions. Many studies exposed participants to one positive and one negative emotion, such as happiness and anger (Kirkham & Levita., 2019; Neukel et al., 2019), happiness and fear/threat (Davis et al., 2014; Fani et al., 2011; Olsavsky et al., 2019), or happiness and sadness (Kim et al., 2014). In other studies, participants viewed anger and fear, two negative emotions (Dannlowski et al., 2012; Demers et al., 2018; Jedd et al., 2015; Redlich et al., 2015). Certain researchers presented these two negative emotions as well as happiness (Johnson et al., 2010; Olsavsky et al., 2019; Veague et al., 2014). Many studies included four emotions, although the emotions chosen varied across protocols. In Fang et al.’s (2019) and Gong et al.’s (2019) experiments, participants were exposed to faces of happiness, anger, fear, and disgust in addition to neutral faces. English et al. (2018), Peters et al. (2018), Schwaiger et al. (2019), Suzuki et al. (2015), and van Harmelen et al. (2013) also presented the four emotions in addition to neutral stimuli but incorporated sadness in their database instead of disgust.
Very few researchers exposed participants to more complex emotions. One exception is Hartling et al. (2019) who, using short videos of actors, exposed their participants to four basic and 20 complex emotions (e.g., interested, amused, aggrieved, troubled, jealous). Another study used the Read the Mind in the Eyes protocol developed by Baron-Cohen et al. (2001) where only the eyes are shown to participants who must choose among basic and more complex emotions (Schwaiger et al., 2019).
Transformation of stimuli
Most studies presented the emotions to participants without editing the images. However, a few studies used techniques to modify the images and to provide participants with a gradual display of emotions. The morphing technique allows to measure how the emotion is perceived when it is not presented at apex, that is, where a ceiling effect in performance occurs in many populations (Young et al., 1997). With morphing, two emotions of different percentages are combined, resulting in a single image. Some studies used a combination of one emotion with a neutral expression in order to create an array of intensity (Gibb et al., 2009; Russo et al., 2015), whereas other studies morphed two different emotions, with both emotions varying in intensity (Schwaiger et al., 2019; Suzuki et al., 2015; Veague & Hooley, 2014).
Task
Emotion recognition
Part of the studies used experimental designs where participants were asked to identify the emotion presented. Schwaiger et al. (2019) used a task that required participants to respond as quickly and as accurately as possible to identify the basic emotions displayed on adult faces, with each emotion being gradually increased in intensity. Others asked participants to identify the dominant emotion presented on morphed facial expressions, regardless of response time (English et al., 2018; Gibb et al., 2009; Russo et al., 2015; Schwaiger et al., 2019; Suzuki et al., 2015; Veague & Hooley, 2014). In their study, Kirkham and Levita (2019) asked participants to rate the intensity of happiness and anger on two separate scales ranging from 0 to 100%.
Attentional bias
The most common protocol used to evaluate attentional biases toward emotions is the dot-probe task. During this task, a picture of both an emotional and a neutral face are displayed on the right and left sides of a screen for a short period of time (e.g., 800 ms), followed by a dot appearing on either side. Participants must indicate the location of the dot. An attentional bias toward the emotional face will result in faster responses when the dot is located on the same side as the emotional face. This protocol has been used in numerous research studies examining attentional biases in participants with and without a history of maltreatment (Brüne et al., 2013; Davis et al., 2014; Fani et al., 2011; Gibb et al., 2009; Johnson et al., 2010). Recent studies have also combined this protocol with brain imagery (Fang et al., 2019; Gong et al., 2019).
Viewing of facial emotions during brain recording
Many studies have investigated brain activity during emotion processing. Among these studies, some used passive viewing in which participants were exposed to faces without being asked to perform any task (van Harmelen et al., 2013). In other protocols, brain recording occurred while participants performed a memory-matching task. Participants’ accuracy in matching faces was compared to their performance in matching simple geometric shapes (Dannlowski et al., 2012; Demers et al., 2018; Jedd et al., 2015; Peters et al., 2018; Redlich et al., 2015).
Effect of a History of Childhood Maltreatment on Emotion Recognition
Maltreatment and happiness
In their study on emotion recognition, Veague et al. (2014) concluded that a history of abuse predicted difficulties in recognizing happiness as opposed to fear and anger. Studies on attentional biases show contradictions. Fani et al. (2011) found that a history of childhood abuse was related to attentional biases toward happy faces compared to neutral faces. In Davis et al. (2014), childhood abuse was not related to an attention bias toward happiness nor threat. However, participants who had both a history of severe abuse and attachment anxiety had an attentional bias away from happy faces compared to neutral faces.
Maltreatment and anger
Many studies have demonstrated a relationship between a history of childhood maltreatment and the recognition of anger. Gibb et al. (2009) found that individuals with a history of childhood abuse could recognize anger when presented at a lower level of intensity. No difference was found for happiness and sadness. They also found that individuals with a history of childhood abuse display a greater attentional bias toward anger. In a study by Kirkham and Levita (2019), participants with a history of maltreatment and no mental illness reported they would be less likely to avoid a person with an angry face. Finally, in another study, while accuracy in recognizing angry and fear was similar for participants with and without a history of childhood maltreatment, it increased following oxytocin administration but only for the maltreated group (Schwaiger et al., 2019).
