Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2022 Dec 15;17(12):e0278994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0278994

Neuromotor changes in participants with a concussion history can be detected with a custom smartphone app

Christopher K Rhea 1,2,*, Masahiro Yamada 1,3, Nikita A Kuznetsov 1,4, Jason T Jakiela 1,5, Chanel T LoJacono 1,6, Scott E Ross 1, F J Haran 1,7, Jason M Bailie 8,9,10, W Geoffrey Wright 11
Editor: Ryan Thomas Roemmich12
PMCID: PMC9754195  PMID: 36520862

Abstract

Neuromotor dysfunction after a concussion is common, but balance tests used to assess neuromotor dysfunction are typically subjective. Current objective balance tests are either cost- or space-prohibitive, or utilize a static balance protocol, which may mask neuromotor dysfunction due to the simplicity of the task. To address this gap, our team developed an Android-based smartphone app (portable and cost-effective) that uses the sensors in the device (objective) to record movement profiles during a stepping-in-place task (dynamic movement). The purpose of this study was to examine the extent to which our custom smartphone app and protocol could discriminate neuromotor behavior between concussed and non-concussed participants. Data were collected at two university laboratories and two military sites. Participants included civilians and Service Members (N = 216) with and without a clinically diagnosed concussion. Kinematic and variability metrics were derived from a thigh angle time series while the participants completed a series of stepping-in-place tasks in three conditions: eyes open, eyes closed, and head shake. We observed that the standard deviation of the mean maximum angular velocity of the thigh was higher in the participants with a concussion history in the eyes closed and head shake conditions of the stepping-in-place task. Consistent with the optimal movement variability hypothesis, we showed that increased movement variability occurs in participants with a concussion history, for which our smartphone app and protocol were sensitive enough to capture.

Introduction

Concussions have been labeled as a public health concern due to our emerging knowledge of their short- and long-term negative effects [1], including dysfunction in working memory [2], altered sensitivity to sensory information [3], and balance control [4]. The latter has long been used as a clinical indicator of a potential concussion, as the head trauma event can lead to a neurological disruption, which in turn can affect downstream motor functions [5]. Thus, the integration of the neurological system with motor control (termed neuromotor control) affords an opportunity to probe motor behavior that may be disrupted from a concussion. Such was the impetus for balance tests commonly used in concussion assessment and follow-up care, such as the Balance Error Scoring System (BESS) [6], Sensory Organization Test (SOT) [7], and the Balance Tracking System (BTrackS) [8].

When selecting a balance test to be used for concussion assessment, it can either be subjective or objective [9]. Subjective tests, such as the BESS, rely on the perspective of the administrator to grade the participant’s balance performance across various task conditions. While the BESS has been shown to have clinical utility [10], questions about its reliability have been discussed due to the subjective nature of the test [11]. A solution to this challenge is to use an objective test in which sensors and/or a computer quantifies balance control. Such is the case with the SOT, which uses a force plate to measure the center of pressure movement while the participant’s visual, vestibular, and/or somatosensory information is altered in six conditions [7]. Although it is objective, the SOT has the challenge of being cost- and space-prohibitive in most clinical settings. The BTrackS is a more affordable way to objectively measure balance, consisting of a portable force plate connected to an iPad or laptop [8]. Although BtrackS may be suitable for a hospital/rehabilitation setting, it still requires a computer or tablet, which could limit its field deployment capabilities. A method to overcome this limitation can be using a smartphone app (Sway Medical, LLC, Tulsa, OK) [1214]. This method can surpass the above-mentioned limitations (space, cost, portability, objective assessment).

A potential limitation of the existing portable technologies (i.e., BtrackS and Sway Medical) is that they were designed to probe neuromotor control via a static balance task. While such tests do have clinical utility [15, 16], a more challenging dynamic balance task could uncover dysfunctional behavior that may not have been observed in the easier static balance task. This postulate was explored in a recent review in which the authors examined the use of Fitts’ Law as a way to control for and/or scale up task difficulty in balance tasks [17]. While they noted most of the papers in their review only included healthy populations, they did note that Fitts’ law has been used in this context in clinical settings. The idea of scaling up task challenge (or difficulty) to discriminate neuromotor performance is the basis of many clinical tests, including the BESS test. In the original BESS paper where individuals with a concussion were assessed [6], the stances that presented the least difficulty (i.e., controlling balance on a firm surface) exhibited no differences between the concussed and non-concussed groups. However, once task difficulty was scaled up via balance control on a foam surface, discrimination between the groups was observed. Furthermore, a review of gait task analysis for concussions revealed that a more complex task showed differences in many more variables between non-concussed and concussed individuals than a simple gait task [18]. Lastly, in postural control, static balance performance may not reflect performance on a dynamic balance task [19], so they should not be used interchangeably. Thus, there is a need for a portable, objective, and dynamic assessment. To meet this challenge, our team developed an Android-based smartphone app (portable and cost-effective) that uses the sensors in the device (objective) to record movement profiles during a stepping-in-place task (dynamic movement). Version 1 of our app exhibited clinical utility by identifying neuromotor dysfunction in Service Members after blast-exposure [20]. We then developed Version 2 of our app and published its reliability and validity [21].

