Abstract
The reward hypersensitivity model posits that trait reward hypersensitivity should elicit hyper/hypo approach motivation following exposure to recent life events that activate (goal-striving and goal-attainment) or deactivate (goal-failure) the reward system, respectively. To test these hypotheses, eighty-seven young adults with high (HRew) versus moderate (MRew) trait reward sensitivity reported frequency of life events via the Life Event Interview. Brain activation was assessed during the fMRI Monetary Incentive Delay task. Greater exposure to goal-striving events was associated with higher nucleus accumbens (NAc) reward anticipation among HRew participants and lower orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) reward anticipation among MRew participants. Greater exposure to goal-failure events was associated with higher NAc and OFC reward anticipation only among HRew participants. This study demonstrated different neural reward anticipation (but not outcome) following reward-relevant events for HRew versus MRew individuals. Trait reward sensitivity and reward-relevant life events may jointly modulate reward-related brain function, with implications for understanding psychopathology.
Keywords: reward life events, reward anticipation, fMRI, orbitofrontal cortex, nucleus accumbens
Introduction
The brain’s reward system subserves approach motivation and pursuit of rewards and goals (e.g., Haber & Knutson, 2010). The reward hypersensitivity theory (Alloy & Abramson, 2010; Alloy & Nusslock, 2019; Alloy, Olino, Freed & Nusslock, 2016; Depue & Iacono, 1989; Johnson, Edge, Holmes, & Carver, 2012; Nusslock & Alloy, 2017), often applied to bipolar spectrum disorders (BSDs), hypothesizes that individual differences in trait sensitivity to rewards will combine with individuals’ exposure to life events that tend to activate or deactivate the reward system to influence state levels of reward responsivity. However, to our knowledge, as yet, no work has precisely tested these hypotheses. To examine the predictions of the model, the present fMRI study examined activation in reward-related neural regions, the nucleus accumbens (NAc) and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), as a function of recent reward-related life event exposure among individuals with self-reported high trait reward sensitivity (HRew) versus moderate trait reward sensitivity (MRew). The findings from this work may contribute greater understanding of variation in neural reward responses as a function of exposure to naturally occurring reward-relevant life events. In addition, it may have implications for advancing the understanding of psychiatric disorders characterized by aberrant approach motivation and how life events can perturb reward processing implicated in these disorders.
The Reward System
Reward processing is linked to a cortico-striatal neural circuit that promotes approach motivation, goal-directed behavior, emotion, and decision-making (e.g., Haber & Knutson, 2010). Within this neural circuit, the ventral striatum is involved in coding the incentive properties of stimuli and reward prediction errors. As a core region of the ventral striatum, the NAc is particularly sensitive to reward cues, and thus, to anticipation of potentially rewarding outcomes (Haber & Knutson, 2010). The OFC has been implicated in reward-related decision-making and computation of reward value, which may be involved in approach toward rewards or goals. The reward system can be activated or deactivated by external events (e.g., opportunity to win a prize; Haber & Knutson, 2010). Accordingly, elevated NAc activation has been construed as a proxy for hyper-responsivity, whereas blunted NAc activation has been established as a proxy for hypo-responsivity. OFC activation is a more complicated proxy for reward system functioning. The OFC projects input to subcortical regions, including the NAc (Brady & O’Donnell, 2004; Jackson, Frost, & Moghaddam, 2001) based on the perceived value and probability of reward receipt (Phillips, Ladouceur, & Drevets, 2008; Haber & Behrens, 2014). However, subcortical regions also can guide the cortex, highlighting that the OFC may exert regulatory control of the NAc, but subcortical regions like the NAc also can modulate the OFC (Haber & Knutson, 2010; Tottenham & Gabard-Durnam, 2017). Given the bidirectional nature of connections between the OFC and NAc, OFC activation to reward cues may either serve as evidence of responsiveness to reward or regulation of reward, and thus, may reflect heightened or attenuated reward responsivity, depending on the nature of its functional coupling with subcortical reward regions (Haber & Knutson, 2010).
Reward Hypersensitivity and Psychopathology
A hypersensitivity to rewarding stimuli has been associated with multiple forms of psychopathology, including risk for substance use disorders (Alloy et al., 2009; Nusslock & Alloy, 2017), certain externalizing behaviors (Murray, Waller, & Hyde, 2018), and BSD. For example, according to the reward hypersensitivity model of BSDs (Figure 1; Alloy & Nusslock, 2019; Alloy et al., 2016; Depue & Iacono, 1989; Johnson et al., 2012; Nusslock & Alloy, 2017), people with a hypersensitive reward system excessively respond to goal- or reward-relevant cues. This reward hypersensitivity is hypothesized to lead to extreme approach-related affect (e.g., excitement, elation, or anger) and incentive motivation following exposure to specific life events. Such events include those that activate the reward system (i.e., goal-striving, goal-attainment, and failures or losses that can be rectified [leading to anger, in this case]), and, in turn, hypo/manic symptoms. Reward hypersensitivity also can lead to excessive decreases in approach-related affect and motivation in response to events that deactivate the reward system (i.e., irreconcilable failures or losses that cannot be remediated), and, in turn, bipolar depressive symptoms. In other words, a trait propensity toward extreme activation and deactivation of the reward system resulting in abnormalities in approach motivation is the theorized vulnerability to BSDs in this model (Alloy & Nusslock, 2019). Considerable self-report, behavioral, neurophysiological, and neural evidence supports the reward hypersensitivity model of BSDs (see Alloy & Nusslock, 2019; Alloy et al., 2016; Johnson et al., 2012, Nusslock & Alloy, 2017 for reviews), but some neural studies obtain contrary results, including either no difference in reward-related brain activation or hypoactivation for individuals with BSD (Johnson, Mehta, Ketter, Gotlib, & Knutson, 2019; Schreiter et al., 2016; Trost et al., 2014; Yip, Worhunsky, Rogers, & Goodwin., 2015). However, none of these studies examined neural activation following exposure to reward-relevant events, a key element in the reward hypersensitivity model of BSDs.
Figure 1.
The Reward Hypersensitivity Model. This figure illustrates the Reward Hypersensitivity Model.
The Role of Reward-Relevant Life Events
A central component that separates the reward hypersensitivity model from traditional diathesis-stress theories (Monroe & Simons, 1991) is the role of specific life events. Diathesis-stress theories propose that psychopathology emerges from the interaction of a vulnerability (diathesis) and broadly defined environmental stress (Monroe & Simons, 1991). Although the reward hypersensitivity model does involve the concepts of diathesis (trait reward hypersensitivity) and environmental stress (reward-relevant life events), the model offers specificity with an emphasis on the differences among reward-relevant events in terms of triggering reward system activation and deactivation, rather than the “stress” posed by any kind of life events. In other words, not all life events interact with trait reward hypersensitivity in the same manner. Accordingly, individuals with a hypersensitive reward system should manifest more extreme (i.e., hyper or hypo) responses to reward cues following exposure to recent life events that activate or deactivate the reward system compared to individuals with more moderate reward sensitivity. Events involving goal-striving (opportunity to work toward a goal or reward, e.g., a possible promotion) and goal-attainment (actual receipt of a reward, e.g. a raise), hypothesized to activate the reward system, have been associated with increases in hypo/manic symptoms. Events involving irreconcilable goal failures or losses (failures or losses that cannot be remediated, e.g., being fired) have been associated with bipolar depressive symptoms in HRew individuals (Boland et al., 2016). However, there is a paucity of empirical research specifically testing the hypothesized role of such reward-relevant life events in predicting reward system responses per se.