The relationship between childhood maltreatment and the recognition of anger seems to go in the opposite direction when participants with childhood maltreatment experiences have mental disorders. In a clinical sample of participants with bipolar personality disorder (BPD), Russo et al. (2015) found that participants who reported experiencing physical abuse, emotional neglect, or physical neglect were less accurate in recognizing anger than those without such childhood experiences. In a similar sample, Brüne et al. (2013) found that participants with BPD and a history of childhood maltreatment were more likely to show attentional avoidance to expressions of anger, devoting less attention to anger than to neutral stimuli. However, Johnson et al. (2010) revealed that the attentional avoidance to angry faces in women reporting childhood maltreatment was specific to carriers of a short allele in the 5-HTTLPR gene, rather than to major depression or anxiety disorder. In a similar study, Hartling et al. (2019) found that participants with a history of maltreatment and a more stress-responsive genetic profile were less accurate in recognizing emotions in general.
Maltreatment and fear
English et al. (2018) submitted participants to emotional faces while under low and high cognitive load in order to distinguish between strategic and automatic processing. They found that participants with a history of emotional maltreatment recognized fear more quickly when processing emotions under high cognitive load, which suggests an enhanced alertness to threat signals. On the other hand, Suzuki et al. (2015) found that adults with a history of maltreatment made significantly more errors in recognizing fear than anger. Looking at the interaction between childhood maltreatment and mental disorders, these authors also found that depressed adults with a history of maltreatment demonstrated an increased bias toward fear compared to adults with a history of maltreatment but no depression.
Maltreatment and brain activation
Childhood maltreatment is also related to specific brain activation when participants are exposed to facial expressions of emotion as demonstrated in EEG studies. Fang et al. (2019) found larger N170 amplitudes for participants with a history of childhood maltreatment when they performed a dot-probe task with angry, fearful, and happy faces. Although not significant, the maltreated group also showed better accuracy and shorter reaction times in recognizing the emotions. The authors conclude that adults with a history of childhood maltreatment show a hypervigilance to the measured emotions. Using a similar protocol to compare participants with high and low negative schizotypy, Gong et al. (2019) found no main group difference for participants with and without a history of maltreatment on the amplitudes of N170 and P100.
Other research used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine brain activation when participants were exposed to facial emotions. Demers et al. (2018) found that, among participants who experienced childhood maltreatment, facial emotions elicited greater amygdala-based connectivity within the dorsomedial- and right-dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, structures related to stress response, and differences in the hippocampus–amygdala connectivity, a circuit linked to the representation of emotional significance. An enhanced amygdala responsiveness in adults with a history of maltreatment has also been demonstrated in relation to anger and fear (Jedd et al., 2015; van Harmelen et al., 2013) and to threat (Dannlowski et al., 2012). Peters et al. (2018) found that participants with a combined history of maltreatment and internalizing psychopathology had increased corticolimbic reactivity to fearful facial expressions and greater activation of somatosensory areas during fear and anger processing. Finally, studies have shown that the interaction between maltreatment and amygdala reactivity to angry and fearful faces is specific to certain genotypes (Redlich et al., 2015).
Results With Exposure to Children’s Faces
In a study by Dayton et al. (2016), participants were exposed to pictures from the IFEEL database of 1-year-old infants depicting ambiguous facial expressions. Mothers who reported having experienced childhood maltreatment and intimate partner conflict tended to interpret ambiguous infant facial expressions more negatively than other mothers did. Olsavsky et al. (2019) found that mothers with a history of maltreatment showed a blunted bilateral amygdala reactivity to infant emotions, with no differential reactivity to positive, neutral, and negative emotions. This effect occurred only with infant faces, not adult faces. Moreover, for maltreated mothers, a more elevated amygdala response to children’s emotions was related to more sensitive behaviors during a mother–child interaction, whereas the nonmaltreated mothers showed a reversed relation.
Other studies have compared mothers’ reactions to their own child’s emotions with those of unknown children. In Neukel et al. (2019), mothers with and without a history of maltreatment reacted differently to their child’s expressions of happiness. Mothers with a history of maltreatment demonstrated elevated brain activation to their child’s happy faces, which was not the case for other mothers. The authors interpreted the reaction of mothers with a history of maltreatment as greater mentalization and less spontaneous empathic reactions to their child’s positive emotions. Similarly, Kim et al. (2014) found that mothers with history of maltreatment did not show differential responses to their own child’s happy versus sad faces, whereas mothers without a history of maltreatment showed greater amygdala responsiveness to their own child’s sad faces. Table 3 presents a summary of these findings.
Table 3.
Effects of Childhood Maltreatment on Emotional Processing in Adults.