The theoretical foundation for this line of research is rooted in the observation that variability in movement profiles is informative about the health of the neuromotor system. Unlike the traditional view about variability as noise, it has been proposed that movement variability provides sensitive measures of pathologies and movement disorders [2230]—a position that has been formalized into the optimal movement variability hypothesis [22]. Previous research using this and related frameworks have shown that healthy biological systems exhibit a particular magnitude and structure of variability, while injury, aging, or disease can lead to a behavioral shift in movement such that it becomes more variable [2329]. Relative to head trauma, previous research has shown changes in movement variability following a concussion [31, 32], which suggests that neuromotor dysfunction from a concussion can be tested via variability assessment. Moreover, using a metronome to anchor a timing task and then removing the metronome to examine the timing of the continuation movement (known as the synchronization–continuation paradigm) has a long history in basic motor behavior research [33, 34], along with concussion research [35]. This paradigm affords the ability to look at the response (movement pattern) to a constraint (metronome timing), with the working hypothesis that a neurological insult makes it more difficult to adhere to the timing constraint. Thus, we merged the optimal movement variability hypothesis with the synchronization-continuation paradigm to develop our smartphone dynamic balance protocol in order to create a theory-based and empirically driven approach to probe neuromotor performance after a concussion.

The purpose of the current study was to examine the extent to which the neuromotor behavior data obtained from our smartphone app could associate with a history of concussion. Since concussions are a problem in both civilian [36] and Service Member [37] populations, both groups were recruited for this study. We hypothesized that concussed participants would exhibit neuromotor dysfunction indexed by greater variability in their movement during the dynamic balance task.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 216 participants voluntarily participated in the study (N = 100 healthy civilians, N = 54 healthy Service Members, N = 44 civilians with a concussion history, N = 18 Serviced Members with a concussion history). All participants provided written informed consent. The demographics of N = 54 healthy Service Members were not obtained. Sex of the participants for whom demographics were obtained are as follows: (1) for the 100 healthy civilians, 51 were female and 49 were male, (2) for the 44 civilians with a concussion history, 26 were female and 18 were male, (3) and for the 18 Service Members with a concussion history, 4 were female and 14 were male. The height and weight of participants for whom demographics were available were similar between groups (Table 1). Civilian participants were recruited from two universities in the United States (one in the northeast and one in the southeast). Active-duty Service Members were recruited from two sites in the western United States. Both healthy (non-concussed) and participants with a concussion history were recruited. The sampling strategy at all sites was similar and consisted of a convenience sample from participants who self-identified as qualifying for the study. At the civilian sites, flyers were posted in public areas and information was provided verbally to various groups (e.g., via announcements in university courses and with student-athletes). At the military sites, participants were given the opportunity to volunteer outside of their standard daily duty. Participants in the concussion history group must have received an official diagnosis with a concussion by a medical professional in the past six months to qualify for our study. Inclusion criteria for both groups included: (1) 18–50 years old, (2) at least moderately active (> 3 times of physical activity per week), (3) BMI under 33, (4) non-smoker, (5) normal or corrected-to-normal vision, (6) no surgeries in the past six months, (7) ability to walk with a prosthetic or assistive device, and (8) not pregnant at the time of study data collection. The protocol was approved by two institutional review boards, and all participants signed the informed consent forms. If participants did not have a history of a concussion, they were allocated to the non-concussed healthy control (hereafter, healthy) group. If participants had a history of a concussion, they were assigned to the concussion history group. History of concussion was initially self-reported by the participants at the time of recruitment. However, prior to the day of testing, the participants medical professional provided a medical clearance consent form verifying that their patient did have a concussion within the last 6 months and that they supported them enrolling in our study with the knowledge of the type of testing that would occur. It is important to note that given the duration of time since the concussion event presented in Table 1, our population on average was not an acutely concussed population. Rather, our population was comprised of those who had a clinically diagnosed concussion within the past 6 months, and therefore are labeled as those with a concussion history.

Table 1. Demographics by group (healthy, concussed) and population (civilian, Service Member) (M ± SD).

