Abstract
The relationship between social support and well-being is well established in social psychology, with evidence suggesting that these benefits are especially prominent among women. When faced with an environmental stressor, women are more likely to adopt a tend-and-befriend strategy rather than fight-or-flight. Furthermore, female friendships tend to be higher in self-disclosure and more frequently relied on for social support, which is associated with physical and psychological benefits. Women are also more effective at providing social support, further augmenting those benefits. We begin with an overview of the characteristics of women's social ties and how they can be especially useful in times of stress. We then transition to the benefits of female social networks even in the absence of negative events and incorporate research from health and social psychology to consider the positive implications of having strong social bonds and the negative implications of lacking such bonds. Additionally, we consider cross-cultural differences in tendencies to seek out social support and its subsequent benefits, as well as the need for more research with culturally diverse samples. It remains unclear the extent to which patterns of social support benefits for women vary cross-culturally.
This article is part of the theme issue ‘Cooperation among women: evolutionary and cross-cultural perspectives’.
Keywords: social support, well-being, stress, affiliation, sex differences
1. Introduction
One of the key benefits of human bonds is social support, or the ability to turn to others during positive and negative life events and receive some form of aid or supportive response [1,2]. Indeed, humans invest quite heavily into building strong friendships with non-kin, with the hope that should they fall upon bad times or encounter conflict in the future, they will still have someone invested enough in their welfare to provide support [3–5]. The relationship between social support and well-being is robust in social psychology. Social integration is generally associated with positive well-being outcomes, and in times of stress, social support can serve as a buffer against adverse physical and psychological outcomes [1,6–8]. Social support similarly promotes well-being in the face of positive events, as evidenced by work on capitalization and sharing positive news with others [9–11].
However, not every relationship is equally effective in providing these benefits. Some people may be unwilling to provide support due to low intimacy, lack of reciprocity norms or the person in need not demonstrating enough active coping mechanisms or support seeking [12]. People also intentionally seek out different relationships for different types of support. For example, more socially anxious college students tend to increasingly seek out family members for support and decreasingly seek out other-gender friends [13]. Similarly, people report having specific relationships that they turn to for regulating specific emotional needs, such as cheering up sadness or calming down anxiety (i.e. ‘emotionships’; [14]). Thus, it is important to understand which relationships may be more conducive to reaping the health benefits associated with quality social support provision.
One of the ongoing approaches to this question has been to examine sex differences in the association between social support and well-being. Research suggests that women are more effective at providing social support relative to men for both same-sex and opposite-sex others [15,16]. Women are also more likely to seek out and receive social support when faced with environmental stressors compared to men [17], suggesting that social support benefits may be especially prominent among women. Indeed, some research suggests that women are more sensitive to social isolation or low social support compared to men (e.g. [18,19]). However, it remains unclear whether these patterns are universal or cross-culturally variable. Variations in individualist and collectivist orientations, socialization practices, and culturally defined gender roles may moderate the effectiveness of different forms and sources of social support. However, much of the existing research on social support has been conducted in western industrialized samples. The purpose of this paper is to review existing evidence on the benefits of women's social ties, both in times of stress and in the absence of negative events, and consider the generalizability of those benefits for physical, psychological and relationship health. In turn, we hope to highlight future areas of inquiry that would be helpful in furthering our understanding of female friendships.
2. Characteristics of women's social ties
(a) . Success in ancestral environments
To begin, we briefly review some of the characteristics of female social bonds that may have been key to survival in ancestral environments. We specifically focus on aspects of female cooperation, but also recognize that there is an extensive literature on female cooperation and competition in ancestral environments that may be of interest to some readers (e.g. [20,21]). In regard to cooperation, however, researchers hypothesize that women's social ties may have played important roles in the domains of food acquisition, childcare and protection (for a thorough review, see [22]). First, having a strong female network would have been beneficial for optimizing food acquisition, as both kin and non-kin members could share information about food source locations, gather the food together or share the responsibility of childcare through allomothering, thus freeing up more time to gather food [23]. The extent of cooperation with non-kin women relative to kin members could vary depending on societal factors such as the type of cooperative labour network, post-marital residence or household mobility (see [24–26]). Still, reciprocity in childcare may have been a strong incentive for cooperation among non-kin others, as illustrated in present-day forager and boat-dwelling communities (e.g. [27,28]).
Similarly, female–female bonds may have been an important source of protection and mutual aid. The frequent absence of men during hunting parties may have made women vulnerable to predation, and even in the presence of men, recurrent instances of male aggression would have been better deterred by strong female coalitions [23,29]. Furthermore, in the case of patrilocal societies, women would have been incentivized to befriend other non-kin women in order to gain access to resources and reciprocal benefits that they no longer had after leaving their kin network [22,30]. Female cooperation could also have been important in non-patrilocal societies, particularly if the women did not have direct control over resources or were concerned with maintaining favourable reputations [31]. In turn, same-sex female friendships would have prioritized reciprocity, equal power status, kindness/generosity and loyalty/commitment in the face of conflict so as to best capitalize on the benefits afforded by those relationships [22].