Author (Year) | Emotions Affected | Type of Reaction | Population/Maltreatment Type | Compared to Other Emotions |
---|---|---|---|---|
Brüne et al. (2013) | Anger | Avoidance | Physical and emotional neglect and abuse | No avoidance to happy faces |
Dannlowski et al. (2012) | Threat (fear/anger) | Increased right amygdala responsiveness | ||
Davis et al. (2014) | Happiness | Avoidance of positive facial stimuli | History of abuse combined with attachment anxiety only | No attention bias to threat |
Dayton et al. (2016) | Anger Fear Distress |
Heightened perceptions of negative infant emotions (fear,
anger, and distress) Negative attribution bias (ambiguous infant expressions = negative) |
Histories of interpersonal aggression and conflict | |
Demers et al. (2018) | Anger Fear |
Increased hippocampus–amygdala connectivity | ||
English et al. (2018) | Fear | Quicker identification of fear under the high cognitive load condition | No significant predictive effects for anger | |
Fang et al. (2019) | Anger Fear Happiness |
Larger N170 amplitude in the maltreated group No differences in P100 amplitude |
No difference in N170 amplitude for disgust | |
Fani et al. (2011) | Happiness | Attentional bias | Sexual, physical, or emotional abuse With and without childhood maltreatment and/or current PTSD/depressive symptoms |
Relative to neutral faces |
Gibb et al. (2009) | Angry | Attention and interpretation biases (increased sensitivity in the detection of angry expressions at lower levels of emotional intensity) | History of moderate to severe childhood abuse | No biases for happy or sad faces |
Gong et al. (2019) | Anger Disgust Fear Happiness |
Longer latency of P100 (indicating a dysfunction of the visual pathway) | Young adults with high levels of negative schizotypy and childhood maltreatment | |
Hartling et al. (2019) | All trials (four basic + 20 complex emotions) | Lower ability to infer emotions | Individuals carrying a moderate to high Genetic Profile Score who have experienced Early life stress | |
Jedd et al. (2015) | Anger Fear |
Greater activation in prefrontal cortex and basal
ganglia Increased amygdala connectivity with the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex Significantly less accurate during the emotion-matching task |
Compared to the nonmaltreated group | |
Johnson et al. (2010) | Anger | Attentional avoidance | Individuals with a history of moderate to severe childhood physical abuse who also carried at least one copy of the 5-HTTLPR short allele. | Not happy or sad faces |
Kim et al. (2014) | Own infant’s sadness | Blunted amygdala responses | Mothers who were classified as having unresolved trauma in the Adult Attachment Interview | Compared to happiness No differences in regard to unknown faces |
Kirkham & Levita (2019) | Anger | Reduced avoidance (self-reported) | Only among participants with no evidence of mental illness and higher ELS scores | |
Neukel et al. (2019) | Own-child happiness | Elevated activations in face perception and emotion processing networks (e.g., cuneus, MTG, STG, parahippocampal gyri). | Compared to nonmaltreated mothers | Compared to neutral No differences for sad vs. neutral across groups |
Olsavsky et al. (2019) | Anger Fear Distress Happiness Neutral Negative-valenced stimuli |
Blunted bilateral amygdala reactivity to infant faces
(higher amygdala activation to infant faces was associated
with greater sensitivity during mother–infant
interaction) Superior temporal gyrus activation |
Compared to nonexposed mothers Regardless of CME |
Regardless of emotion |
Peters et al. (2018) | Fear Fear and Anger |
Enhanced corticolimbic reactivity Greater activation in visual processing and somatosensory areas |
Internalizing psychopathology + Early life adversity | Versus shapes Versus shapes No significant findings for sad versus shapes or happy versus shapes |
Redlich et al. (2015) | Anger Fear |
Increased amygdala reactivity | Higher neural response in C allele carriers compared to T
homozygotes. Emotional abuse and emotional neglect showed the strongest association with amygdala responsiveness |
|
Russo et al. (2015) | Anger | Poorer performance in facial emotion recognition | Bipolar disorder patients with a positive childhood history of emotional neglect | No significant differences for disgust, fear, sadness, surprise, or happiness |
Schwaiger et al. (2019) | Anger Fear |
Increase in emotion recognition accuracy | Non-significant for sad and happy faces | |
Suzuki et al. (2015) | Fear | More errors (misattributions) | Healthy individuals (no depression) with a history of childhood maltreatment | Compared to anger |
van Harmelen et al. (2013) | Anger Fear Sadness Happiness Neutral |
Enhanced bilateral amygdala reactivity | Independent of psychiatric status | Smaller effect sizes for neutral faces |
Veague & Hooley (2014) | Happiness | Difficulties detecting (when stimulus exposure times were short [500 ms]) | Abuse history was not associated with recognition of anger or fear and unrelated to anger bias |
Note. ELS = Early Life Stress, MTG = Middle Temporal Gyrus, STG = Superior Temporal Gyrus, CME = Child Maltreatment Exposure.
Discussion
A growing number of studies have examined how adults with a history of maltreatment react to emotions. The main conclusion is that the experience of childhood maltreatment does alter the perception of emotions in adults. Despite the wide variety of methodologies employed to measure child maltreatment, as well as the different protocols used to measure perceptual abilities related to emotion recognition, most of the studies reviewed found a difference between adults who were exposed to maltreatment during childhood and those who were not (see Table 4 for a brief resume of the study).
Table 4.
Summary of Critical Findings.
|
|
|
|
Table 5.
Implications of the Review for Practice, Policy, and Research.
Practice | Policy | Research |
---|---|---|
|
|
|
Anger and fear are the emotions that have been included in the largest number of studies. Maltreatment experiences are related to an increased recognition of these emotions (English et al., 2018, for fear; Gibb et al., 2009, for anger). These emotions also elicit greater brain activation in adults with a history of maltreatment, particularly in the amygdala region (Jedd et al., 2015; Redlich et al., 2015; van Harmelen et al., 2013). However, for both emotions, when childhood maltreatment is combined with a mental disorder, recognition seems to be decreased and participants show greater avoidance to the emotional stimuli (Brüne et al., 2013; Kirkham & Levita, 2019; Suzuki et al., 2015).
As for positive emotions, participants with a history of maltreatment seem to have more difficulty recognizing happiness (Veague et al., 2014). They also demonstrate more attentional-biases toward this emotion (Fani et al., 2011). However, when they also have a psychopathology, they show attentional biases away from happiness (Davis et al., 2014).
To date, psychopathology is one of the most studied potential mechanisms linking childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition in adulthood. For instance, Maoz et al. (2016) found that people with social anxiety disorder have faster reaction times when looking at anger and slower reactions times for happiness. Participants with social anxiety disorder also show a tendency to misinterpret neutral expressions of emotion as anger (Peschard & Philippot, 2017). Lower performance rates in detecting emotions have also been observed in people with psychopathy and antisocial traits (Iria & Barbosa, 2009; Pham & Philippot, 2010). Similarly, difficulties and negative misattribution errors in emotion recognition have been reported in people with borderline personality disorder (Nicol et al., 2014; Veague & Hooley, 2014).