Demographics by Group (Civilians and Service Members Collapsed)
Category Healthy (n = 154) Concussion History (n = 62) p-value
Height (cm) 171.99 ± 9.70 171.43 ± 12.59 0.749
Weight (kg) 70.93 ± 13.82 75.26 ± 16.19 0.072
Time from concussion
to testing (weeks)
N/A 10.13 ± 8.67 N/A
Healthy and Concussion History within Each Population
Population Civilians Service Members
Category Healthy (n = 100) Concussion History (n = 44) Healthy (n = 54) Concussion History (n = 18)
Height (cm) 171.99 ± 9.70 170.08 ± 13.87 N/A 174.72 ± 8.13
Weight (kg) 70.93 ± 13.82 73.75 ± 17.90 N/A 78.94 ± 13.46
Time from concussion to testing (weeks) N/A 6.52 ± 6.32 N/A 18.16 ± 8.15

Study design and protocol

This study was a cross-sectional design that was part of a larger project that required participants to complete several static and dynamic balance tests in a single testing session. This paper focuses on our dynamic balance test from our smartphone app (named AccWalker). The protocol for AccWalker included strapping a customized armband onto the lateral aspect of the participants’ thigh, halfway between the lateral epicondyle and the greater trochanter, and placing the smartphone in the armband holder (Fig 1A). Participants were instructed to (a) naturally step in place while synchronizing their step timing to the app’s metronome, (b) look straightforward during trials, and (c) during familiarization trials, participants were informed to raise their knees higher if they were not moving as they normally walked (e.g., feet are not leaving the floor). The pacing metronome (period = 0.575 s or 1.15 s per stride) was provided for the initial 10 s of each trial. After 10 s, the metronome turned off, and the participant was instructed to continue stepping for 60 s. No other information was provided to participants in an attempt to capture natural kinematic patterns. The general protocol of the stepping-in-place task consisted of three trials for each of the three following conditions to alter sensory input: (1) with eyes open (EO), (2) with eyes closed (EC) to remove visual information, and (3) while rotating the head left-right to perturb vestibular information (head shake; HS). A 30-second practice session was provided for the EO and HS conditions prior to data collection to familiarize the participant with the task. The manual of the protocol was recorded by the smartphone app developing team for consistency and shared the protocols with partner teams.

Fig 1. Smartphone app.

Fig 1

(A) Placement of the phone on the thigh and the illustration of stepping movement. (B) Representative time series of the thigh flexion angle in the sagittal plane during the stepping in place task. (C) Study design and dependent variables extracted from the smartphone app.

For the present study, participants at the civilian sites performed 3 trials per condition for the EO, EC, and HS conditions (i.e., 60 seconds per trial x 3 trials x 3 conditions). Due to time constraints, participants at one military site performed only 2 trials of all three conditions (i.e., 60 seconds per trial x 2 trials x 3 conditions), and participants at the other military site performed 2 trials per condition only the EC and HS conditions (i.e., 60 second per trial x 2 trials x 2 conditions). In both sites, all participants completed the task in the order of EO, EC, HS for civilians and EC, HS for service members, with brief standing rest between trials (20–30 seconds). To compare common data that were collected at all sites, the mean of all dependent variables for the first 2 trials (instead of all 3 trials) were calculated for the EC and HS conditions, which were then submitted for statistical analysis. This is congruent with previous work showing acceptable concurrent validity when averaging 2 trials in our protocol [21].

Data collection and dependent variables

With the phone placed on the lateral aspect of the thigh, the thigh flexion angle in the sagittal plane was quantified over time, with 0 degrees representing the leg perpendicular to the ground plane (neutral standing position), positive angles representing thigh flexion angles beyond neutral, and negative angles representing thigh extension angles beyond neutral (Fig 1B). This thigh angle time series data were obtained from the Android smartphone’s (Moto X2nd Generation, version 5.1 or Google Pixel, version 8.1.0 or 9) sensor fusion from the accelerometer, gyroscope, and magnetometer, which were recorded at 100.86Hz as described in our previous work [21]. The extracted thigh angle data from the phone were processed with a customized MATLAB script (R2020a, MathWorks Inc., Natick, MA). The obtained data were evenly distributed to 100Hz to consider sampling lags with spline interpolation. Then, the data were filtered with the 4th order low-pass Butterworth filter (both directions) with a cutoff at 5Hz. Non-normal trials (e.g., the phone slipped from the original position; participants stopped or paused during trials; data with noise) were visually detected. For all trials, the first 10 seconds from each trial, during the metronome tones were provided, were removed, resulting in the analysis of 60 s duration for each trial.

Our previous work showed which spatial and temporal variables derived from the thigh angle time series are reliable and valid [21]. For the present study, we report seven variables derived from the app (Fig 1C): four that describe general performance kinematics and three that describe movement variability. The general performance kinematics variables include (1) the mean of maximum angular velocity (MaxVel), (2) the mean of peak flexion angle (PeakFlex), (3) the mean of knee range of motion (ROM), and (4) the mean of stride time. The first three variables represent spatial characteristics (i.e., primary variables) and the last variable represents the temporal characteristics (i.e., confirmation purpose). For MaxVel and PeakFlex, maximum values (peaks) of the knee angular velocity and knee angle, respectively, were obtained from each stride across one trial. The mean of these peaks was calculated. For the knee extension angle used in the ROM calculation, the obtained thigh angle data were partitioned into 100 bins and plotted into a histogram. Then, the most frequent bin was selected since the longest phase of a step-in-place task is the single-leg stance (i.e., when the knee is extended). The maximum value from this bin was defined as the knee extension angle. ROM was found as the mean difference between the peak flexion and knee extension. Stride time was determined by quantifying the intervals in seconds between maximal thigh flexion events. Stride time was analyzed to examine whether participants complied with the temporal restriction prescribed during the first 10 seconds of the task.