(b) . Tend-and-befriend strategy
The benefits of female social ties may also have a biobehavioural basis, with one particular feature being the tendency to respond to stress with protective and affiliative behaviour, better known as the tend-and-befriend strategy. Whereas males typically react to stress with a fight-or-flight strategy, females are more likely to react by seeking support and trying to help one another [17,32,33]. This response is based in the attachment/caregiving system that is also responsible for maternal bonding [17], with the release of oxytocin triggering affiliative and protective behaviour aimed at alleviating the stress response by reducing stress-induced cortisol levels [32]. Indeed, after being exposed to a laboratory stressor, women were more likely to make cooperative and other-oriented social decisions, whereas men were more likely to make competitive and selfish decisions, mirroring the two strategies of tend-and-befriend and fight-or-flight [34]. Success with the tend-and-befriend strategy is likely contingent on women having a social support network to call upon during distress. Accordingly, women ought to place greater emphasis on maintaining such a support network, and indeed, girls tend to have more connection-oriented goals and greater concerns about rejection and interpersonal vulnerability compared to boys (see [35] for a review). By maintaining a strong support network, women can be better equipped to respond to stress through affiliation and subsequent triggering of the oxytocin response, further emphasizing the importance of female social ties for promoting overall health and well-being.
(c) . Other general characteristics
Research from communication and social psychology highlights other socialization-based perspectives on the benefits of female friendships. Attachment theory, for instance, suggests that early experiences with attachment figures shape expectations for social interactions later in life [36,37]. Indeed, adolescent girls with high-quality maternal relationships tend to have both a higher quantity and quality of female friends, suggesting that their mothers modelled a socioemotional foundation for successfully establishing a strong social support network with other women [38]. Other perspectives emphasize same-sex peer socialization as a basis for friendship values, with increased same-sex interactions leading to more sex-typed behaviours [35]. Starting in early childhood and continuing through development and adulthood, girls tend to be higher in self-disclosure and intimacy relative to boys and emphasize emotional sharing and talking over shared activities [35,39–42]. Both self-disclosure and emotional support are perceived as important for promoting intimacy among women [39,43], and women's friendships are primarily maintained through support provisioning and being open with one another [44]. Along these lines, women are also more prone to experiencing friendship jealousy over the potential loss of a same-sex friend compared to men, presumably because women invest more in their friendships (making them harder to replace) and face a greater risk of personal information being exposed given the high levels of self-disclosure during intimacy development [45]. Thus, not only do women respond to stress with affiliative behaviour, but they also create and maintain relationships that may be particularly conducive to openness and seeking comfort in times of stress.
However, another behaviour related to self-disclosure is co-rumination, or excessively talking about a negative problem or stressor with someone [46]. As with self-disclosure, women tend to engage in co-rumination more often than men, starting from early childhood and continuing into adulthood [46–48]. While self-disclosure is generally associated with benefits for relationship satisfaction and intimacy (see [49] for a review), the co-rumination aspect of female friendship is accompanied by several tradeoffs. On the one hand, co-rumination contributes to positive friendship adjustment and higher quality relationships among girls [46–48]. In fact, co-rumination has been linked to adrenocortical attunement, which may serve as a bonding mechanism in the same way as oxytocin release does for stress reduction during the tend-and-befriend response [50]. However, co-rumination frequency also predicts increased depressive and anxiety symptoms [46–48], highlighting an important drawback to this coping and bonding mechanism. Nevertheless, the prominence of co-rumination in women's friendships underscores the general value of self-disclosure and emotional support, both of which facilitate reliance on female bonds in the presence and absence of negative life events.
3. Benefits of women's social ties for well-being
(a) . Social bonds as buffers against stress
As noted in our opening paragraph, a large literature has shown that the quality and availability of a caring social network is closely linked to health and well-being. For example, a meta-analysis by Holt-Lunstad et al. [51] found that across numerous large epidemiological studies, people who reported having fewer or low-quality social connections had a much higher mortality rate at follow-up than those who reported having robust high-quality connections to others. Across the world, research has also shown that the availability of a supportive social network is associated with well-being and happiness (e.g. [52]) and social isolation is a risk factor for mental health symptomology (e.g. [53]). Furthermore, the critical role that social bonds play in health and well-being has been demonstrated in real time on the world stage over the past 2 years as the COVID-19 pandemic strained social networks and a secondary pandemic of felt social isolation took hold (e.g. [54]).
Although the literature on mechanisms linking social integration to health and well-being is incomplete, one of the most cited and researched pathways is the role that social ties play in mitigating the deleterious effects of life's challenges. Specifically, negatively valanced emotions, such as fear, anxiety and anger, serve important functions for survival because they can serve as signals of potential threats and activate an appropriate stress response (e.g. [55]). However, repeated, prolonged or chronic experience of negative emotions or experiencing a stress response that is disproportionate or even unwarranted by the situation is detrimental to health and well-being (e.g. [56]). In short, negative events and other stressors are part of life, but how people respond to both major problems and the everyday hassles of life predicts physical health and well-being.
Research on stress responses suggests that social networks play a critical role in both our perceptions of and reactions to potentially threatening or challenging situations (e.g. [57]). We know that people can and often do turn to others for comfort, advice, assistance or simple connection in times of stress [7,58]. Studies in the laboratory have shown that women benefit from receiving support from others when undergoing stressful tasks. Specifically, studies have shown that women experiencing stressors display a mitigated physiological (e.g. heart rate, cortisol production) or psychological (e.g. anxiety, distress) response to stress when they receive social support from others before, during or directly after the stressful task when compared to men receiving support or women who are receiving no support (e.g. [59,60]).