When looking at the high proportion of studies connecting a history of childhood maltreatment, emotion recognition, and mental disorders, one might be tempted to conclude that the particularities in emotion recognition relates more to psychopathology than to maltreatment. However, considering that many children exposed to maltreatment are known to have impaired emotion recognition in childhood (Assed et al., 2020; Harms et al., 2019), it is likely that in this population, the deficit in emotion recognition contributes to psychopathology rather than the other way around. In any case, longitudinal studies are needed to better understand the trajectory of this co-occurrence.
Moreover, some studies included in the systematic review have established a link between maltreatment experiences and emotion recognition, but only for adults with a particular genetic profile (Hartling et al., 2019; Johnson et al., 2010; Redlich et al., 2015). A genetic contribution to emotion recognition was also found in research with other populations. A study with twins found that similarities in emotion recognition among twins could mostly be attributed to genetic factors (Rappaport et al., 2018). Therefore, epigenetics could explain multiple complex developmental pathways that predict the emotional recognition abilities of individuals exposed to maltreatment during childhood. For example, St-Laurent et al. (2019) found that the continuity of maltreatment across generations is predicted by the level of stressors in the family ecology, pleading in favor of a diathesis-stress model in which adverse experiences lead to further maltreatment only in certain stressful contexts.
A recent meta-analysis by Saarinen et al. (2021) examined the effects of early adversity, defined as important stressors encountered before the age of 18 years, on emotion recognition. The meta-analysis combined results from studies conducted with children and adults, with the average age of participants being of approximately 10 years for the behavioral studies, and 20 years for the fMRI studies. The results from the behavioral studies cannot be compared to the results of the current systematic review since most participants in the meta-analysis were children. The fMRI results from children and adult participants indicate higher brain activation, but only for sad faces, independent of mental disorders. This is different from the results of the current systematic review where anger and fear were found to elicit brain activation. This distinction could be explained by different factors. First, the response to emotions in children and adults may differ, and until we have a better understanding of the evolution of emotion recognition following a history of maltreatment, considering the two groups separately may offer different insights. Second, Sareeine et al.’s study looked at early adversity contrary to the current study that focuses specifically on child maltreatment. Researchers emphasize the difference between child maltreatment and other early adversities both conceptually (Mersky et al., 2017) and in terms of their consequences on child development (Atzl et al., 2019). Finally, of the 23 studies examined in the current systematic review, only 11 included sadness. More studies examining this emotion in maltreated adults may be necessary.
Most studies have been conducted using adult faces as stimuli. However, the few studies that have used infant faces have also shown a difference between adults with and without childhood maltreatment experiences. Dayton et al. (2016) reported a negative misattribution of ambiguous infant expressions in mothers reporting maltreatment experiences as well as intimate partner conflict. Cerebral imagery also shows differential brain activation in mothers with and without past experiences of maltreatment (Kim et al., 2014; Neukel et al., 2019). Results from these studies may contribute to explain the link between childhood trauma and lower parental sensitivity. While childhood maltreatment has been associated with parenting difficulties (see Savage et al., 2019, for a review), the underlying mechanisms that explain why there is continuity in the intergenerational transmission of maltreatment in some cases, and discontinuity in others, are still poorly understood. A recent study demonstrates that emotion recognition was differentially associated with maternal sensitivity in mothers with and without a history of childhood maltreatment (Bérubé et al., 2020). Mothers who had experienced severe maltreatment and who were better at recognizing emotions expressed on child faces demonstrated less sensitive behaviors when interacting with their own child. For mothers with traumatic childhood experiences, being exposed to emotional signals may trigger trauma-related reactions and lead to difficulties in parenting. Such processes may play a role in perpetuating the intergenerational cycle of maltreatment.
The results of this systematic review of the literature are difficult to interpret because of the variability in the measurement of maltreatment and the diversity of tasks used to measure emotion recognition. For instance, some studies have included a sample of maltreated children referred to child protection services, while others obtained retrospective reports of maltreatment from participants. A growing number of studies show that maltreatment based on self-reports and what is referred to as an objective measure, such as child protection services (CPS) reports, do not target the same population. In a study by Najman et al. (2020), children were followed over a 30-year period. Very low concordance was found with only 17% of adults reporting maltreatment having a CPS file. Of all children referred to CPS over time, 60% did not indicate a history of severe maltreatment on self-report measures. Similarly, in a meta-analysis, Baldwin et al. (2019) concluded that approximately half of adults with a prospective observation of child maltreatment (such as CPS reports) will retrospectively report child maltreatment, and half of adults who self-report child maltreatment will have been reported to CPS as children. Danese and Widom (2020) found a difference in the psychopathological consequences of maltreatment between the two groups. Self-reports of maltreatment were associated with a higher risk of mental health problems, whether they were substantiated by CPS data or not. Conversely, CPS data, if not combined with a self-report of maltreatment, have little predictive value.
Moreover, most of the studies we examined either combined the different forms of maltreatment in a general concept or examined some specific forms but not others. Studies with children have demonstrated that different forms of maltreatment are related to differences in the perception of emotions. A study by Turgeon et al. (2020) established that adults who reported having experienced childhood physical abuse were less able to recognize the emotional expressions of fear and sadness displayed on child faces. Furthermore, childhood experiences of emotional and sexual abuse were associated with difficulties in recognizing anger. Finally, physical neglect was related to difficulties in recognizing happiness, whereas emotional neglect was associated with higher accuracy scores in recognizing anger. These results suggest that different types of maltreatment may lead to different consequences in adults. More studies are needed to validate this knowledge.
Limitations and Future Directions
While this systematic review makes significant contributions to understanding the link between childhood maltreatment and emotional processing in adulthood, findings should be interpreted in light of the following limitations. First, quality assessment of studies was not documented using a systematic tool. However, only studies with a rigorous methodology and published in a peer-reviewed journal were considered in the synthesis, and interrater agreement was assessed at each step of the review.