To our knowledge, only three studies, two with healthy samples and one with individuals with BSD, have examined the effects of an acute lab stressor on neural reward processing. Kumar et al. (2014) found that an acute lab stressor (negative performance feedback) led to increased striatal activation during reward anticipation, but decreased striatal activation during reward outcome relative to a no stress condition. Conversely, Ossewaarde et al. (2011) observed that an acute lab stressor (aversive movie clips) led to decreased medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) activation to reward anticipation, but had no effect on striatal activation. Berghorst et al. (2016) examined the threat of monetary deductions and poor performance feedback on neural reward processing in bipolar individuals. Compared to healthy controls, bipolar participants in a euthymic or mildly depressed state exhibited elevated amygdala activation during reward anticipation and elevated striatal putamen activation during reward outcome in the stress condition compared to the no stress condition. Note that these acute lab stressors likely were not perceived as irreconcilable failures.
Likewise, studies have examined the effect of recent perceived life stress on neural reward processing. Among healthy participants, Treadway, Buckholtz, and Zald (2013) found that greater perceived stress within the past month was associated with lower mPFC activation to reward outcome, but not reward anticipation. In a sample of patients with major depression and healthy controls, Kumar et al. (2015) reported that participants with greater perceived life stress exhibited higher mPFC activation to reward outcome during an acute lab stress condition (negative performance feedback) than a no stress condition, and this effect was primarily attributable to the depressed participants.
Although these findings suggest that acute negative lab stressors and recent perceived life stress influence neural responses to reward anticipation or outcome, none of these studies examined the effects of the actual frequency of recent life event exposure or of reward system-activating and deactivating events specifically on neural reward responsiveness. Moreover, no prior studies have examined neural reward responsiveness following reward-relevant event exposure in HRew individuals whose reward processing should be most susceptible to such life event exposure. In other words, it is plausible that neural reward responsiveness may be activated or deactivated following exposure to reward-relevant life events particularly strongly among HRew individuals.
The Current Study
The present study examined whether greater recent exposure to different types of reward-relevant life events is associated with specific patterns of approach motivation indexed by neural reward activation among reward-hypersensitive individuals compared to moderate reward-sensitive individuals. Although this study did not directly test hypotheses related to mood-related psychopathology (e.g., BSD and substance use), it has implications for understanding the pathophysiology of these conditions. According to the reward hypersensitivity theory, individuals with a trait hypersensitive reward system should exhibit elevated responses to cues signaling both the possible success and failure to obtain reward (Figure 1). We predicted that among HRew individuals, heightened exposure to reward system-activating events involving goal-striving or goal-attainment would be associated with heightened activation in the OFC and NAc (Hypothesis 1), as indicators of reward system activation. Further, we predicted that among the HRew individuals, heightened exposure to irreconcilable goal failure, involving triggers of approach cessation and deactivation of the reward system, would be associated with lower activation in the NAc (Hypothesis 2). We were less certain about our predictions for the OFC following heightened exposure to irreconcilable goal failure, but suggested that reward hypersensitive individuals may display elevated OFC activation, given that they may engage the OFC in a manner that attenuates NAc signaling following failure and loss (i.e., cortico-striatal attenuation tendencies; Ng, Alloy, & Smith, 2019; Young et al., 2016; Hypothesis 2). Because the reward hypersensitivity model highlights the role of approach motivation, a construct that is related to reward anticipation, but not outcome (Alloy & Nusslock, 2019; Chase et al., 2013; Nusslock et al., 2012), our hypotheses focused on neural activation during reward anticipation. If we obtained significant effects for reward anticipation, we also explored the effects of reward-relevant life events on activation during reward outcome to assess specificity to reward anticipation. Finally, our objective also was to determine whether any observed distinct neural function occurs specifically during reward, but not loss, processing. Thus, we examined the relationship between trait reward sensitivity, recent exposure to reward-relevant events, and neural activation to rewards above and beyond the effect of loss-related neural activation by adjusting for OFC and NAc activation during loss anticipation or outcome.
Method
Participant Recruitment
Participants for the current study came from a larger longitudinal study, the Teen Emotion and Motivation project (Project TEAM), described in detail elsewhere (Alloy et al., 2012). Project TEAM recruited MRew and HRew individuals based on scores falling in the 40th-60th percentile (MRew) or top 15th percentile (HRew) on both of two self-report trait reward sensitivity measures, the Behavioral Activation System Scale (cutpoint ≥ 43 for HRew and ≥ 37 and ≤ 39 for MRew) and the Sensitivity to Reward (cutpoint ≥ 16 for HRew and ≥ 10.4 and ≤ 12.6 for MRew) subscale of the Sensitivity to Punishment Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire. Project TEAM participants completed follow-up assessments that assessed reward-relevant life events approximately every six months. All TEAM participants still involved in the study when the fMRI scan was introduced were invited to participate in the MRI session except those who were excluded based on the following criteria: a lifetime history of psychosis, ferrous metal in any part of the body, lifetime history of head trauma, claustrophobia, left-handedness, and pregnancy. A subset of participants (all right-handed) consented and completed an additional fMRI component approximately 26 months (SD = 28 months) after the start of their participation in Project TEAM. Those who participated in the fMRI scan were screened for eligibility (standard MRI exclusion criteria were applied), provided informed consent, and completed a set of trait and state self-report measures on the day of the fMRI scan. Participants completed the Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) reward task (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2007) in the scanner. All study protocols were approved by the Institutional Review Board at Temple University before the start of data collection.
Current Study Participants
Of the 133 participants who completed the fMRI scan, 26 were excluded due to excessive head motion (>3mm), four were excluded due to behavioral task acquisition errors, and 16 were excluded because they were missing life event data in the 6-month time period before the scan. Thus, participants in the current study included 59 HRew and 28 MRew individuals (N = 87), with an average age of 21.05 years (SD = 2.08, range = 18-28 years) on the day of the scan. Participants were 52% female, 54% White, 25% Black, 9% Asian, 6% Bi/Multiracial, 1% Native American, and 5% Other or Unknown race. MRew and HRew participants differed on mood disorder history, χ2(1) = 20.704, p < .001. However, they did not differ on age at scan, t(84) = .868, p = .388, gender, χ2(1) = .386, p = .534, race, χ2(5) = 4.962, p = .421, or whether they were taking psychotropic medication at time of scan, χ2(2) = .134, p = .714. Furthermore, participants included in the study did not differ from those who were excluded on mood disorder history, χ2(1) = 1.244, p = .265, age at scan, t(131) = −.834, p = .406, gender, χ2(1) = .109, p = .741, race, χ2(5) = 2.777, p = .734, whether they were taking psychotropic medication at the time of scan, χ2(2) = 2.233, p = .327, or reward group χ2(1) = .177, p = .674. Demographic information and data characteristics by trait reward sensitivity group are in Table 1.
Table 1.