(b) . Importance of social support quality
What is also clear from the body of research on social support provision is that the quality of social support matters a great deal. When support is unskilled, overbearing or misses the mark in some way, the physiological and psychological response to the stressor can be exacerbated (e.g. [59]). Low-quality social support leads recipients to feel indebted, incompetent or may highlight weakness or draw more attention to the stressor (e.g. [61]). For these reasons, the quality and timing of social support is critical in determining whether that support will have a beneficial or harmful impact on the recipient; support that is responsive to the recipient's needs is beneficial, whereas support that is unresponsive to the recipient's needs can lead to increased stress (e.g. [62]).
Interestingly, there is empirical evidence that women are more skilled than men at offering social support. Specifically, women seem to be better at calibrating their social support provision to the stress level of the recipient. For example, Neff & Karney [63] found that while men and women provided similar levels of support over time, they differed in how they responded to the changing needs of the recipient over time. That is, women tended to provide more support on days the recipient felt more stress and less support on days of low stress. Men's support provision, however, was not closely calibrated to the needs of the recipient. In addition, there is evidence that when undergoing stress themselves, women still provide high-quality support to close others, whereas the quality of men's support provision declines as their own stress levels increase [64]. Support that is unwanted, ill-timed, overbearing or otherwise unresponsive to the recipient's needs is particularly problematic for the recipient (e.g. [65]), suggesting that members of female friendship networks may be, on average, more likely to provide higher quality support when needed, even under stressful circumstances. However, it is unclear the degree to which gender role socialization and cultural norms shape these differences in support provision.
In addition, there is evidence that women are particularly adept at readily activating their support networks in times of stress [66]. That is, compared to men, women tend seek out social support more often, as well as provide social support more often [67,68]. Women also report engaging in more coping strategies of all types, including social support seeking (e.g. [69]). However, women also tend to report more stressors in everyday life than men (e.g. [70]). For example, Day & Livingstone [71] found that women tended to rate hypothetical problems as more stressful than men, but also reported seeking support from their friends and networks more readily than men. Thus, it is difficult to disentangle women's higher tendency to seek support from their increased reported stress levels.
However, given that women self-disclose to a greater degree than men in their social relationships (e.g. [39]) and are more likely to respond with affiliation tendencies when stressed compared to men (e.g. [32]), it would stand to reason that women benefit from their support networks more than men do, especially in times of stress. However, the empirical findings on gender differences in social support benefits are more complicated (e.g. [72]). Most studies show that the strong association between lacking a social support network and mortality risk are consistent across gender (e.g. [51]). However, careful examination of the data suggests that different networks may be responsible for those similar effects. For example, in the classic epidemiological study, Berkman & Syme [73] showed that men who were not married carried a much higher risk of mortality than unmarried women, while women's mortality risk was much more strongly tied to their network of friends and community than men's mortality risk (see also [72]). This latter finding may lend support to evolutionary perspectives on women's social ties that are grounded in the extent to which women in ancestral environments may have had to rely on non-kin for childcare, resources and protection.
(c) . Mental processes and social support
Reaching out to others in times of stress can also be metaphorical because even our mental representations of our close social ties can influence perceptions of threat and subsequent stress response. That is, research has shown that having the perception that members of our social network would be available to help if it were needed strongly predicts health and well-being (e.g. [74]). And in the lab, for example, Eisenberger et al. [75] found that being able to view someone with whom participants had a close social bond while receiving mildly painful stimuli led to reductions in self-reported pain ratings and less neural activity related to distress. Smith et al. [76] similarly found that women who wrote about a supportive social tie showed less physiological reactively to a stressful speech task compared to those who wrote about an acquaintance who was not considered a source of support.
Finally, there is some evidence that social networks might influence the number of events in one's life that are experienced as a stressor in the first place. In order to experience an event or stimuli as negative or threatening, it needs to be noted and perceived as a problem. However, work in non-human primates suggests that group size varies in accordance with threats of predators such that group size increases with greater threat from predators in the environment [77]. In their Social Baseline Theory, Beckes & Coan [78] describe a process called risk distribution. They argue that the presence of benevolent social partners was evolutionarily advantageous and also, ironically, reduced our vigilance to threat. Specifically, the social baseline theory in humans proposes that as the number of people in a socially cooperative group increases, the statistical likelihood of environmental risks (e.g. being attacked by predators) for any one individual in a group goes down. Vigilance to threat could be calibrated to the availability of a social network because the social partners literally provided more eyes and ears on the environment. This reduced threat vigilance can mean that smaller or more distant threats are not perceived as such when in a group of benevolent social partners. The opposite effect is also posited by the theory; isolation or being part of a hostile social group should increase vigilance and lowers the threshold of threat detection, such that small or distant dangers are noted and interpreted as threats in these conditions (e.g. [79]).
Research by Eisenberger et al. [80] is consistent with this hypothesis; participants who reported interacting more often with supportive others in a daily diary study showed less neural distress during a later laboratory stressor. It is reasonable to suggest that because women's social networks are characterized as more intimate, supportive and disclosing than men's networks, they are more likely to experience the availability of benevolent social ties than men and in turn experience fewer threats in their environment. However, more research is needed to understand how gender might interact with the predictions of Social Baseline Theory, especially in light of evidence suggesting that women may interpret hypothetical stressful situations more negatively than men, all else being equal (e.g. [71]).