Second, it would have been interesting to transform the current systematic review into a meta-analysis study. To perform the calculations required for a meta-analysis, authors usually set a threshold of a minimum of three articles on the same data type (Saarinen et al., 2021). The current review describes the results of studies with particular attention to the specific populations that were studied and the methodologies used. In fact, data regarding adults are separated from data regarding children. A history of childhood maltreatment is also acknowledged as a specific form of early childhood adversity and only data pertaining to this form of trauma were examined. Finally, the different tasks used to measure emotion recognition are considered separately. By doing so, many clusters did not meet the minimum criterion required to calculate scores in a meta-analysis. Thus, instead of presenting calculations for certain specific combinations of methodology/population/trauma, this article favors a descriptive approach that considers all parameters.
Third, results were not interpreted in light of sociodemographic data, such as gender, age, or ethnicity. As for ethnicity, two studies conducted with a similar population (i.e., predominantly African American adults, female, with a low socioeconomic status) exposed participants to the same emotion recognition task and found conflicting results. While Fani et al. (2011) found an attentional bias toward happiness, Davis et al. (2014) reported avoidance of positive facial stimuli (i.e., happiness). The differences in these studies may be explained by the ethnicity of the presented emotional stimuli. Davis et al. (2014) exposed participants to both African American and Caucasian models, whereas Fani et al. (2011) mainly exposed participants to Caucasian models. Results from a meta-analysis examining the effect of culture on emotion recognition reveal an in-group advantage, indicating that emotions are best recognized when they are expressed and perceived by the same ethnic group (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002). This factor was not controlled for in most of the studies we examined. Future research should consider this factor as its effect on emotion recognition is now better documented.
Finally, the samples included in the review do not fully represent diversity in terms of socioeconomic status, ethnicity, gender identity, age, and culture. This limitation must be considered when interpreting the results. Future studies examining the association between childhood maltreatment and emotion recognition should be conducted with diverse samples and all studies should include information about participants’ sociodemographic characteristics.
Conclusion
Results from this study indicate that childhood experiences of maltreatment influence adults’ ability to recognize and discriminate between emotions expressed on both adult and child faces. Additional studies are needed to understand the mechanisms underlying this relationship (e.g., type of maltreatment, age at onset, severity/chronicity). This knowledge is crucial to better support adults with a history of maltreatment and help these parents recognize their child’s signals and needs. This understanding could ultimately help reduce the intergenerational cycle of maltreatment.
Authors Biographies
Annie Bérubé is professor in the Department of Psychology and Psychoeducation at the Université du Québec en Outaouais. She is interested in the factors that contribute to the involvement of parents in the different spheres of their child’s life. Her research aims to understand the mechanisms that explain why some parents find it particularly difficult to meet their child’s needs. Her current studies focus on the contribution of the perception of emotions and stress generated by the parent–child interaction on the parent’s commitment to their child. She also focuses on the impact of activities offered in the community for the families. In this component of her research, program evaluation is used to better understand which program elements are most likely to support families. She leads the Ricochet team (https://uqo.ca/ricochet).
Jessica Turgeon is currently pursuing her doctoral studies in psychology at the Université du Québec à Trois-Rivières. Her master’s thesis focused on the recognition of emotions in mothers who have experienced child abuse. She is a psychoeducator and member of the Ordre des psychoéducateurs et psychoéducatrices du Québec.
Caroline Blais is professor in the Department of Psychology and Psychoeducation at the Université du Québec en Outaouais. Her work uses techniques derived from psychophysics (e.g., bubbles, reverse correlation) to better understand how sociocultural environment modulates fundamental visual processes, ranging from the extraction of low-level visual information (e.g., sensitivity to spatial frequencies) to the processing of complex visual information (e.g., face, objects, scenes). She holds the Canada Research Chair in in Cognitive and Social Vision.
Daniel Fiset is professor in the Department of Psychology and Psychoeducation at the Université du Québec en Outaouais. His work aims to understand how the brain and more specifically the occipito-temporal areas use visual information to effectively recognize faces, written words, and letters. To this end, he uses psychophysical techniques (e.g., bubbles, reverse correlation) as well as electrophysiology ( electroencephalography [EEG] and event-related potentials [ERP]) to investigate low-level visual properties (e.g., spatial frequencies and orientations) and how they modulate the recognition of stimuli. His studies are done thanks to the collaboration of healthy participants as well as people who have difficulty recognizing faces (prosopagnosia) and written words (alexia or dyslexia).
Footnotes
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Funding: The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (grant number 430-2016-00521). This work also received the support of the Centre de recherche universitaire pour les jeunes enfants et leur famille (grant number 7053088—CRUJeF).