Summary of sample characteristics by Trait Reward Sensitivity Group (N=87)
Variable | HRew (n=59) | MRew (n=28) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
M (SD) | % (n) | M (SD) | % (n) | |
Gender | ||||
Female | 49.2 (29) | 57.1 (16) | ||
Age (years) | 20.9 (2.1) | 21.3 (2.1) | ||
Race | ||||
White | 52.5 (31) | 60.7 (17) | ||
Black | 22.0 (13) | 32.1 (9) | ||
Asian | 11.9 (7) | 3.6 (1) | ||
Bi-/Multi-racial | 6.8 (4) | 3.6 (1) | ||
Native American | 1.7 (1) | 0.0 (0) | ||
Other or Unknown | 5.1 (3) | 0.0 (0) | ||
Psychotropic Medication Status | ||||
Not taken within past month | 94.9 (56) | 92.9 (26) | ||
Currently taking | 5.1 (3) | 7.1 (2) | ||
Lifetime Mood Disorder History | ||||
Bipolar Disorder | 42.4 (25) | 0.0 (0) | ||
Major Depressive Disorder | 11.9 (7) | 3.6 (1) | ||
Reward-Relevant Life Events | ||||
Goal-Striving | 5.0 (4.1) | 6.3 (5.6) | ||
Goal-Attainment | 5.4 (4.6) | 6.0 (5.6) | ||
Goal-Failure | 7.9 (7.6) | 9.9 (9.1) | ||
BAS total | 46 (3.1) | 38 (1.0) | ||
SR total | 17.8 (3.0) | 11 (1.6) |
Note. M, SD and % and n are used to represent mean, standard deviation, percentage, and frequency, respectively. Three HRew individuals reported current use of a prescribed antidepressant, stimulant, and anxiolytic. Two MRew individuals reported current use of a prescribed antidepressant. Goal-Striving, Goal-Attainment, and Goal-Failure events represent the three types of reward-relevant life events that participants experienced in the past six months. Abbreviations: HRew, High Trait Reward Sensitivity; MRew, Moderate Trait Reward Sensitivity; BAS, Behavioral Activation System scale; SR, Sensitivity to Reward subscale.
Measures
Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scales
The Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System Scale (BIS/BAS; Carver & White, 1994) is a widely used self-report questionnaire consisting of 20 items that assess trait-level indicators of the behavioral activation (reward sensitivity) and inhibition (punishment sensitivity) systems. Total BAS subscale scores were used to screen participants and determine their eligibility for the MRew and HRew groups in Project TEAM (Alloy et al., 2012). The BAS is dimensional (Liu, Burke, Abramson, & Alloy, 2018) and forms a general factor of reward sensitivity (Kelley et al., 2019). Participants were asked to rate items on a scale from one to four (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = strongly agree), with a possible total BAS score ranging from 13 to 52 The total BAS scale consists of items related to reward sensitivity constructs such as drive (“I go out of the way to get things I want”), fun seeking (“I often act on the spur of the moment”), and reward responsiveness (“When I’m doing well at something I love to keep at it”). In the baseline TEAM sample, it had acceptable internal consistency (alpha = .80) and has been shown to have acceptable retest reliability (Meyer, Johnson, & Winters, 2001).
Sensitivity to Punishment Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire
Participants also were screened based on the Sensitivity to Reward (SR) subscale of the Sensitivity to Punishment Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ; Torrubia, Ávila, Moltó, & Caseras, 2001). The SR subscale consists of 24 “yes/no” items assessing reward sensitivity (i.e., “Do you often do things to be praised?”, “Do you like to compete and do everything you can to win?”). A sum score was formed based on the number of “yes” item responses, with a possible score ranging from 0 to 24. In our baseline sample, the SR subscale had acceptable internal consistency (alpha = .76), which is supported by the extant literature (alphas = .75-.83; Torrubia et al., 2001). At baseline, the BAS-Total and SR scales correlated r = .40 with each other (Alloy et al., 2012).
Life Events Scale/Life Events Interview
The Life Events Scale (LES; Francis-Raniere, Alloy, & Abramson, 2006) is a self-report questionnaire that assesses the presence of a broad range of 193 positive and negative life events that typically occur among adolescents and young adults, including major and minor events related to school, achievement, career, health, finances, family, friends, and romantic relationships. The Life Events Interview (LEI; Francis-Raniere et al., 2006) was completed by trained interviewers to confirm whether the events endorsed by the participant on the LES met the a priori event-definition criteria. Interviewers were trained via review of audiotaped interviews, live observation, and role playing and were tested on their knowledge of the LEI manual via written exam and mock interview. Only events meeting event-definition criteria that occurred within the six months prior to the fMRI scan were included in the present study. LES events had been rated a priori by three Project TEAM investigators independent of actual interviews with participants as (1) goal-striving events (defined as opportunity to obtain goals/rewards or remove obstacles to goals/rewards; e.g., “you applied for a new job”), (2) goal-attainment events (defined as actually obtaining goals/rewards; e.g., “you won a significant award for your achievements at work”), or (3) goal-failure events involving definite failures to achieve goals or losses (defined as failure to obtain goals/rewards or loss of goals/rewards; e.g., “you received an F in an important course;” see Urosević et al., 2010 for more details), The interrater reliability of the event categorizations was α = .79 for goal-striving, .91 for goal-attainment, and .94 for goal-failure events (Urosević et al., 2010). All qualifying events were summed within each category; thus, scores represent the counts of the number of events within each category that occurred in the six months prior to the fMRI scan. The LES and LEI have demonstrated good reliability and validity in past research (Alloy et al., 2006; Boland et al., 2016; Francis-Raniere et al., 2006).
fMRI Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) Task
During the fMRI scan, participants completed the MID task (Samanez-Larkin et al., 2007; Figure 2), a widely used and well validated measure of neural reward function (Knutson, Westdorp, Kaiser, & Hommer, 2000). In a pre-scan training session, participants were instructed on how to complete the MID. They were told that they would have the opportunity to win money during reward trials (indicated by a circle cue signaling Win $0.00, Win $1.50, or Win $5.00) or avoid losing money during loss trials (indicated by a square cue signaling Lose $0.00, Lose $1.50, or Lose $5.00). The trial cue was presented for 2 seconds (s), followed by a jittered fixation and a solid white square. Participants had to press a button while the white square was on the screen to either win (reward trials) or avoid losing (loss trials) money. Next, participants received feedback (2s) indicating the amount of money won or lost for each trial, followed by another jittered fixation as an intertrial interval. Thus, the MID task allowed us to examine separately neural activity to reward/loss anticipation (the period between presentation of the initial trial cue and presentation of the target square, 2-2.5s), and reward/loss outcome (the feedback presentation period, 2s). Initial target duration was calculated based on the participant’s reaction time during the pre-scan trials, and then dynamically updated to keep hit rate at approximately 66% (this was calculated separately for each trial type). Participants completed the six trial types eight times in random order, for a total of 96 trials across two MID runs.
Figure 2.
Monetary Incentive Delay (MID) task. These figures illustrate the (A) trial structure and (B) possible reward and loss cues of the MID task designed to examine neural activation during anticipation and outcome of monetary reward and loss (adapted from: Young & Nusslock, Positive mood enhances reward-related neural activity, Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2016, 11(6), 934-44, by permission of Oxford University Press).
fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis
Neuroimaging data were collected using a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Verio wide-bore MRI scanner with a standard 12-channel head coil at Temple University Medical Center. Functional BOLD scans were collected using the following parameters: coverage = 36 axial slices, 4mm thick (FOV = 236 mm), matrix = 64x64, voxel size = 3.7 x 3.7 x 4.0 mm, TR = 2000 ms, TE = 25 ms, flip angle = 70 degrees, acquisition volumes = 292. Structural 3D MPRAGE scans also were collected in the sagittal plane with the following parameters: voxel size = 0.5 x 0.5 x 1.0 mm, TR = 1600 ms, TE = 2.46 ms, FOV = 252, flip angle = 9 degrees, acquisition volumes = 176.