The variables that quantify movement variability are (1) SD MaxVel, (2) CV Peak Flex, and (3) CV Stride Time. To calculate SD MaxVel, the SD of MaxVel was extracted for each individual. Then, the mean and dispersion of the MaxVel SDs were calculated within each group. Thus, SD MaxVel represents the dispersion of SD (i.e., variability across individuals). For CV Peak Flex and CV Stride Time, CV was obtained by SD divided by its corresponding mean.

During the dependent variable extraction, non-normal trials were removed. Trials were considered as errors and removed if the phone slipped from the strap, participants did not comply with the metronome pace (e.g., participants stopped or paused during trials), or ROM was less than 10 degrees.

Statistical design

For each dependent variable, a linear mixed-effect model analysis was adopted. The original model specification was three fixed effects and one random effect. The condition (EC, HS), group (Healthy, Concussion History), and the interaction between condition and group were fixed effects and the individual intercept variance was the random effect (Fig 2B). The model specification process was predetermined as follows: (a) the original model was tested, if necessary, with a model with different variance structures; (b) the condition x group interaction term was omitted if the interaction term was not significant; and (c) the final model was determined based on the results of model comparison with Likelihood Ratio Test and AIC (Akaike Information Criterion) (Fig 2A). A model with parsimonious parameters was chosen if the two models were not statistically different. Since the task used in the present study constrained the temporal characteristics (i.e., stride time) and measured spatial characteristics, Stride Time was fitted first. The healthy group was a reference for the group variable, and the EC condition was a reference for the condition variable. The results were interpreted based on the output of the final model, with the coefficient value representing the magnitude change between the conditions or groups. Alpha was set at 0.05.

Fig 2. Data processing and model specification flow chart.

Fig 2

Abbreviations: HS = head shake, EC = Eyes-closed conditions. (A) Flow chart of the data processing. The two boxes in the bottom are the sample size submitted for the statistical analyses for all variables except CV Stride time. *1 n = the number of subjects, nt = the number of trials. *2 for CV stride time, n = 138 healthy and n = 61 concussed participants for the EC condition and n = 141 healthy and n = 60 concussed participants for the HS condition were submitted for analyses. (B) the model specification process: Fixed effects coefficients are B0, B1, B2, and B3, j = j-th group of i-th individual. u0i represents the random effect of the individual intercept, and e0i represents the residuals, where both are assumed to be normally distributed.

Results

Participant demographics and time since concussion are presented in Table 1. No difference in height (t160 = 0.320, p = 0.749) and weight (t160 = -1.812, p = 0.072) were observed between healthy and concussion history groups.

During data processing, a total of 12 participants from the healthy group and one participant from the concussion history group were removed (Fig 2A). Nine of these participants were from the same cohort (i.e., healthy service members). Further, additional four trials were removed for CV Stride time as outliers. Following the model specification for the Stride Time variable, three additional outliers were identified, which were likely due to the incompliance with the stepping pace. Thus, these trials were removed. This resulted in n = 141 healthy and n = 61 concussion history participants for the EC condition, n = 140 healthy, and n = 61 concussion history participants for the HS condition (four additional trials removed for CV Stride Time) (Fig 2A). These remaining data were submitted for statistical analyses.

General performance kinematics

The results of the final model are for the general performance kinematics are summarized in Table 2. As described above, we identified outliers that were likely due to incompliance with the stepping pace after the diagnosis of residuals of the final model. Thus, the model specification process was repeated without these outliers. First, homogeneity of variance between healthy and concussion history participants was potentially different from visual inspection. However, a model with a heterogeneous variance structure of the residuals was not statistically different from the original model with a homogeneous variance structure [X2(1) = 2.592, p = 0.107]. Next, the group and condition interaction of the fixed effect was omitted in the final model since it was not significant [t = 1.001, p = 0.318]. The results of the final model (Table 2) showed no difference between conditions (p = 0.090) nor group (p = 0.087). The estimated means of the stride time were 1.15 s for the healthy group and 1.13 s for the concussion history group after controlling for the condition. This result suggests that the step interval of both groups approximated the target pace (i.e., 1.15 s), and the following spatial and variability dependent variables were not confounded by differences in the imposed temporal characteristics.

Table 2. Final model and statistical results of each general performance kinematic dependent variable.