ORCID iD: Annie Bérubé
https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6505-2085
References
- Ainsworth M. S. (1979). Infant–mother attachment. American Psychologist, 34(10), 932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Assed M. M., Khafif T. C., Belizario G. O., Fatorelli R., de Ameida Rocca C. C., de Pádua Serafim A. (2020). Facial emotion recognition in maltreated children: A systematic review. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 1–17. 10.1007/s10826-019-01636-w [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Atzl V. M., Narayan A. J., Rivera L. M., Lieberman A. F. (2019). Adverse childhood experiences and prenatal mental health: Type of ACEs and age of maltreatment onset. Journal of Family Psychology, 33(3), 304–314. 10.1037/fam0000510 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baldwin J. R., Reuben A., Newbury J. B., Danese A. (2019). Agreement between prospective and retrospective measures of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(6), 584–593. 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.0097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Hill J., Raste Y., Plumb I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251. 10.1017/S0021963001006643 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bartlett J. D., Kotake C., Fauth R., Easterbrooks M. A. (2017). Intergenerational transmission of child abuse and neglect: Do maltreatment type, perpetrator, and substantiation status matter? Child Abuse & Neglect, 63, 84–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein D. P., Stein J. A., Newcomb M. D., Walker E., Pogge D., Ahluvalia T., Stokes J., Handelsman L., Medrano M., Desmond D., Zule W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bérubé A., Blais C., Fournier A., Turgeon J., Forget H., Coutu S., Dubeau D. (2020). Childhood maltreatment moderates the relationship between emotion recognition and maternal sensitive behaviors. Child Abuse & Neglect, 102, 104432. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bifulco A., Brown G. W., Harris T. O. (1994). Childhood experience of care and abuse (CECA): A retrospective interview measure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(8), 1419–1435. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb01284.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brüne M., Ebert A., Kolb M., Tas C., Edel M. A., Roser P. (2013). Oxytocin influences avoidant reactions to social threat in adults with borderline personality disorder. Human Psychopharmacoly, 28(6), 552–561. 10.1002/hup.2343 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buisman R. S., Pittner K., Tollenaar M. S., Lindenberg J., van den Berg L. J., Compier-de Block L. H., van Ginkel J. R., Alink L. R., van IJzendoorn M. H. (2020). Intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment using a multi-informant multi-generation family design. PLoS One, 15(3), e0225839. 10.1371/journal.pone.0225839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen S., Jin Q. (2015, October). Multi-modal dimensional emotion recognition using recurrent neural networks. In Proceedings of the 5th International Workshop on Audio/Visual Emotion Challenge (pp. 49–56).
- Danese A., Widom C. S. (2020). Objective and subjective experiences of child maltreatment and their relationships with psychopathology. Nature Human Behaviour, 1–8. 10.1038/s41562-020-0880-3 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Dannlowski U., Stuhrmann A., Beutelmann V., Zwanzger P., Lenzen T., Grotegerd D., Domschke K., Hohoff C., Ohrmann P., Bauer J., Lindner C., Postert C., Konrad C., Arolt V., Heindel W., Suslow T., Kugel H. (2012). Limbic scars: Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment revealed by functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 71(4), 286–293. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.10.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- da Silva Ferreira G. C., Crippa J. A., de Lima Osório F. (2014). Facial emotion processing and recognition among maltreated children: A systematic literature review. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1460. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01460 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davis J. S., Fani N., Ressler K., Jovanovic T., Tone E. B., Bradley B. (2014). Attachment anxiety moderates the relationship between childhood maltreatment and attention bias for emotion in adults. Psychiatry Research, 217(1–2), 79–85. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.03.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayton C. J., Huth-Bocks A. C., Busuito A. (2016). The influence of interpersonal aggression on maternal perceptions of infant emotions: Associations with early parenting quality. Emotion, 16(4), 436–448. 10.1037/emo0000114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Graaf R., Bijl R. V., Smit F., Vollebergh W. A., Spijker J. (2002). Risk factors for 12-month comorbidity of mood, anxiety, and substance use disorders: Findings from the Netherlands mental health survey and incidence study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(4), 620–629. 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.620 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demers L. A., McKenzie K. J., Hunt R. H., Cicchetti D., Cowell R. A., Rogosch F. A., Toth S., Thomas K. M. (2018). Separable effects of childhood maltreatment and adult adaptive functioning on amygdala connectivity during emotion processing. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(2), 116–124. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.08.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ebner N. C., Riediger M., Lindenberger U. (2010). FACES—A database of facial expressions in young, middle-aged, and older women and men: Development and validation. Behavior Research Methods, 42(1), 351–362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P., Friesen W. V. (1976). Pictures of facial affect [Slides]. Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University. [Google Scholar]
- Ekman P., Friesen W. V. (1978). Manual of the facial action coding system (FACS). Trans. ed. Vol. Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto.
- Elfenbein H. A., Ambady N. (2002). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203. 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Emde R. N., Osofsky J. D., Butterfield P. M. (1993). The IFEEL pictures: A new instrument for interpreting emotions. International Universities Press. [Google Scholar]
- English L. H., Wisener M., Bailey H. N. (2018). Childhood emotional maltreatment, anxiety, attachment, and mindfulness: Associations with facial emotion recognition. Child Abuse & Neglect, 80, 146–160. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2018.02.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fang J., Wang S., Liu J., Gong J. (2019). Early ERP components to emotional facial expressions in young adult victims of childhood maltreatment. Psychiatry Research, 275, 120–128. 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.03.024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fani N., Bradley-Davino B., Ressler K. J., McClure-Tone E. B. (2011). Attention bias in adult survivors of childhood maltreatment with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35(1), 57–67. 10.1007/s10608-010-9294-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- George C., Kaplan N., Main M. (1996). Adult attachment interview: 3rd Edition. [Unpublished manuscript]. Department of Psychology, University of California. [Google Scholar]