Data were analyzed using a general linear model in SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, London, UK). Functional images were realigned and corrected for errors in slice-timing. Images then were spatially normalized to MNI space and smoothed using a 6 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. We computed translational movement in millimeters (x, y, z) and rotational motion in degrees (pitch, roll, yaw) based on SPM8 parameters to correct motion for the functional images in each participant. The final sample had less than 3 mm of head movement.
The hemodynamic signal was deconvolved using a general linear model identifying the six trial types during the MID anticipation and outcome phase. The anticipation phase was defined as the period after presentation of the cue indicating the possibility to win or lose money but prior to presentation of the target square (2-2.5s). The outcome phase was defined as the period after presentation of the feedback (2s). Six variables of no interest for motion were included. First-level voxel-wise t-statistics were computed for each participant contrasting reward (i.e., Win $1.50, Win $5.00) vs. non-reward (i.e., Win $0.00) trials to calculate reward anticipation and outcome, and loss (i.e., Lose $1.50, Lose $5.00) vs. non-loss (i.e., Lose $0.00) trials to calculate loss anticipation and outcome. We combined $1.50 and $5.00 trials to be consistent with previous research (e.g., Samanez-Larkin et al., 2007), for ease of interpretation, and to heighten reliability (i.e., more trials in the combined score).
Significant clusters of activation at the whole-brain level were determined at a voxel-wise height threshold of p< 0.001 uncorrected, with family-wise error (FWE) correction at the cluster-level for multiple spatial comparisons (p< 0.05, k= 20 voxels). All reported coordinates are in the standard space of Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI).
We extracted parameter estimates (beta-weights) from predefined regions-of-interest (ROIs) for the NAc and OFC during reward and loss anticipation and outcome, and exported these parameter estimates into R for analyses. For the bilateral NAc (Figure 3A) and bilateral OFC (Figure 4A), we used an anatomically defined bilateral ROI mask using the Harvard Oxford Atlas. The masks met a probabilistic threshold of at least 25%. The use of this OFC ROI mask provides the advantage of maximizing the balance between Type I and Type II error. On one hand, applying multiple OFC ROIs to detect activation in smaller regions would increase risk of Type I error. On the other hand, using a ROI mask covering the entire OFC would require a large effect to detect significant associations, and thus, increase risk for Type II error. Although excluding portions of the supramedial OFC, the Harvard Oxford Atlas mask does cover a relatively large portion of the OFC and at the same time lowers risk for false negative findings. Furthermore, prior literature on the link between reward-related brain function and psychiatric conditions characterized by reward abnormalities (e.g., depression, mania, substance use) often report effects in more lateral portions of the OFC (Forbes, Rodriguez, Musselman, & Narendran, 2014; Nusslock et al., 2012). This suggests that the OFC areas covered by the Harvard Oxford Atlas mask are particularly relevant to this area of research.
Figure 3.
Exposure to Reward-Relevant Life Events and Bilateral Nucleus Accumbens (NAc) Activation by Trait Reward Status. These figures illustrate (A) ROI for the bilateral NAc structurally derived with Harvard Oxford Atlas, (B) total goal-striving events (centered) as a function of activation in the NAc during reward anticipation for individuals with moderate versus high trait reward status, and (C) total goal-failure events (centered) as a function of activation in the NAc during reward anticipation for individuals with moderate versus high trait reward status; * = p < .05
Figure 4.
Exposure to Reward-Relevant Life Events and Bilateral Orbitofrontal Cortex (OFC) Activation by Trait Reward Status. These figures illustrate (A) Region-of-interest for the bilateral OFC as defined by the Harvard Oxford Atlas, (B) total goal-striving events (centered) as a function of activation in the OFC during reward anticipation for individuals with moderate versus high trait reward status, and (C) total goal-failure events (centered) as a function of activation in the OFC during reward anticipation for individuals with moderate versus high trait reward status; * = p < .05; ** = p < 0.01
Statistical Analysis Approach
All analyses were conducted in R. First, we conducted preliminary analyses using bivariate correlations (if the potential covariates were continuous variables) and independent-samples t-tests (if the potential covariates were categorical variables) to determine whether demographics or clinical characteristics should be included as covariates in the primary analyses. The variables were included as covariates if they were associated with the dependent variables (i.e., reward-related neural activation). The main effects of reward and loss during the anticipation and outcome phases also were examined to ensure that the MID task activated expected regions. Next, we employed moderation analyses to examine whether trait reward sensitivity group (MRew or HRew) moderated the relationship between the frequency of occurrence of goal-striving, goal-attainment, and goal-failure life events in the six months prior to the fMRI scan and neural activation during reward anticipation and outcome in the NAc and OFC. Thus, we ran three separate sets of primary analyses for each of the two ROIs (activation to reward anticipation in the NAc and OFC), with goal-striving, goal-attainment, or goal-failure events as the independent variable and reward sensitivity group as the moderator. To protect against Type I error inflation due to multiple comparisons, we applied the Fisher’s protected t-test which required a significant omnibus interaction result in order to proceed to simple slope analyses (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2013). If there were significant group by events interactions for neural reward anticipation, follow-up exploratory analyses were conducted for activation in the corresponding region during reward outcome to confirm specificity to reward anticipation. The first step in the regression models included any covariates found to be related to reward-related neural activation in the NAc or OFC during anticipation or outcome. In the next step, we included the mean-centered total number of goal-striving, goal-attainment, or goal-failure events and the trait reward sensitivity group, followed by the product term of mean-centered life events and trait reward sensitivity group. To determine whether the results hold above and beyond mood disorder history, we repeated the aforementioned primary analyses with the addition of mood disorder history as a covariate (see Tables S1 & S2 in Supplementary Material, available online).
Results
Preliminary Analyses
Preliminary analyses were conducted to examine whether there were associations between potential covariates (age at the time of the scan, gender, a history of mood disorder diagnosis, use of psychotropic medication at the time of scan, NAc activation during loss anticipation and outcome, and OFC activation during loss anticipation and outcome) and the dependent variables (NAc activation during reward anticipation and outcome, as well as OFC activation during reward anticipation and outcome; Miller & Chapman, 2001).
Participant age at the time of the scan was not significantly associated with NAc activation during reward anticipation (r = −.004, p = .972), OFC activation during reward anticipation (r = .040, p = .715), NAc activation during reward outcome (r = .090, p = .413), or OFC activation during reward outcome (r = .119, p = .280). Gender was not significantly associated with NAc reward anticipation (t[82] = −1.074, p = .286), OFC reward anticipation (t[82] = −.391, p = .697), NAc reward outcome (t[82] = .050, p = .960), or OFC reward outcome (t[82] = .416, p = .679). Having a history of a mood disorder diagnosis was not significantly associated with NAc reward anticipation (t[85] = .014, p = .988), OFC reward anticipation (t[85] = −1.660, p = .101), NAc reward outcome (t[85] = −1.138, p = .258), or OFC reward outcome (t[85] = −1.159, p = .250). Use of psychotropic medication at the time of the scan was not significantly associated with NAc reward anticipation (t[85] = −.028, p = .978), OFC reward anticipation (t[85] = −1.040, p = .301), NAc reward outcome (t[85]= −.955, p = .342), or OFC reward outcome (t[85] = −1.783, p = .078). Thus, none of these variables were included as covariates in the primary analyses because they did not relate to the NAc and OFC activation dependent variables.