Stride Time
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 1.1503 0.006 200 208.716 < 0.001 StrideTimeji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition -0.005 0.003 200 -1.703 0.0901
Group -0.017 0.001 200 -1.718 0.0873
Mean Maximum Knee Flexion Velocity (Max Vel) in deg/s
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 170.314 3.309 200 51.476 < 0.001 MaxVelji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition -5.944 1.565 200 -3.798 0.0002
Group 8.301 5.847 200 1.421 0.157
Mean Peak Knee Flexion Angle (Peak Flex) in deg
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 41.663 0.842 200 49.472 < 0.001 PeakFlexji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition -1.639 0.379 200 -4.326 < 0.001
Group 0.810 1.492 200 0.543 0.588
Mean of Range of Motion (ROM) in deg
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 37.782 0.782 200 48.323 < 0.001 ROMji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition -1.502 0.381 200 -3.941 0.0001
Group 1.261 1.379 200 0.914 0.362

For other variables, the model specification process was similar to Stride time. Specifically, visual inspection showed an indication of different variances by group, but they were all non-significant, and no interaction was not found. Thus, the final model had two fixed effects (group and condition) and one random intercept. Therefore, the main effects of the final model were reported for other variables. For variability measures, we did not identify an indication of heterogeneous variance. Thus, after fitting the original model, it was compared with a model without an interaction term to determine the final model.

For the mean of maximum knee velocity (MaxVel), there was no main effect of group (p = 0.157), but the main effect was found in condition (p < 0.001), suggesting that the change from the EC condition to the HS condition was associated with 5.944 (m/s) decrease in MaxVel (p < 0.001) (Fig 3).

Fig 3. Estimated marginal means of SD knee maximum angular velocity.

Fig 3

EC = Eyes-closed, HS = Head Shake conditions; Bars = Standard error of the mean; the values are estimated marginal means from the final model.

For the mean of maximum knee flexion (PeakFlex), the results showed no significant effect of group (p = 0.588) and a significant main effect of condition, suggesting that the HS condition was associated with a 1.639-degrees decrease in the knee flexion angle relative to the EC condition (p < 0.001).

For the mean range of motion (ROM), similarly, there was no significant effect of group (p = 0.362) and a significant main effect of condition, suggesting that the HS condition was associated with a 1.502-degrees decrease in ROM relative to the EC condition (p < 0.001).

Movement variability variables

The results of the final model are for the movement variability variables are summarized in Table 3. For the variability (SD) of MaxVel (SD_MaxVel), the main effects were found in both condition (p = 0.031) and group (p < 0.001) (Table 2). The change in the HS condition to the EC condition was associated with an increase in variability by 0.400. The concussion history group was associated with a 2.001-point variability increase after controlling for the condition.

Table 3. Final model and statistical results of each movement variability dependent variable.

Variability (SD) of Maximum Velocity (SD_MaxVel)
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 12.110 0.283 200 42.828 < 0.001 SD_MaxVelji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition 0.400 0.185 200 2.167 0.0314
Group 2.001 0.486 200 4.120 < 0. 001
Variability (CV) of Peak knee flexion (CV_PeakFlex)
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 6.432 0.189 199 34.091 < 0.001 CVPeakFlexji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition 0.370 0.135 199 2.738 0.0067
Group 0.268 0.319 199 0.839 0.4023
Variability (CV) of Stride time (CV_Stride Time)
Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model
Intercept 2.510 0.085 200 29.356 < 0.001 CV_StrideTimeji=β0+B1*Conji+B2*Grpji++u0i+εji
Condition 0.177 0.083 197 2.128 0.0346
Group 0.241 0.135 200 1.785 0.0758

For the variability (CV) of Peak Knee Flexion (CV_PeakFlex), the results of one participant were extreme scores. We were confident that it was a processing error (i.e., the extreme score was 34.92 compared to the estimated mean of 6.43 with SE = 0.189). Thus, this participant was removed. Our results showed no main effect of group (p = 0.402) with a main effect of condition (p = 0.007). The HS condition was associated with a 0.370-point variability increase compared to the EC condition.

For the variability (CV) of Stride Time (CV_Stride Time), the main effect was found in condition (p = 0.035) with no difference in group (p = 0.076). The HS condition was associated with an increase of variability by 0.177 points.

Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to examine the extent to which the neuromotor behavior data obtained from our smartphone app could associate with a history of concussion. Aligned with our hypothesis, we showed that movement variability (via SD Max Vel) was significantly higher in the EC and HS conditions for participants with a concussion compared to participants without a concussion. We discuss our findings in the context of previous balance control tests for concussion assessment and using the optimal movement variability hypothesis to frame our observations.

Previous work has shown that neuromotor dysfunction occurs after a concussion [31, 38, 39]. Specifically, balance control becomes more variable due to neuromotor sensory integration challenges [32]. This is thought to result from the neurometabolic cascade that happens after a significant head impact [40], leading to challenges downstream in controlling motor actions [5]. For this reason, balance tasks have a long history of being used in concussion assessments [41]. Our findings align with previous work showing that the control of movement can become more variable after a concussion [31, 32, 4244]. More specifically, our participants with a concussion exhibited a higher standard deviation of the maximum angular velocity of their thigh during the stepping-in-place task, indicating the inability to maintain consistent motion, even though they were cued by a metronome at the beginning of the task (utilizing the synchronization-continuation paradigm). Previous studies suggested that metrics including velocity information are more sensitive to gait impairments in post-stroke patients than joint angles and timing parameters alone [45]. Our findings are consistent with this observation and extend it by adopting the paradigm in a manner suitable for field-testing.