- Gibb B. E., Schofield C. A., Coles M. E. (2009). Reported history of childhood abuse and young adults’ information-processing biases for facial displays of emotion. Child Maltreatment, 14(2), 148–156. 10.1177/1077559508326358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gong J., Liu J., Shangguan L., Zhang Q., Peng Z., Li Z., Chen C., Shi L. (2019). Childhood maltreatment impacts the early stage of facial emotion processing in young adults with negative schizotypy. Neuropsychologia, 134, 107215. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greene C. A., Haisley L., Wallace C., Ford J. D. (2020). Intergenerational effects of childhood maltreatment: A systematic review of the parenting practices of adult survivors of childhood abuse, neglect, and violence. Clinical Psychology Review, 80, 101891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gur R. E., McGrath C., Chan R. M., Schroeder L., Turner T., Turetsky B. I., Kohler C., Alsop D., Maldjian J., Ragland J. D., Gur R. C. (2002). An fMRI study of facial emotion processing in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(12), 1992–1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hariri A. R., Bookheimer S. Y., Mazziotta J. C. (2000). Modulating emotional responses: Effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system. Neuroreport, 11(1), 43–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harms M. B., Leitzke B. T., Pollak S. D. (2019). Maltreatment and emotional development. In Handbook of emotional development (pp. 767–786). Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Hart H., Rubia K. (2012). Neuroimaging of child abuse: A critical review. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 52. 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hartling C., Fan Y., Weigand A., Trilla I., Gartner M., Bajbouj M., Dziobek I., Grimm S. (2019). Interaction of HPA axis genetics and early life stress shapes emotion recognition in healthy adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 28–37. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hein T. C., Monk C. S. (2017). Research review: Neural response to threat in children, adolescents, and adults after child maltreatment—A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(3), 222–230. 10.1111/jcpp.12651 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iria C., Barbosa F. (2009). Perception of facial expressions of fear: Comparative research with criminal and non-criminal psychopaths. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry & Psychology, 20(1), 66–73. 10.1080/14789940802214218 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffee S. R. (2017). Child maltreatment and risk for psychopathology in childhood and adulthood. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13(1), 525–551. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-045005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jedd K., Hunt R. H., Cicchetti D., Hunt E., Cowell R. A., Rogosch F. A., Toth S. L., Thomas K. M. (2015). Long-term consequences of childhood maltreatment: Altered amygdala functional connectivity. Development and Psychopathology, 27(4 Pt 2), 1577–1589. 10.1017/s0954579415000954 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson A. L., Gibb B. E., McGeary J. (2010). Reports of childhood physical abuse, 5-HTTLPR genotype, and women’s attentional biases for angry faces. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 34(4), 380–387. 10.1007/s10608-009-9269-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kanade T., Cohn J. F., Tian Y. (2000, March). Comprehensive database for facial expression analysis. In Proceedings Fourth IEEE International Conference on Automatic Face and Gesture Recognition (Cat. No. PR00580) (pp. 46–53). IEEE. 10.1109/AFGR.2002.1004141 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kim S., Fonagy P., Allen J., Strathearn L. (2014). Mothers’ unresolved trauma blunts amygdala response to infant distress. Social Neuroscience, 9(4), 352–363. 10.1080/17470919.2014.896287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirkham E. J., Levita L. (2019). Early life stress is associated with reduced avoidance of threatening facial expressions. Development and Psychopathology, 1–9. 10.1017/S095457941900110X [DOI] [PubMed]
- Kliemann D., Rosenblau G., Bölte S., Heekeren H. R., Dziobek I. (2013). Face puzzle—two new video-based tasks for measuring explicit and implicit aspects of facial emotion recognition. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 376. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lundqvist D., Flykt A., Ohman A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces (KDEF). Karolinska Institute. [Google Scholar]
- Madigan S., Cyr C., Eirich R., Fearon R. P., Ly A., Rash C., Poole J., Alink L. R. (2019). Testing the cycle of maltreatment hypothesis: Meta-analytic evidence of the intergenerational transmission of child maltreatment. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 23–51. 10.1017/S0954579418001700 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maoz K., Eldar S., Stoddard J., Pine D. S., Leibenluft E., Bar-Haim Y. (2016). Angry-happy interpretations of ambiguous faces in social anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Research, 241, 122–127. 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Masten C. L., Guyer A. E., Hodgdon H. B., McClure E. B., Charney D. S., Ernst M., Kaufman J., Pine D. S., Monk C. S. (2008). Recognition of facial emotions among maltreated children with high rates of post-traumatic stress disorder. Child Abuse & Neglect, 32(1), 139–153. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matsumoto D., Ekman P. (1988). Japanese and Caucasian facial expressions of emotion (JACFEE) Intercultural and emotion research laboratory. Department of Psychology, San Francisco State University. [Google Scholar]
- Mersky J. P., Janczewski C. E., Topitzes J. (2017). Rethinking the measurement of adversity: Moving toward second-generation research on adverse childhood experiences. Child Maltreatment, 22(1), 58–68. 10.1177/1077559516679513 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogg K., Bradley B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(6), 595–604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher D., Shamseer L., Clarke M., Ghersi D., Liberati A., Petticrew M., Shekelle P., Stewart L. A. & PRISMA-P Group. (2015). Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Systematic Reviews, 4(1), 1. 10.1186/2046-4053-4-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Najman J. M., Kisely S., Scott J. G., Strathearn L., Clavarino A., Williams G. M., Middeldorp C., Bernstein D. (2020). Agency notification and retrospective self-reports of childhood maltreatment in a 30-year cohort: Estimating population prevalence from different data sources. Child Abuse & Neglect, 109. 10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104744 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neukel C., Herpertz S. C., Hinid-Attar C., Zietlow A. L., Fuchs A., Moehler E., Bermpohl F., Bertsch K. (2019). Neural processing of the own child’s facial emotions in mothers with a history of early life maltreatment. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 269(2), 171–181. 10.1007/s00406-018-0929-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicol K., Pope M., Hall J. (2014). Facial emotion recognition in borderline personality: An association, with childhood experience. Psychiatry Research, 218(1–2), 256–258. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.04.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olsavsky A. K., Stoddard J., Erhart A., Tribble R., Kim P. (2019). Neural processing of infant and adult face emotion and maternal exposure to childhood maltreatment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14(9), 997–1008. 