Significant associations were found between OFC activation during anticipation of reward and during anticipation of loss (r = .282, p = .004), as well as between OFC activation during reward and loss related outcome (r = .349, p < .001). Similarly, NAc activation during reward anticipation was associated with NAc activation during loss anticipation (r = .346, p < .001). There also was a significant association between NAc activation during reward outcome and NAc activation during loss outcome (r = .645, p < .001). Thus, these loss variables were retained as the only covariates in the primary analyses.
Finally, our sample displayed whole-brain clusters of neural activity in expected regions during the MID (see Supplementary Material, available online).
Primary Analyses
Main Effects for Trait Reward Sensitivity and Frequency of Recent Reward-Relevant Events
We did not detect any main effects for trait reward sensitivity group on NAc (B = .022, SE = .277, t = .080, p = .937, ΔR2 < .001) or OFC (B = .206, SE = .151, t = 1.365, p = .175, ΔR2 = .017) activation to reward anticipation. There were no main effects for frequency of recent goal-striving (NAc: B = .021, SE = .031, t = .658, p = .513, ΔR2 = .004; OFC: B = −.009, SE = .017, t = −.500, p = .618, ΔR2 = .003), goal-attainment (NAc: B = .033, SE = .029, t = 1.107, p = .271, ΔR2 = .013; OFC: B = .005, SE = .016, t = .290, p = .772, ΔR2 = .001), or goal-failure (NAc: B = .017, SE = .018, t = .970, p = .335, ΔR2 = .010; OFC: B = .015, SE = .010, t = 1.530, p = .130, ΔR2 = .026) life events on ROI activation to reward anticipation. Next, we report results on the interaction between the frequency of each type of reward-relevant events and trait reward sensitivity group on ROI activation.
Frequency of Recent Reward-Relevant Events X Trait Reward Sensitivity Effects
Goal-striving life events.
As predicted in Hypothesis 1, the interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and recent goal-striving events was associated with NAc activation to reward anticipation (B = .158, SE = .061, t = 2.581, p = .012, ΔR2 = .067; Figure 3B). Specifically, greater exposure to goal-striving events was associated with heightened activation in the NAc among HRew individuals (B = .092, SE = .042, t = 2.207, p = .03, ΔR2 = .078), but not MRew individuals (B = −.066, SE = .045, t = −1.468, p = .146, ΔR2 = .066).
The interaction of trait reward sensitivity and goal-striving events also was associated with OFC activation during reward anticipation (B = .088, SE = .033, t = 2.645, p = .010, ΔR2 = .074; Figure 4B), such that greater exposure to goal-striving events was associated with decreased activation in the OFC among MRew individuals (B = −.053, SE = .025, t = −2.155, p = .034, ΔR2 = .208). Contrary to Hypothesis 1, however, the association between these events and OFC activation did not reach statistical significance among HRew individuals (B = .035, SE = .023, t = 1.556, p = .123, ΔR2 = .037).
Given the significant interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-striving events for NAc and OFC activation during reward anticipation, we conducted follow-up analyses for neural activation during reward outcome to examine specificity of the observed effects. We did not detect an interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-striving events on NAc activation during reward outcome (B = .058, SE = .055, t = 1.063, p = .291, ΔR2 = .008), or OFC activation during reward outcome (B = .007, SE = .035, t = .204, p = .839, ΔR2 < .001).
Goal-attainment life events.
Partially consistent with Hypothesis 1, the interaction of trait reward sensitivity group and recent goal-attainment events was associated with OFC activation during reward anticipation (B = .070, SE = .032, t = 2.147, p = .035, ΔR2 = .050). However, the simple slope analysis revealed that exposure to goal-attainment events was not associated with OFC activation among HRew individuals (B = .036, SE = .021, t = 1.696, p = .094, ΔR2 = .048) or MRew individuals (B = −.034, SE = .025, t = −1.378, p = .172, ΔR2 = .081). The interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-attainment events on NAc activation during reward anticipation did not reach statistical significance (B = .090, SE = .061, t = 1.494, p = .139, ΔR2 = .024).
Goal-failure life events.
The interaction of trait reward sensitivity group and recent goal-failure events was associated with NAc activation during reward anticipation (B = .086, SE = .035, t = 2.443, p = .017, ΔR2 = .061; Figure 3C). However, counter to predictions in Hypothesis 2, greater exposure to goal-failure events predicted heightened NAc activation among HRew individuals (B = .051, SE = .022, t = 2.272, p = .026, ΔR2 = .083). The association between goal-failure events and NAc activation was not significant among MRew individuals (B = −.036, SE = .028, t = −1.296, p = .199, ΔR2 = .052).
The interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-failure events was associated with OFC activation during reward anticipation (B = .055, SE = .019, t = 2.908, p = .005, ΔR2 = .085; Figure 4C). The conditional effects revealed that greater exposure to goal failures and losses was associated with greater OFC activation among HRew individuals (B = .039, SE = .012, t = 3.251, p = .002, ΔR2 = .152), but not MRew individuals (B = −.016, SE = .015, t = −1.106, p = .272, ΔR2 = .046).
Given the significant interaction results between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-failure events for NAc and OFC activation during reward anticipation, we conducted follow-up analyses for neural activation during reward outcome as well. There was no significant interaction between trait reward sensitivity group and goal-failure events on NAc activation (B = −.020, SE = .032, t = −.643, p = .522, ΔR2 = .003) or OFC activation (B = −.018, SE = .020, t = −.887, p = .378, ΔR2 = .008) during reward outcome.
Supplemental Analyses
The results of primary analyses were largely unaffected by the inclusion of mood disorder history as an additional covariate. Summary of these statistical models compared to primary analyses is shown in Tables S1 and S2 in the Supplemental Material available online.
Discussion
According to the reward hypersensitivity model, specific reward-relevant life events are posited to play a critical role in excessive activation or deactivation of a hypersensitive reward system. The results of the present analyses offer partial support for the hypothesized relationships of recent reward-relevant life events with neural activation during reward anticipation among high versus moderate reward sensitive individuals. Specifically, the findings indicate that greater exposure to recent reward-relevant life events was associated with distinct reward-related neural function among reward hypersensitive individuals. Importantly, recent reward system-activating life events (goal-striving, but not goal-attainment) and reward system-deactivating (goal-failure) life events each were associated with elevated neural activation patterns in reward hypersensitive individuals. Furthermore, such associations appeared specific to reward anticipation, but not reward outcome.