The impetus for this project was the goal of creating an objective, cost-effective, and portable way to measure neuromotor control in individuals with a suspected concussion. Our solution for that challenge was to develop a custom smartphone app and a specific testing protocol. It should be noted that smartphone apps have been previously developed to meet this challenge, but many of these apps have not been rigorously and scientifically tested [46, 47]. An app that has been tested and is commercially available is the Sway Balance app (Sway Medical, LLC, Tulsa, OK), which has been shown to be valid [14], reliable [12], and have clinical utility [13]. Another empirically-based portable balance test is the BTrackS [8, 48, 49], which is an objective and relatively cost-effective solution to measure neuromotor performance [8]. In the context of concussion assessment, BTrackS was shown to have clinically acceptable sensitivity to detect dysfunctional balance performance [16]. In that study, 16 out of 25 participants with a concussion were observed to have increased variability in their balance control, an observation that aligns with our current findings. While the Sway Balance app and BTrackS have both demonstrated clinical utility, both assesses balance control with a static task, which may mask neurosensory dysfunction relative to a more challenging dynamic task [17, 18]. Thus, our dynamic balance protocol was developed to overcome this potential limitation in mind. However, it should be noted that the current paper does not address the static versus dynamic balance comparison. Nevertheless, our data do show that individuals with a concussion exhibited elevated variability in the dynamic balance control relative to non-concussed individuals, adding important insight to our knowledge base on ways that a concussion may affect movement patterns.

The observation that an increase in variability (SD maximum angular velocity) was exhibited in our participants with a history of concussion relative to our healthy participants aligns with the optimal movement variability hypothesis (OMVH) [22]. OMVH builds upon decades of research that show variability patterns in a biological system are informative about its health and functional capacity [2230]. The original work in this area focused on a unidirectional change in the variability pattern—from a complex signal toward a less complex signal—in the presence of aging and disease [29]. This was articulated as the loss of complexity hypothesis, which was rooted in the premise that a healthy biological system exhibits a certain level of variability, which could be described mathematically as a more complex signal (commonly via the use of entropy metrics). When changes to the biological system occur due to aging or disease (e.g., reduced neural firing rate, reduced reaction time, reduced aerobic and anerobic capacity), a less variable (and less complex) signal was commonly observed. This framework originated in cardiovascular dynamics research [28, 29, 50, 51], but has since been extended to study human movement [22, 23, 27, 5257]. Moreover, this framework has been extended to include a bi-directional movement of variability, which is the premise of the OMVH. That is, a continuum of variability is considered, with the “optimal” variability residing in the middle of the continuum. A shift away from the middle—toward less complex variability on one side or overly complex variability on the other side—represents a non-optimal variability pattern reflecting reduced functional capacity. In the context of this study and the dynamic balance task, the variability exhibited by the non-concussed individuals would be considered “optimal”. The individuals with a concussion history exhibited elevated variability in their movement pattern relative to the non-concussed individuals. This observation aligns with the increased variability that was observed in the BESS [10] and in the SOT [38] after a concussion. Our findings also align with Purkayastha et al. [31], where they showed an increase in anterior-posterior variability during a static balance task in participants with a concussion relative to controls. While our study was not a within-subjects tracking study where data before and after the concussive event were available, it would be hypothesized that the individuals with a concussion likely shifted from the optimal zone of variability toward the increased variability on the continuum. While outside the scope of this study, a prospective tracking study could confirm this shift, as well as the potential shift back toward the optimal variability zone after the concussion effects reside. Such a study design would help provide more contextual information when using the OMVH as the framework.

It should be noted that temporal dynamics have significantly contributed to our knowledge of the optimal movement variability hypothesis. Under this framework, temporal dynamics are typically examined via a metric(s) that quantify the structure of the variability (i.e., sample entropy). However, a limitation of this approach is that enough data points (and more importantly, cyclic events) must be present in order for repeating patterns to naturally emerge. Short datasets typically do not allow for an emergence of such repeating patterns, and thus are not good candidates for structure of variability metrics. Our task was confined to 60 seconds of stepping-in-place per trial, leading to ~52 steps on average. While nonlinear dynamics could be run on such a short dataset, it would open up questions about validity and reliability of such an approach. Thus, we elected to focus on magnitude of variability metrics (i.e., coefficient of variation), which is more appropriate to quantify variability in short time series.

While the current study was a retrospective design that tested participants with a concussion history, it is important to note that work has been done to predict the risk of motion sickness after head trauma [5860]. This approach aligns with the general approach to identify risk factors for a concussion [61], which has led to questions related to what strategies can be used to modify the sport and/or player care to reduce concussion risk [62]. As these approaches advance, it is plausible that such predictive analytics could be part of a smartphone app similar to the one tested in this paper, which could enhance athlete care.