(d) . Social bonds in the absence of stress
Although it is clear that social bonds serve as a buffer in times of stress, there is also a large literature showing a direct effect of social bonds on health and well-being (e.g. [7]). That is, even in the absence of negative events, people who are integrated into a benevolent social network have higher well-being and better health than those who are more socially isolated or, worse, report having hostile or ambivalent social ties (e.g. [81]). Indeed, the negative impact that social isolation and feelings of loneliness have on their own, independent of other life stressors, are well-documented across studies of both humans and non-human primates. Harlow et al.'s [82] primate studies dramatically showed that social isolation can interrupt developmental processes and have a long-lasting impact on overall functioning. In humans, feelings of loneliness and social deprivation are associated with dramatic increases in the risk for mental illness onset or the exacerbation of existing mental health symptoms (e.g. [53]). Across cultures, the loss of close bonds with family, friends and partners by death, divorce, relationship dissolution or disagreement are ranked as some of the most stressful negative events one can experience (e.g. [83]). These consistent associations between a lack of social bonds and lower well-being further emphasizes the importance of social support networks for promoting health and well-being, regardless of whether a stressor currently exists.
Even though the detrimental effects of social isolation and loneliness seem universal, women seem to be more sensitive to social isolation and social rejection than men. For example, in laboratory studies women showed stronger cortisol stress reactivity than men to a failure when it was social in nature but not when it was an achievement failure [19]. Women also report higher levels of loneliness across age groups [84], and this is especially so when subjective measures of ‘feeling lonely’ are used over more objective measure, such as the number of friends one has in their network [85]. On the flipside, women in particular see strong physical and mental health benefits of having a network of close and trusting friends and family [38,86,87]. Thus, even in the absence of an immediate or ongoing stressor, having a strong social network is crucial for supporting physical and psychological health, especially among women.
4. Cross-cultural differences in social support benefits
One should exercise caution, however, in generalizing the benefits of women's social ties to all women. Although research on gender–culture interactions in social support benefits is quite limited, some studies suggest that gender differences are more prevalent than cultural differences. For example, both Israeli Arab and Jewish women report higher availability and importance of emotional social support relative to men [88]. Similarly, emotional and informational support from various community members consistently mitigated the negative effects of discrimination on life satisfaction for both Latin American and Chinese female migrants in Spain but did not consistently provide such benefits for male migrants of both cultures [89]. However, other studies suggest that sociocultural factors may influence the extent of support effectiveness. In the same study, Pines & Zaidman [88] found that Israeli Arabs reported higher quality relationships with potential sources of support than Israeli Jews did. Hussein et al. [90] also found that, despite having higher levels of perceived social support, Arab mothers had higher levels of post-traumatic stress symptoms following a child's traumatic medical event compared to Jewish mothers. In this case, the negative experiences of being an ethnic minority and exposure to other forms of violence may have moderated the buffering effects of social support. Thus, it is important to consider differences in life experiences, social upbringing and self-construal prior to generalizing the benefits of female social support across cultures.
(a) . Self-construal as individualist versus collectivist
One primary distinction in cross-cultural research is between individualism and collectivism, or the extent to which individuals perceive themselves as independent and unique or interdependent and connected to others [91]. A key value of collectivist societies is preserving harmony and putting others' needs above the self, which can manifest in a hesitancy to seek out social support and not wanting to harm or burden close others with one's problems [58,91,92]. Indeed, Asians and Asian Americans are less likely to seek out social support in response to stressors compared to European Americans, with relational and group harmony concerns being the primary reasons for this reluctance [58,93–95]. Thus, whereas people in individualist societies may seek out social support to fulfil personal goals and a desire for self-expression, people in collectivist societies appear to forgo such direct requests due to broader concerns with collective goals and maintaining a harmonious social environment [58,91,96].
However, this evidence does not suggest that those from collectivistic societies do not use or derive benefits from social support. Instead, the type of support considered to be most effective may vary, with those from collectivist cultures benefiting from instrumental, implicit and unsolicited support, and those from individualist cultures benefiting from emotional, explicit and solicited support [96]. Whereas explicitly asking for emotional support can be perceived as burdening another individual, implicit support in the form of recognizing the presence of supportive others does not disrupt relational harmony and instead reinforces the presence of a reliable social network [92,96]. Consistent with this idea of cultural fit in social support, Mortenson et al. [94] found that European Americans were more likely to seek out emotional support compared to sojourning Chinese students, and Chinese students had a stronger preference for instrumental support than emotional support. Similarly, when Asians and Asian Americans were primed to think about soliciting explicit social support, they experienced heightened cortisol and psychological stress responses to a laboratory stressor, whereas European Americans experienced heightened stress responses when primed to think about their implicit social support group [92]. That is, social support was only beneficial to the extent that the type of support fit with one's cultural values and self-construal. Thus, whereas emotional expression and self-disclosure may lead to well-being outcomes in individualist societies, the same may not be true for collectivist societies in which more indirect forms of social support are beneficial [96].