10.1093/scan/nsz069 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peschard V., Philippot P. (2017). Overestimation of threat from neutral faces and voices in social anxiety. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 57, 206–211. 10.1016/j.jbtep.2017.06.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peters A. T., Burkhouse K. L., Kinney K. L., Luan Phan K. (2018). The roles of early-life adversity and rumination in neural response to emotional faces amongst anxious and depressed adults. Psychological Medicine, 49(13), 2267–2278. 10.1017/S0033291718003203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pham T. H., Philippot P. (2010). Decoding of facial expression of emotion in criminal psychopaths. Journal of Personality Disorders, 24(4), 445–459. 10.1521/pedi.2010.24.4.445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak S. D. (2008). Mechanisms linking early experience and the emergence of emotions: Illustrations from the study of maltreated children. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(6), 370–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak S. D., Cicchetti D., Hornung K., Reed A. (2000). Recognizing emotion in faces: developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology, 36(5), 679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak S. D., Messner M., Kistler D. J., Cohn J. F. (2009). Development of perceptual expertise in emotion recognition. Cognition, 110(2), 242–247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pollak S. D., Sinha P. (2002). Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology, 38(5), 784. 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rappaport L. M., Carney D. M., Verhulst B., Neale M. C., Blair J., Brotman M. A., Pine D. S., Leibenluft E., Hettema J. M., Roberson-Nay R. (2018). A developmental twin study of emotion recognition and its negative affective clinical correlates. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(12), 925–933. 10.1016/j.jaac.2018.05.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Redlich R., Stacey D., Opel N., Grotegerd D., Dohm K., Kugel H., Heindel W., Arolt V., Baune B. T., Dannlowski U. (2015). Evidence of an IFN-gamma by early life stress interaction in the regulation of amygdala reactivity to emotional stimuli. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 166–173. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robbins T. W., James M., Owen A. M., Sahakian B. J., McInnes L., Rabbitt P. (1994). Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTAB): A factor analytic study of a large sample of normal elderly volunteers. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 5(5), 266–281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russo M., Mahon K., Shanahan M., Solon C., Ramjas E., Turpin J., Burdick K. E. (2015). The association between childhood trauma and facial emotion recognition in adults with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research, 229(3), 771–776. 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saarinen A., Keltikangas-Järvinen L., Jääskeläinen E., Huhtaniska S., Pudas J., Tovar-Perdomo S., Penttilä M., Miettunen J., Lieslehto J. (2021). Early adversity and emotion processing from faces: A meta-analysis on behavioral and neurophysiological responses. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Sanders B., Becker-Lausen E. (1995). The measurement of psychological maltreatment: Early data on the child abuse and trauma scale. Child Abuse & Neglect, 19(3), 315–323. 10.1016/S0145-2134(94)00131-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savage L. É., Tarabulsy G. M., Pearson J., Collin-Vézina D., Gagné L. M. (2019). Maternal history of childhood maltreatment and later parenting behavior: A meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 9–21. 10.1017/S0954579418001542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schwaiger M., Heinrichs M., Kumsta R. (2019). Oxytocin administration and emotion recognition abilities in adults with a history of childhood adversity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 99, 66–71. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.08.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shipman K., Schneider R., Sims C. (2005). Emotion socialization in maltreating and nonmaltreating mother-child dyads: Implications for children’s adjustment. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34(3), 590–596. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- St-Laurent D., Dubois-Comtois K., Milot T., Cantinotti M. (2019). Intergenerational continuity/discontinuity of child maltreatment among low-income mother–child dyads: The roles of childhood maltreatment characteristics, maternal psychological functioning, and family ecology. Development and Psychopathology, 31(1), 189–202. 10.1017/S095457941800161X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strathearn L., Kim S. (2013). Mothers' amygdala response to positive or negative infant affect is modulated by personal relevance. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 7, 176. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strathearn L., Li J., Fonagy P., Montague P. R. (2008). What’s in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics, 122(1), 40–51. 10.1542/peds.2007-1566 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suzuki A., Poon L., Kumari V., Cleare A. J. (2015). Fear biases in emotional face processing following childhood trauma as a marker of resilience and vulnerability to depression. Child Maltreatment, 20(4), 240–250. 10.1177/1077559515600781 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Teicher M. H., Samson J. A. (2016). Annual research review: Enduring neurobiological effects of childhood abuse and neglect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(3), 241–266. 10.1111/jcpp.12507 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N., Hare T. A., Casey B. J. (2011). Behavioral assessment of emotion discrimination, emotion regulation, and cognitive control in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turgeon J., Bérubé A., Blais C., Lemieux A., Fournier A. (2020). Recognition of children’s emotional facial expressions among mothers reporting a history of childhood maltreatment. PLoS One, 15(12), e0243083. 10.1371/journal.pone.0243083 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Harmelen A. L., van Tol M. J., Demenescu L. R., van der Wee N. J., Veltman D. J., Aleman A., van Buchem M. A., Spinhoven P., Penninx B. W., Elzinga B. M. (2013). Enhanced amygdala reactivity to emotional faces in adults reporting childhood emotional maltreatment. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(4), 362–369. 10.1093/scan/nss007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Ijzendoorn M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg M. J., Coughlan B., Reijman S. (2020). Annual research review: Umbrella synthesis of meta-analyses on child maltreatment antecedents and interventions: Differential susceptibility perspective on risk and resilience. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 61(3), 272–290. 10.1111/jcpp.13147 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veague H. B., Hooley J. M. (2014). Enhanced sensitivity and response bias for male anger in women with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Research (Print), 215(3), 687–693. 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.12.045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young A. W., Rowland D., Calder A. J., Etcoff N. L., Seth A., Perrett D. I. (1997). Facial expression megamix: Tests of dimensional and category accounts of emotion recognition. Cognition, 63(3), 271e313. 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00003-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]