There are several limitations of this study to acknowledge. First, although the inclusion criteria required the concussive event to have occurred no longer than six months prior to our testing, the number of weeks since the event varied substantially in our population. Concussion symptoms can dissipate for many people 7–10 days after the event, so future work should explore the factor of time-since-injury as part of their study design. Prior to the main analysis, we had run a correlation analysis within the concussion history group between the dependent variable and time from concussion (n = 54; time from concussion was missing for n = 8). The results of the Spearman correlation did not show a significant association (rho = - 0.013, p = 0.888) (Fig 4). Thus, we did not include this variable in our model. However, it is possible that this characteristic is unique to our cohort. More research is warranted to understand movement variability and the time from concussion. Second, the testing environment differed between our groups (civilians in a university laboratory, Service Members with a concussion history in a military clinical setting, healthy Service Members on a military training site). While our app and protocol were designed to be deployed to diverse settings, future research should examine how different environmental factors (e.g., performing the task on concrete vs. gravel or in a quiet laboratory vs. noisy field-based setting) may influence the results. Third, although the stepping-in-place task is dynamic, one could argue that it is not functional outside of military context, so future work should focus on the inclusion of more functional tasks to examine dynamic balance control. Fourth, it is unclear if the dynamic measures in this study are more sensitive than existing static assessments, so a head-to-head study comparing static and dynamic balance assessments after a concussion is warranted in the future. Fifth, we acknowledge that sex can moderate the control of balance [63, 64], as well as the effect of a concussion on neuromotor control [65]. Unfortunately, sex was not reported for our entire dataset, so we were not able to include this variable in our statistical analyses. Thus, we recommend future work makes it a priority to include sex as a biological variable, congruent with current NIH policy [66]. Sixth, the observation that SD MaxVel was sensitive to neuromotor changes, but CV PeakFlex and CV Stride Time were not (although generally trending in the same direction as SD MaxVel) needs to be further examined. Understanding this etiology will be key for future research. Lastly, the participants in this study could be generally characterized and young, fit, and physically active. Thus, these results may not generalize to populations outside of this characterization.

Fig 4. Association between time from concussion and SD Max Vel.

Fig 4

Data showing no association between the number of days since the concussion event and neuromotor performance as assessed with SD Max Vel.

Conclusion

We aimed to explore the extent to which neuromotor performance changes after a concussion could be detected with a custom smartphone app. We showed that increased variability in performance (SD maximum angular velocity of the thigh) during the dynamic balance task was observed in the participants with a concussion relative to the participants without a concussion. These findings are consistent with the optimal movement variability hypothesis that indicates variability characteristics will change if the neuromotor system is disrupted. Collectively, this study shows that neuromotor performance on an objective, cost-effective, and portable dynamic balance assessment is associated with a history of concussion.

Supporting information

S1 File. Raw data.

Dependent variable and demographic data for each participant.

(XLSX)

Data Availability

All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by funding the Office of the Assistant Secretary of Defense for Health Affairs under award no. W81XWH-15-1-0094 to CKR. The sponsor did not play any role in the study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. The opinions or assertions contained herein are the private ones of the authors and are not to be construed as official or reflecting the views of the Department of Defense, the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences or any other agency of the U.S. Government.