(b) . Cultural differences within the context of women's social ties
Based on this evidence, the nature of women's social ties may differ depending on whether one's self-construal is more independent or interdependent. For example, women in the United States and Russia reported higher levels of emotional reliance on friends and family relative to women in Turkey and Korea, with these two groupings corresponding to relatively more individualist and collectivist countries, respectively [97]. Similarly, European American women indicated a greater tendency to seek out emotional support relative to men, whereas Chinese women did not differ from Chinese men in their preferences for emotional support [94]. Thus, the extent to which women rely on each other for emotional support may vary, with women in individualistic societies having greater emotional reliance than those in collectivist societies. Furthermore, a study by Morling et al. [98] found that pregnant women in the United States experienced more positive pregnancy outcomes when they prioritized the individual coping strategy of acceptance, whereas pregnant women in Japan experienced more positive outcomes when they prioritized social assurance as a coping strategy. Thus, the ways in which women benefit from social support ties may also vary, with women in collectivist cultures placing greater emphasis on interdependent rather than independent outcomes relative to women in individualist cultures.
At the same time, having a strong social network may be of greater importance in collectivist societies than in individualist societies. Indeed, not having a best friend and lacking reciprocity in close friendships was associated with loneliness among women in Greece, a collectivist society, but not in the United States [99]. Similarly, negative-quality social relationships were associated with depressed affect for women in Japan and France but not in the United States [100]. Thus, although direct reliance on social support in the face of ongoing stressors may be more detrimental, or at least less helpful, to women in collectivist societies, the lack of an ongoing social support network, regardless of the presence of stressors, may also be more detrimental to women in collectivist societies. However, the individualist–collectivist distinction is but one of many sociocultural dimensions that may explain cross-culture differences in social support. In order to better understand the extent to which social support benefits can be generalized across cultures, more research is needed on the intersection between gender and culture in regard to what women's social ties can offer for physical, psychological and relational health.
5. Conclusion
The current research suggests that social support is fundamental to health and well-being and that the nature of women's social ties (i.e. high self-disclosure, intimacy, etc.) may make social support especially important and beneficial for women. Women not only seek out social support more often than men but are also more skilled at providing responsive social support to others and are more sensitive to the absence of strong social ties in general. Future work is needed to uncover the biological pathways that might link these patterns to outcomes such as physical health and psychological well-being. It is also not clear the extent to which any differences in men's and women's social support behaviours stem from present-day cultural norms. The majority of research has focused heavily on western industrialized samples and studies that examine cross-cultural and contextual differences often ignore gender. Thus, in order to more fully understand the nature of female social bonds and their role in health, well-being and reproduction, more research is needed that explicitly examines gender differences across variable social and cultural contexts.
Data accessibility
This article has no additional data.
Authors' contributions
A.B.: conceptualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; S.L.G.: conceptualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
We received no funding for this study.
References
- 1.Feeney BC, Collins NL. 2015. A new look at social support: a theoretical perspective on thriving through relationships. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 19, 113-147. ( 10.1177/1088868314544222) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Gable SL, Bedrov A. 2021. Social isolation and social support in good times and bad times. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 44, 89-93. ( 10.1016/j.copsyc.2021.08.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.DeScioli P, Kurzban R. 2009. The alliance hypothesis for human friendship. PLoS ONE 4, e5802. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0005802) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Silk JB. 2003. Cooperation without counting: the puzzle of friendship. In Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (ed. Hammerstein P.), pp. 37-54. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tooby J, Cosmides L. 1996. Friendship and the banker's paradox: other pathways to the evolution of adaptations for altruism. In Proceedings of the British Academy. Evolution of social behaviour patterns in primates and man (eds Runciman WG, Smith JM, Dubar RIM), Vol. 88, pp. 119-143. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cohen S. 1988. Psychosocial models of the role of social support in the etiology of physical disease. Health Psychol. 7, 269-297. ( 10.1037/0278-6133.7.3.269) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cohen S, Wills TA. 1985. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol. Bull. 98, 310-357. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Wilcox BL. 1981. Social support, life stress, and psychological adjustment: a test of the buffering hypothesis. Am. J. Commun. Psychol. 9, 371-386. ( 10.1007/bf00918169) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Gable SL, Reis HT. 2010. Good news! Capitalizing on positive events in an interpersonal context. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 42, 195-257. ( 10.1016/S0065-2601(10)42004-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Gable SL, Reis HT, Impett EA, Asher ER. 2004. What do you do when things go right? The intrapersonal and interpersonal benefits of sharing positive events. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 228-245. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.228) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Reis HT, Smith SM, Carmichael CL, Caprariello PA, Tsai FF, Rodrigues A, Maniaci MR. 2010. Are you happy for me? How sharing positive events with others provides personal and interpersonal benefits. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 99, 311-329. ( 10.1037/a0018344) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dunkel-Schetter C, Skokan LA. 1990. Determinants of social support provision in personal relationships. J. Soc. Pers. Relationship. 7, 437-450. ( 10.1177/0265407590074002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Caldwell RA, Reinhart MA. 1988. The relationship of personality to individual differences in the use of type and source of social support. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 6, 140-146. ( 10.1521/jscp.1988.6.1.140) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cheung EO, Gardner WL, Anderson JF. 2015. Emotionships: examining people’s emotion-regulation relationships and their consequences for well-being. Social Psychol. Personality Sci. 6, 407-414. ( 10.1177/1948550614564223) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Glynn LM, Christenfeld N, Gerin W. 1999. Gender, social support, and cardiovascular responses to stress. Psychosom. Med. 61, 234-242. ( 10.1097/00006842-199903000-00016) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sarason IG, Sarason BR, Shearin EN. 1986. Social support as an individual difference variable: its stability, origins, and relational aspects. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 50, 845-855. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.50.4.845) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RA, Updegraff JA. 2000. Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol. Rev. 107, 411-429. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kendler KS, Myers J, Prescott CA. 2005. Sex differences in the relationship between social support and risk for major depression: a longitudinal study of opposite-sex twin pairs. Am. J. Psychiatry 162, 250-256. ( 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.250) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stroud LR, Salovey P, Epel ES. 2002. Sex differences in stress responses: social rejection versus achievement stress. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 318-327. ( 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01333-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Benenson JF. 2014. Warriors and worriers: the survival of the sexes. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Campbell A. 2013. A mind of her own: the evolutionary psychology of women. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Reynolds TA. 2022. Our grandmothers' legacy: challenges faced by female ancestors leave traces in modern women's same-sex relationships. Arch. Sex. Behav. 51, 3225-3256. ( 10.1007/s10508-020-01768-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Taylor S, Gonzaga G. 2006. Evolution, relationships, and health: the social shaping hypothesis. In Evolution and social psychology (eds Schaller M, Simpson JA, Kenrick DT), pp. 211-236. New York, NY: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kraft T, et al. 2021. Female cooperative labor networks in hunter-gatherers and horticulturalists. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210431. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0431) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Page A, Migliano A, Viguier S, Smith D, Dyble M, Hassan A. 2021. Sedentarisation and maternal childcare networks: role of risk-buffering, gender and demography. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210435. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0435) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Seabright E, et al. 2021. Repercussions of patrilocal residence on mothers’ social support networks among Tsimane forager-farmers. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210442. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0442) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Page AE, et al. 2019. Testing adaptive hypotheses of alloparenting in Agta foragers. Nat. Hum. Behav. 3, 1154-1163. ( 10.1038/s41562-019-0679-2) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Starkweather K, Reynolds A, Zohora F, Alam N. 2021. Shodagor women cooperate across domains of work and childcare to solve an adaptive problem. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 378, 20210433. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0433) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Smuts B. 1992. Male aggression against women. Hum. Nat. 3, 1-44. ( 10.1007/BF02692265) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Geary DC, Flinn MV. 2002. Sex differences in behavioral and hormonal response to social threat: commentary on Taylor et al. (2000). Psychol. Rev. 109, 745-750. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.109.4.745) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Rucas SL, Alami S. 2022. Female–female competition occurs irrespective of patrilocality. Arch. Sex. Behav. 51, 3287-3292. ( 10.1007/s10508-021-02221-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Taylor SE. 2006. Tend and befriend: biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 15, 273-277. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00451.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Turton S, Campbell C. 2005. Tend and befriend versus fight or flight: gender differences in behavioral response to stress among university students. J. Appl. Biobehav. Res. 10, 209-232. ( 10.1111/j.1751-9861.2005.tb00013.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Nickels N, Kubicki K, Maestripieri D. 2017. Sex differences in the effects of psychosocial stress on cooperative and prosocial behavior: evidence for ‘flight or fight’ in males and ‘tend and befriend ‘in females. Adapt. Hum. Behav. Physiol. 3, 171-183. ( 10.1007/s40750-017-0062-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rose AJ, Rudolph KD. 2006. A review of sex differences in peer relationship processes: potential trade-offs for the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychol. Bull. 132, 98-131. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.98) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Ainsworth MS. 1979. Infant–mother attachment. Am. Psychol. 34, 932-937. ( 10.1037/0003-066X.34.10.932) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bowlby J. 1969. Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: attachment. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]
- 38.Rodrigues MA, Sanford SR, Rogers MP, Lee KM, Wilson MA, Amos J, Hunter CD, Clancy KB. 2020. From maternal tending to adolescent befriending: the adolescent transition of social support. Am. J. Primatol. 82, e23050. ( 10.1002/ajp.23050) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Bauminger N, Finzi-Dottan R, Chason S, Har-Even D. 2008. Intimacy in adolescent friendship: the roles of attachment, coherence, and self-disclosure. J. Soc. Pers. Relationship. 25, 409-428. ( 10.1177/0265407508090866) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Caldwell MA, Peplau LA. 1982. Sex differences in same-sex friendship. Sex Roles 8, 721-732. ( 10.1007/BF00287568) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hall JA. 2011. Sex differences in friendship expectations: a meta-analysis. J. Soc. Pers. Relationship. 28, 723-747. ( 10.1177/0265407510386192) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Jones DC. 1991. Friendship satisfaction and gender: an examination of sex differences in contributors to friendship satisfaction. J. Soc. Pers. Relationship. 8, 167-185. ( 10.1177/0265407591082002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Fehr B. 2004. Intimacy expectations in same-sex friendships: a prototype interaction-pattern model. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 265-284. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.265) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Oswald DL, Clark EM, Kelly CM. 2004. Friendship maintenance: an analysis of individual and dyad behaviors. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 23, 413-441. ( 10.1521/jscp.23.3.413.35460) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Krems JL, Williams KEG, Merrie LA, Kendrick DT, Aktipis A. 2022. Sex (similarities and) differences in friendship jealousy. Evol. Hum. Behav. 43, 97-106. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2021.11.005) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rose AJ. 2002. Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and boys. Child Dev. 73, 1830-1843. ( 10.1111/1467-8624.00509) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Calmes CA, Roberts JE. 2008. Rumination in interpersonal relationships: does co-rumination explain gender differences in emotional distress and relationship satisfaction among college students? Cogn. Ther. Res. 32, 577-590. ( 10.1007/s10608-008-9200-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Rose AJ, Carlson W, Waller EM. 2007. Prospective associations of co-rumination with friendship and emotional adjustment: considering the socioemotional trade-offs of co-rumination. Dev. Psychol. 43, 1019-1031. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.1019) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Collins NL, Miller LC. 1994. Self-disclosure and liking: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 116, 457-475. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.116.3.457) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rankin A, Swearingen-Stanborough C, Granger DA, Byrd-Craven J. 2018. The role of co-rumination and adrenocortical attunement in young women's close friendships. Psychoneuroendocrinology 98, 61-66. ( 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.07.027) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. 2010. Social relationships and mortality risk: a meta-analytic review. PLoS Med. 7, e1000316. ( 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L. 2018. Advances in subjective well-being research. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2, 253-260. ( 10.1038/s41562-018-0307-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ernst JM, Cacioppo JT. 1999. Lonely hearts: psychological perspectives on loneliness. Appl. Prev. Psychol. 8, 1-22. ( 10.1016/S0962-1849(99)80008-0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Pancani L, Marinucci M, Aureli N, Riva P. 2021. Forced social isolation and mental health: a study on 1,006 Italians under COVID-19 lockdown. Front. Psychol. 12, 1540. ( 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.663799) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tooby J, Cosmides L. 2008. The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In Handbook of emotions (eds Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM, Barrett LF), pp. 114-137. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Repetti RL, Taylor SE, Seeman TE. 2002. Risky families: family social environments and the mental and physical health of offspring. Psychol. Bull. 128, 330-366. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.330) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT. 2009. Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Health Psychol. 28, 354-363. ( 10.1037/a0014400) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Kim HS, Sherman DK, Taylor SE. 2008. Culture and social support. Am. Psychol. 63, 518-526. ( 10.1037/0003-066X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Kors DJ, Linden W, Gerin W. 1997. Evaluation interferes with social support: effects on cardiovascular stress reactivity in women. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 16, 1-23. ( 10.1521/jscp.1997.16.1.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Uno D, Uchino BN, Smith TW. 2002. Relationship quality moderates the effect of social support given by close friends on cardiovascular reactivity in women. Int. J. Behav. Med. 9, 243-262. ( 10.1207/S15327558IJBM0903_06) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Rafaeli E, Gleason ME. 2009. Skilled support within intimate relationships. J. Fam. Theory Rev. 1, 20-37. ( 10.1111/j.1756-2589.2009.00003.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Maisel NC, Gable SL. 2009. The paradox of received social support: the importance of responsiveness. Psychol. Sci. 20, 928-932. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02388.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Neff LA, Karney BR. 2005. Gender differences in social support: a question of skill or responsiveness? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 88, 79-90. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.79) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bodenmann G, Meuwly N, Germann J, Nussbeck FW, Heinrichs M, Bradbury TN. 2015. Effects of stress on the social support provided by men and women in intimate relationships. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1584-1594. ( 10.1177/0956797615594616) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Rini C, Schetter CD, Hobel CJ, Glynn LM, Sandman CA. 2006. Effective social support: antecedents and consequences of partner support during pregnancy. Pers. Relationship. 13, 207-229. ( 10.1111/j.1475-6811.2006.00114.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Barbee AP, Cunningham MR, Winstead BA, Derlega VJ, Gulley MR, Yankeelov PA, Druen PB. 1993. Effects of gender role expectations on the social support process. J. Soc. Issues 49, 175-190. ( 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1993.tb01175.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Dalgard OS, Dowrick C, Lehtinen V, Vazquez-Barquero JL, Casey P, Wilkinson G, Ayuso-Mateos JL, Page H, Dunn G. 2006. Negative life events, social support and gender difference in depression. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 41, 444-451. ( 10.1007/s00127-006-0051-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Zhou B, Heather D, Cesare AD, Ryder AG. 2017. Ask and you might receive: the actor–partner interdependence model approach to estimating cultural and gender variations in social support. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 412-428. ( 10.1002/ejsp.2251) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Tamres LK, Janicki D, Helgeson VS. 2002. Sex differences in coping behavior: a meta-analytic review and an examination of relative coping. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 6, 2-30. ( 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0601_1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Cohen S, Williamson GM. 1988. Perceived stress in a probability sample of the United States. In The social psychology of health (eds Spaceapan S, Oskamp S), pp. 31-67. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Day AL, Livingstone HA. 2003. Gender differences in perceptions of stressors and utilization of social support among university students. Can. J. Behav. Sci. 35, 73-83. ( 10.1037/h0087190) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Shumaker SA, Hill DR. 1991. Gender differences in social support and physical health. Health Psychol. 10, 102-111. ( 10.1037/0278-6133.10.2.102) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Berkman LF, Syme SL. 1979. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: a nine-year follow-up study of Alameda county residents. Am. J. Epidemiol. 