Partially supporting Hypothesis 1, both NAc and OFC activation during reward anticipation were associated with the interaction between exposure to recent goal-striving life events and trait reward hypersensitivity. In line with prior studies finding that elevated NAc reward anticipation is a neural correlate of psychopathology (Harada et al., 2013; Nusslock et al., 2012) and the role of goal-striving events in precipitating clinical symptoms (Nusslock et al., 2007), we found that among HRew individuals, NAc reward anticipation was higher for those exposed to a greater number of recent goal-striving life events. However, contrary to our expectations, there was no difference in OFC reward anticipation across levels of exposure to recent goal-striving life events for the HRew group. Instead, greater exposure to recent goal-striving life events was associated with lower OFC reward anticipation for the MRew group. This finding may be explained by the “coasting” phenomenon (Fulford, Johnson, Llabre, & Carver, 2010), in which most individuals tend to “coast” (i.e., not work as hard) after an extensive level of goal-striving, whereas individuals who are hypersensitive to goal-striving events may not coast, but continue to push through. In other words, whereas MRew individuals displayed lower OFC anticipation following a high level of exposure to goal-striving events, there was no difference for HRew individuals regardless of frequency of exposure to goal-striving events. Although we detected that exposure to goal-striving events was differentially associated with NAc and OFC activation between the trait reward groups, there were no comparable results for exposure to goal-attainment events. This may be attributable to power issues given that the current sample size was sufficiently powered (.80) to detect a small-moderate effect (i.e., r2≈.12), which is higher than the observed effects. Future work should replicate these findings in a larger sample.
In examining Hypothesis 2, we found that greater exposure to recent goal-failure life events was associated with heightened OFC reward anticipation for HRew individuals, but not MRew individuals. Surprisingly, among the HRew individuals, greater exposure to recent goal-failure events also was associated with greater NAc reward anticipation. One potential interpretation is that this reflected inefficiency in down-regulation of reward responsiveness among HRew individuals exposed to recent goal-failure events. For instance, there might have been a greater demand for OFC activation to provide optimal regulatory control of the NAc, which had been hyperactivated during reward anticipation. The OFC projection of inhibitory input to subcortical regions might have been inefficient, and thus, resulted in the observation of hyper-activation in NAc during reward anticipation. Another potential interpretation is that HRew individuals up-regulated, rather than down-regulated, reward responsiveness in the face of loss and failures, reflecting resilience and persistence. In other words, heightened OFC and NAc activation might have reflected hyper-responsiveness to reward. This may be due, in part, to the perceived nature of the goal-failure life events that were assessed in this study. Specifically, most of these life events might not necessarily reflect definitive failures or losses that could not be overcome. And, it is also possible that HRew individuals perceive these failures or losses as remediable challenges that can be overcome, such that exposure to these events activated rather than deactivated the reward system among HRew individuals, and thereby, were associated with a greater level of OFC and NAc reward anticipation following frequent exposure to these events. Future research should test the relationship between reward sensitivity, reward-related brain function and irreconcilable versus remediable reward-related failures/losses. Self-report questions assessing whether a reported goal-failure life event has been construed as a definitive failure versus remediable challenge also may help clarify the nature of goal-failure events an individual experienced.
Finally, as predicted, the current findings demonstrate that exposure to reward-relevant life events specifically was associated with neural activation during reward anticipation rather than outcome. This finding is in line with the reward hypersensitivity model, which suggests that HRew individuals should be particularly sensitive to excessive increases in approach motivation to cues that signal the chance to obtain rewards, as opposed to the receipt of those rewards (Alloy et al., 2016; Alloy & Abramson, 2010; Alloy & Nusslock, 2019).
The present study and prior studies that examined the relationship between stressors and reward-related brain function yielded inconsistent results. Such discrepancies may have resulted from the heterogeneity of samples and study measurements across these studies (Berghorst et al., 2016; Kumar et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2015; Ossewaarde et al., 2011; Treadway et al., 2013). To our knowledge, the present study was the first to specifically investigate life events based on their reward- or goal-pursuit status when examining the link between exposure to life events and reward-related neural function. This approach allows us to identify the precise aspect of life event exposure (i.e., the reward-related events’ ability to trigger reward system activation and deactivation) that might have played a role in reward-related brain function, but that has not otherwise been examined in prior literature (Berghorst et al., 2016; Kumar et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2015; Ossewaarde et al., 2011; Treadway et al., 2013). Further, compared to lab-induced stressors that were administered in related literature (Berghorst et al., 2016; Kumar et al., 2014; Ossewaarde et al., 2011), the assessment of naturally occurring life events in the current study allowed for greater ecological validity and understanding of naturalistic responses to the events.
Critically, the present study also was the first to include individuals varying in trait reward sensitivity when examining the associations between reward-relevant life event exposure and reward-related neural function. The prior studies included only healthy participants (Kumar et al., 2014; Ossewaarde et al., 2011; Treadway et al., 2013) or individuals with mood disorders (Berghorst et al., 2016). Although Kumar et al. (2015) did examine both individuals with and without depression, neither that study nor the aforementioned ones examined trait reward sensitivity, a stable characteristic that might generate distinct reward-related neural function following stress exposure. In fact, our study demonstrated that the relationship between reward-relevant life event exposure and reward-related brain function was unique among HRew compared to MRew individuals. This finding supports our claim that it is critical to also consider this stable personality factor. Our data suggest that trait reward sensitivity or event exposure alone was not sufficient to explain the variance of activation in the OFC and NAc during reward anticipation, given a lack of significant main effects. In sum, the interaction between trait reward sensitivity and reward-related event exposure offers a more comprehensive picture of neural reward processing.
Our findings have implications for understanding psychiatric conditions characterized by abnormal reward function. Abnormal trait reward sensitivity, exposure to reward-relevant life events, and distinct reward brain function each separately have been associated with psychiatric conditions, including bipolar disorder, major depression, and substance use disorder (see Nusslock & Alloy [2017] for review). However, the prior literature yielded heterogenous findings. This might be attributable to examination of these constructs in isolation. In line with this claim, the current study provides preliminary evidence that these constructs may modulate each other rather than contributing to psychopathology in isolated fashion. Thus, a research model that involves all three constructs may yield better understanding of the pathophysiology of psychiatric disorders. For example, assessment of trait reward sensitivity, exposure to reward-relevant life events, and reward-related neural function may lead to more precise estimation of risk level for a psychiatric condition, given the current findings suggesting that trait reward sensitivity or exposure to life events alone may not be a reliable predictor. This is speculative, at best, because the current study did not actually examine whether the observed distinct neural function was associated with psychiatric symptoms. However, in line with this speculation, prior work has demonstrated that altered function in the OFC and NAc may confer neural risk for mood psychopathology (Chase et al., 2013; Nusslock et al., 2012; Satterthwaite et al., 2015; Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, 2015; Zald & Treadway, 2017). Future research is necessary to examine the combination of trait reward sensitivity and exposure to reward-relevant life events in neural reward function in individuals with psychiatric disorders characterized by elevated reward function, such as BSD and substance use disorder, or blunted reward function, such as major depression, as well as individuals without psychiatric disorders.
The findings of the current study were bolstered by several strengths. First, the utilization of contextual life events interviews with objective event definition criteria offered an opportunity to capture objective reward cues present in natural settings while minimizing self-report biases. Second, the study design assessed the reward-relevant life events temporally prior to the fMRI scan. Third, the criteria utilized to classify individuals into the HRew and MRew groups were based on data from a large community sample. This approach allowed us to maximize the difference in reward sensitivity between the HRew and MRew groups and established the two groups using absolute levels of moderate versus high reward sensitivity.