109, 186-204. ( 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112674) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Kaul M, Lakey B. 2003. Where is the support in perceived support? The role of generic relationship satisfaction and enacted support in perceived support's relation to low distress. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 22, 59-78. ( 10.1521/jscp.22.1.59.22761) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Eisenberger NI, Master SL, Inagaki TK, Taylor SE, Shirinyan D, Lieberman MD, Naliboff BD. 2011. Attachment figures activate a safety signal-related neural region and reduce pain experience. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 11 721-11 726. ( 10.1073/pnas.1108239108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Smith TW, Ruiz JM, Uchino BN. 2004. Mental activation of supportive ties, hostility, and cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory stress in young men and women. Health Psychol. 23, 476-485. ( 10.1037/0278-6133.23.5.476) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Van Schaik CP. 1983. Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour 87, 120-144. ( 10.1163/156853983X00147) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Beckes L, Coan JA. 2011. Social baseline theory: the role of social proximity in emotion and economy of action. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 5, 976-988. ( 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00400.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Coan JA, Schaefer HS, Davidson RJ. 2006. Lending a hand: social regulation of the neural response to threat. Psychol. Sci. 17, 1032-1039. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Eisenberger NI, Taylor SE, Gable SL, Hilmert CJ, Lieberman MD. 2007. Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. Neuroimage 35, 1601-1612. ( 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Holt-Lunstad J, Uchino BN, Smith TW, Hicks A. 2007. On the importance of relationship quality: the impact of ambivalence in friendships on cardiovascular functioning. Ann. Behav. Med. 33, 278-290. ( 10.1007/bf02879910) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Harlow HF, Dodsworth RO, Harlow MK. 1965. Total social isolation in monkeys. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 54, 90-97. ( 10.1073/pnas.54.1.90) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.McAndrew FT, Akande A, Turner S, Sharma Y. 1998. A cross-cultural ranking of stressful life events in Germany, India, South Africa, and the United States. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 29, 717-727. ( 10.1177/0022022198296003) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Nicolaisen M, Thorsen K. 2014. Loneliness among men and women—a five-year follow-up study. Aging Mental Health 18, 194-206. ( 10.1080/13607863.2013.821457) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Borys S, Perlman D. 1985. Gender differences in loneliness. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 11, 63-74. ( 10.1177/0146167285111006) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Almquist YB, Östberg V, Rostila M, Edling C, Rydgren J. 2014. Friendship network characteristics and psychological well-being in late adolescence: exploring differences by gender and gender composition. Scand. J. Public Health 42, 146-154. ( 10.1177/1403494813510793) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Schwarzer R, Leppin A. 1989. Social support and health: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Health 3, 1-15. ( 10.1080/08870448908400361) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Pines AM, Zaidman N. 2003. Gender, culture, and social support: a male–female, Israeli Jewish–Arab comparison. Sex Roles 49, 571-586. ( 10.1023/B:SERS.0000003128.99279.94) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89.García-Cid A, Hombrados-Mendieta I, Gómez-Jacinto L, Millán-Franco M, del Pino-Brunet N. 2020. The moderating effect of gender as a protective factor against discrimination in migrants from Latin America and China. J. Community Psychol. 48, 1964-1984. ( 10.1002/jcop.22395) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Hussein S, Sadeh Y, Dekel R, Shadmi E, Brezner A, Landa J, Silberg T. 2021. Using a biopsychosocial approach to examine differences in post-traumatic stress symptoms between Arab and Jewish Israeli mothers following a child's traumatic medical event. Int. J. Equity Health 20, 1-12. ( 10.1186/s12939-021-01429-y) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Markus HR, Kitayama S. 1991. Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychol. Rev. 98, 224-253. ( 10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Taylor SE, Welch WT, Kim HS, Sherman DK. 2007. Cultural differences in the impact of social support on psychological and biological stress responses. Psychol. Sci. 18, 831-837. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01987.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Kim HS, Sherman DK, Ko D, Taylor SE. 2006. Pursuit of comfort and pursuit of harmony: culture, relationships, and social support seeking. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 32, 1595-1607. ( 10.1177/0146167206291991) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Mortenson ST, Burleson BR, Feng B, Liu M. 2009. Cultural similarities and differences in seeking social support as a means of coping: a comparison of European Americans and Chinese and an evaluation of the mediating effects of self-construal. J. Int. Intercult. Commun. 2, 208-239. ( 10.1080/17513050902985331) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Taylor SE, Sherman DK, Kim HS, Jarcho J, Takagi K, Dunagan MS. 2004. Culture and social support: who seeks it and why? J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 354-362. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.87.3.354) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wu DC, Kim HS, Collins NL. 2021. Perceived responsiveness across cultures: the role of cultural fit in social support use. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 15, e12634. ( 10.1111/spc3.12634) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Ryan RM, La Guardia JG, Solky-Butzel J, Chirkov V, Kim Y. 2005. On the interpersonal regulation of emotions: emotional reliance across gender, relationships, and cultures. Pers. Relationship. 12, 145-163. ( 10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00106.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Morling B, Kitayama S, Miyamoto Y. 2003. American and Japanese women use different coping strategies during normal pregnancy. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 29, 1533-1546. ( 10.1177/0146167203256878) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Malikiosi-Loizos M, Anderson LR. 1999. Accessible friendships, inclusive friendships, reciprocated friendships as related to social and emotional loneliness in Greece and the USA. Eur. Psychol. 4, 165-178. ( 10.1027/1016-9040.4.3.165) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Antonucci TC, Lansford JE, Akiyama H, Smith J, Baltes MM, Takahashi K, Fuhrer R, Dartigues JF. 2002. Differences between men and women in social relations, resource deficits, and depressive symptomatology during later life in four nations. J. Soc. Issues 58, 767-783. ( 10.1111/1540-4560.00289) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
This article has no additional data.