However, this study also had some limitations. First, the present findings cannot yield causal claims given the absence of a baseline fMRI scan visit. Despite the fact that the events assessed occurred prior to the fMRI scan, the temporal precedence of these effects did not allow for ruling out the possibility that reward hypersensitivity generates more exposure to reward-relevant life events. Future work should adopt a more complete longitudinal design that offers the opportunity to examine the relationship between reward-relevant life events and reward-related neural activation over time with repeated scans and repeated events assessments. Second, recall bias might occur given the retrospective nature of the life events interview. Although other observational approaches, such as ecological momentary assessment, can reduce this bias, the life event interview has an advantage of minimizing the interruption of participants’ daily life and time burden to participate in the study. Further, the present study reduced memory bias by only including reported events that had an onset or offset date within 12 months prior to the interview. Third, the current results may be limited because Project TEAM excluded a low trait reward sensitivity group to address the project’s primary aims. For instance, it is unclear whether individuals with low trait reward sensitivity yield distinct or share similar reward-related brain function compared to the HRew and MRew groups following exposure to reward-relevant life events. Furthermore, due to the unequal sample size across the trait reward sensitivity groups, there was less information to estimate the effects of reward-relevant life event exposure on neural activation for the smaller (i.e., MRew) than the larger group (i.e., HRew), and the unbalanced sample size could reduce statistical power (Aguinis, 1995). As such, future work should examine this question on a full spectrum of trait reward sensitivity with equal sample size across the subgroups. Approximately a third of our sample reported a lifetime mood disorder history, which may help to explain the heightened motion observed in our sample and why a large number of participants did not meet inclusion criteria for MRI analyses. However, our findings were comparable with or without a lifetime mood disorder history as a covariate, offering support that the observed results were not a product of mood disorder history. Additionally, although the current study reported gender, race, and ethnicity, other demographic data, such as education level and socioeconomic status were not collected, and thus, limited the generalizability of the reported findings. Finally, this study focused on the presence, but not intensity, of reward-relevant life events. However, it is possible that intensity of events also is relevant, given that related research demonstrated associations between stressor severity and reward-related neural activation (Kumar et al., 2015; Treadway et al., 2013). Future work should compare the effects of frequency vs. intensity of reward-relevant event exposure.
Conclusions
Notwithstanding the limitations, the current study is the first to examine reward-related neural function following exposure to naturally occurring reward-relevant life events among individuals with high versus moderate levels of reward sensitivity. Partially consistent with the reward hypersensitivity model, our study found that trait levels of reward sensitivity moderated the relationship between exposure to reward-relevant life events and reward-related brain function. The present work advances the understanding of reward-related brain function by highlighting that exposure to reward-relevant life events and trait reward sensitivity jointly may moderate OFC and NAc activation specifically during reward anticipation, but not outcome. Although trait reward hypersensitivity, stress exposure, and reward-related neural function each have been linked to psychopathology in isolation, the results of the present study support the need to examine reward-related psychiatric conditions using a comprehensive model that incorporates the interaction among these constructs.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments & Disclosures
Preparation of this work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grants (NIMH R01MH077908 and NIMH R01MH102310) to Lauren B. Alloy, and the National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship Program (Grant No. 1650457) to Erin E. Dunning. The authors report that they have no conflicts of interest.
References
- Aguinis H (1995). Statistical power problems with moderated multiple regression in management research. Journal of management, 21(6), 1141–1158. [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, & Abramson LY (2010). The role of the behavioral approach system (BAS) in bipolar spectrum disorders. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(3), 189–194. 10.1177/0963721410370292 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Walshaw PD, Cogswell A, Smith JM, Neeren AM, … Nusslock R (2006). Behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity and bipolar spectrum disorders: A retrospective and concurrent behavioral high-risk design. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 143–155. 10.1007/s11031-006-9003-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Bender RE, Wagner CA, Whitehouse WG, Abramson LY, Hogan ME, … Harmon-Jones E (2009). Bipolar spectrum–substance use co-occurrence: Behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity and impulsiveness as shared personality vulnerabilities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(3), 549–565. 10.1037/a0016061 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Bender RE, Whitehouse WG, Wagner CA, Liu RT, Grant DA, … Abramson LY (2012). High behavioral approach system (BAS) sensitivity, reward responsiveness, and goal-striving predict first onset of bipolar spectrum disorders: A prospective behavioral high-risk design. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(2), 339–351. 10.1037/a0025877 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, & Nusslock R (2019). Future directions for understanding adolescent bipolar spectrum disorders: A reward hypersensitivity perspective. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 48(4), 669–683. 10.1080/15374416.2019.1567347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloy LB, Olino T, Freed RD, & Nusslock R (2016). Role of reward sensitivity and processing in major depressive and bipolar spectrum disorders. Behavior Therapy, 47(5), 600–621. 10.1016/j.beth.2016.02.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berghorst LH, Kumar P, Greve DN, Deckersbach T, Ongur D, Dutra SJ, & Pizzagalli DA (2016). Stress and reward processing in bipolar disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Bipolar Disorders, 18(7), 602–611. 10.1111/bdi.12444 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boland EM, Stange JP, LaBelle DR, Shapero BG, Weiss RB, Abramson LY, & Alloy LB (2015). Affective Disruption From Social Rhythm and Behavioral Approach System (BAS) Sensitivities. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(3), 418–432. 10.1177/2167702615603368 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brady AM, & O’Donnell P (2004). Dopaminergic Modulation of Prefrontal Cortical Input to Nucleus Accumbens Neurons In Vivo. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(5), 1040–1049. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4178-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carver CS, & White TL (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319–333. 10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman LJ, & Chapman JP (1987). The measurement of handedness. Brain and Cognition, 6(2), 175–183. 10.1016/0278-2626(87)90118-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chase HW, Nusslock R, Almeida JR, Forbes EE, LaBarbara EJ, & Phillips ML (2013). Dissociable patterns of abnormal frontal cortical activation during anticipation of an uncertain reward or loss in bipolar versus major depression. Bipolar Disorders, 15(8), 839–854. 10.1111/bdi.12132 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, & Aiken LS (2013). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Colodro-Conde L, Couvy-Duchesne B, Zhu G, Coventry WL, Byrne EM, Gordon S, & Martin NG (2018). A direct test of the diathesis–stress model for depression. Molecular Psychiatry, 23(7), 1590–1596. 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.08.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Di Martino A, Scheres A, Margulies DS, Kelly AMC, Uddin LQ, Shehzad Z, … Milham MP (2008). Functional connectivity of human striatum: A resting state fMRI study. Cerebral Cortex, 18(12), 2735–2747. 10.1093/cercor/bhn041 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Forbes EE, Rodriguez EE, Musselman S, & Narendran R (2014). Prefrontal Response and Frontostriatal Functional Connectivity to Monetary Reward in Abstinent Alcohol-Dependent Young Adults. PLoS ONE, 9(5), e94640. 10.1371/journal.pone.0094640 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis-Raniere EL, Alloy LB, & Abramson LY (2006). Depressive personality styles and bipolar spectrum disorders: Prospective tests of the event congruency hypothesis. Bipolar Disorders, 8(4), 382–399. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2006.00337.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fulford D, Johnson SL, Llabre MM, & Carver CS (2010). Pushing and coasting in dynamic goal pursuit: Coasting is attenuated in bipolar disorder. Psychological Science, 21(7), 1021–1027. 10.1177/0956797610373372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, & Behrens TEJ (2014). The neural network underlying incentive-based learning: implications for interpreting circuit disruptions in psychiatric disorders. Neuron, 83(5), 1019–1039. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, & Knutson B (2010). The reward circuit: Linking primate anatomy and human imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1), 4–26. 10.1038/npp.2009.129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haber SN, & Behrens TEJ (2014). The Neural Network Underlying Incentive-Based Learning: Implications for Interpreting Circuit Disruptions in Psychiatric Disorders. Neuron, 83(5), 1019–1039. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.031 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harada M, Hoaki N, Terao T, Hatano K, Kohno K, Araki Y, … Kochiyama T (2013). Hyperthymic temperament and brightness judgment in healthy subjects: Involvement of left inferior orbitofrontal cortex. Journal of Affective Disorders, 151(1), 143–148. 10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jackson ME, Frost AS, & Moghaddam B (2001). Stimulation of prefrontal cortex at physiologically relevant frequencies inhibits dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neurochemistry, 78(4), 920–923. 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2001.00499.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SL, Edge MD, Holmes MK, & Carver CS (2012). The behavioral activation system and mania. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 8, 243–267. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143148 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SL, Mehta H, Ketter TA, Gotlib IH, & Knutson B (2019). Neural responses to monetary incentives in bipolar disorder. NeuroImage: Clinical, 24, 102018. 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.102018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelley NJ, Kramer AM, Young KS, Echiverri-Cohen AM, Chat IK-Y, Bookheimer SY, … Zinbarg RE (2019). Evidence for a general factor of behavioral activation system sensitivity. Journal of Research in Personality, 79, 30–39. 10.1016/j.jrp.2019.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E, & Hommer D (2000). FMRI visualization of brain activity during a monetary incentive delay task. NeuroImage, 12(1), 20–27. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar P, Berghorst LH, Nickerson LD, Dutra SJ, Goer FK, Greve DN, & Pizzagalli DA (2014). Differential effects of acute stress on anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward processing. Neuroscience, 266, 1–12. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.01.058 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kumar P, Slavich GM, Berghorst LH, Treadway MT, Brooks NH, Dutra SJ, … Pizzagalli DA (2015). Perceived life stress exposure modulates reward-related medial prefrontal cortex responses to acute stress in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 180, 104–111. 10.1016/j.jad.2015.03.035 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lawrence NS, Williams AM, Surguladze S, Giampietro V, Brammer MJ, Andrew C, … Phillips ML (2004). Subcortical and ventral prefrontal cortical neural responses to facial expressions distinguish patients with bipolar disorder and major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 55(6), 578–587. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liu RT, Burke TA, Abramson LY, & Alloy LB (2018). The Behavioral Approach System (BAS) model of vulnerability to bipolar disorder: Evidence of a continuum of BAS sensitivity across adolescence. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46, 1333–1349. 10.1007/s10802-017-0363-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer B, Johnson SL, & Winters R (2001). Responsiveness to threat and incentive in bipolar disorder: Relations of the BIS/BAS scales with symptoms. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23(3), 133–143. 10.1023/A:1010929402770 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miller GA, & Chapman JP (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 40–48. 10.1037/0021-843x.110.1.40 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monroe SM, & Simons AD (1991). Diathesis-stress theories in the context of life stress research: implications for the depressive disorders. Psychological bulletin, 110(3), 406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Murray L, Waller R, & Hyde LW (2018). A systematic review examining the link between psychopathic personality traits, antisocial behavior, and neural reactivity during reward and loss processing. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 9(6), 497. 10.1037/per0000308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ng TH, Alloy LB, & Smith DV (2019). Meta-analysis of reward processing in major depressive disorder reveals distinct abnormalities within the reward circuit. Translational Psychiatry, 9(1), 1–10. 10.1038/s41398-019-0644-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusslock R, Abramson LY, Harmon-Jones E, Alloy LB, & Hogan ME (2007). A goal-striving life event and the onset of hypomanic and depressive episodes and symptoms: Perspective from the Behavioral Approach System (BAS) dysregulation theory. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 105–115. 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusslock R, & Alloy LB (2017). Reward processing and mood-related symptoms: An RDoC and translational neuroscience perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 3–16. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nusslock R, Almeida JR, Forbes EE, Versace A, Frank E, LaBarbara EJ, … Phillips ML (2012). Waiting to win: Elevated striatal and orbitofrontal cortical activity during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder adults. Bipolar Disorders, 14(3), 249–260. 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2012.01012.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ossewaarde L, Qin S, Marle V, Wingen GA, Fernández G, & Hermans EJ (2011). Stress-induced reduction in reward-related prefrontal cortex function. NeuroImage, 55(1), 345–352. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD, & Drevets WC (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 13, 833–857. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samanez-Larkin GR, Gibbs SEB, Khanna K, Nielsen L, Carstensen LL, & Knutson B (2007). Anticipation of monetary gain but not loss in healthy older adults. Nature Neuroscience, 10(6), 787–791. 10.1038/nn1894 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Samanez-Larkin GR, & Knutson B (2015). Decision making in the ageing brain: changes in affective and motivational circuits. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 16(5), 278–289. 10.1038/nrn3917 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Satterthwaite TD, Kable JW, Vandekar L, Katchmar N, Bassett DS, Baldassano CF, … Wolf DH (2015). Common and dissociable dysfunction of the reward system in bipolar and unipolar depression. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(9), 2258–2268. 10.1038/npp.2015.75 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schneider W, Eschman A, Zuccolotto A, & Psychology Software Tools, I. (2002). E-prime reference guide. Psychology Software Tools, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Schreiter S, Spengler S, Willert A, Mohnke S, Herold D, Erk S, … Bermpohl F (2016). Neural alterations of fronto-striatal circuitry during reward anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine, 46(15), 3187–3198. 10.1017/S0033291716001963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, & Gabard-Durnam LJ (2017). The developing amygdala: a student of the world and a teacher of the cortex. Current opinion in psychology, 17, 55–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torrubia R, Ávila C, Moltó J, & Caseras X (2001). The sensitivity to punishment and sensitivity to reward questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 31(6), 837–862. 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, & Zald D (2013). Perceived stress predicts altered reward and loss feedback processing in medial prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00180 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trost S, Diekhof EK, Zvonik K, Lewandowski M, Usher J, Keil M, … Gruber O (2014). Disturbed anterior prefrontal control of the mesolimbic reward system and increased impulsivity in bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39(8), 1914–1923 10.1038/npp.2014.39 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Urosević S, Abramson LY, Alloy LB, Nusslock R, Harmon-Jones E, Bender R, & Hogan ME (2010). Increased rates of events that activate or deactivate the behavioral approach system, but not events related to goal attainment, in bipolar spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 119(3), 610–615. 10.1037/a0019533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitton AE, Treadway MT, & Pizzagalli DA (2015). Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(1), 7–12. 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yip SW, Worhunsky PD, Rogers RD, & Goodwin GM (2015). Hypoactivation of the ventral and dorsal striatum during reward and loss anticipation in antipsychotic and mood stabilizer-naive bipolar disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 40(3), 658–666. 10.1038/npp.2014.215 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Young CB, Chen T, Nusslock R, Keller J, Scatzberg AF, & Menon V (2016). Anhedonia and general distress associated with dissociable connectivity of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in major depressive disorder. Translational Psychiatry, 6, 810. 10.1038/tp.2016.80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zald DH, McHugo M, Ray KL, Glahn DC, Eickhoff SB, & Laird AR (2014). Meta-analytic connectivity modeling reveals differential functional connectivity of the medial and lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 24(1), 232–248. 10.1093/cercor/bhs308 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zald DH, & Treadway MT (2017). Reward Processing, Neuroeconomics, and Psychopathology. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 13(1), 471–495. 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032816-044957 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.