References

  • 1.Wiebe DJ, Comstock RD, Nance ML. Concussion research: A public health priority. Injury Prevention. 2011;17(1):69–70. doi: 10.1136/ip.2010.031211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Green SL, Keightley ML, Lobaugh NJ, Dawson DR, Mihailidis A. Changes in working memory performance in youth following concussion. Brain injury. 2018;32(2):182–90. doi: 10.1080/02699052.2017.1358396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Campbell KR, Parrington L, Peterka RJ, Martini DN, Hullar TE, Horak FB, et al. Exploring persistent complaints of imbalance after mTBI: Oculomotor, peripheral vestibular and central sensory integration function. Journal of Vestibular Research. in press. doi: 10.3233/VES-201590 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Guskiewicz KM. Postural stability assessment following concussion: one piece of the puzzle. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine. 2001;11(3):182–9. doi: 10.1097/00042752-200107000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Longhi L, Saatman KE, Fujimoto S, Raghupathi R, Meaney DF, Davis J, et al. Temporal window of vulnerability to repetitive experimental concussive brain injury. Neurosurgery. 2005;56(2):364–74. doi: 10.1227/01.neu.0000149008.73513.44 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Riemann BL, Guskiewicz KM. Effects of mild head injury on postural stability as measured through clinical balance testing. Journal of Athletic Training. 2000;35(1):19–25. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Nashner LM, Peters JF. Dynamic posturography in the diagnosis and management of dizziness and balance disorders. Neurologic clinics. 1990. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Chang JO, Levy SS, Seay SW, Goble DJ. An alternative to the balance error scoring system: Using a low-cost balance board to improve the validity/reliability of sports-related concussion balance testing. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine. 2014;24(3):256–62. doi: 10.1097/JSM.0000000000000016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Kaufman KR, Brey RH, Chou L-S, Rabatin A, Brown AW, Basford JR. Comparison of subjective and objective measurements of balance disorders following traumatic brain injury. Medical Engineering & Physics. 2006;28(3):234–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bell DR, Guskiewicz KM, Clark MA, Padua DA. Systematic review of the balance error scoring system. Sports Health: A Multidisciplinary Approach. 2011;3(3):287–95. doi: 10.1177/1941738111403122 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Finnoff JT, Peterson VJ, Hollman JH, Smith J. Intrarater and interrater reliability of the Balance Error Scoring System (BESS). PM&R. 2009;1(1):50–4. doi: 10.1016/j.pmrj.2008.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Amick RZ, Chaparro A, Patterson JA, Jorgensen MJ. Test-retest reliability of the sway balance mobile application. Journal of Mobile Technology in Medicine. 2015;4(2):40–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Patterson JA, Amick RZ, Pandya PD, Hakansson N, Jorgensen MJ. Comparison of a mobile technology application with the Balance Error Scoring System. International Journal of Athletic Therapy and Training. 2014;19(3):4–7. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Patterson JA, Amick RZ, Thummar T, Rogers ME. Validation of measures from the smartphone sway balance application: a pilot study. International Journal of Sports Physical Therapy. 2014;9(2):135–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Guskiewicz KM, Ross SE, Marshall SW. Postural stability and neuropsychological deficits after concussion in collegiate athletes. Journal of Athletic Training. 2001;36(3):263–73. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Goble DJ, Manyak KA, Abdenour TE, Rauh MJ, Baweja HS. An initial evaluation of the BTrackS balance plate and sports balance software for concussion diagnosis. International Journal of Sports Physical Therapy. 2016;11(2):149–55. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Aloraini SM, Gelley G, Glazebrook C, Sibley KM, Singer J, Passmore S. Motor behavior concepts in the study of balance: A scoping review. Journal of motor behavior. 2020;52(1):97–121. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2019.1582472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fino PC, Parrington L, Pitt W, Martini DN, Chesnutt JC, Chou L-S, et al. Detecting gait abnormalities after concussion or mild traumatic brain injury: A systematic review of single-task, dual-task, and complex gait. Gait & Posture. 2018;62:157–66. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.03.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hrysomallis C, McLaughlin P, Goodman C. Relationship between static and dynamic balance tests among elite Australian Footballers. Journal of science and medicine in sport. 2006;9(4):288–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2006.05.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rhea CK, Kuznetsov NA, Ross SE, Long B, Jakiela JT, Bailie JM, et al. Development of a portable tool for screening neuromotor sequelae from repetitive low-level blast exposure. Military Medicine. 2017;182(3/4):147–54. doi: 10.7205/MILMED-D-16-00140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Kuznetsov NA, Robins RK, Long B, Jakiela JT, Haran FJ, Ross SE, et al. Validity and reliability of smartphone orientation measurement to quantify dynamic balance function. Physiological Measurement. 2018;39:02NT1. doi: 10.1088/1361-6579/aaa3c2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Stergiou N, Harbourne RT, Cavanaugh JT. Optimal movement variability: A new theoretical perspective for neurologic physical therapy. Journal of Neurologic Physical Therapy. 2006;30:120–9. doi: 10.1097/01.npt.0000281949.48193.d9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Stergiou N, Decker LM. Human movement variability, nonlinear dynamics, and pathology: Is there a connection? Human Movement Science. 2011;30(5):869–88. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2011.06.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.van Emmerik RE, Ducharme SW, Amado A, Hamill J. Comparing dynamical systems concepts and techniques for biomechanical analysis. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2016;5(1):3–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2016.01.013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.West BJ. The Fractal Tapestry of Life: A Review of Fractal Physiology. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences. 2021;25(3):261–96. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Morrison S, Newell K. Dimension and complexity in human movement and posture. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences. 2015;19(4):395–418. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Vaillancourt DE, Newell K. Changing complexity in human behavior and physiology through aging and disease. Neurobiology of Aging 2002;23(1):1–11. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00247-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Lipsitz LA. Dynamics of stability: The physiologic basis of functional health and frailty. Journal of Gerontology: Biological Sciences. 2002;57A(3):B115–B25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL. Loss of ’complexity’ and aging: potential applications of fractals and chaos theory to senescence. JAMA: The Journal of the American Medicine Association. 1992;267(13):1806–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.da Costa CSN, Batistão MV, Rocha NACF. Quality and structure of variability in children during motor development: A systematic review. Research in developmental disabilities. 2013;34(9):2810–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Purkayastha S, Adair H, Woodruff A, Ryan LJ, Williams B, James E, et al. Balance testing following concussion: postural sway versus complexity index. PM&R. 2019;11(11):1184–92. doi: 10.1002/pmrj.12129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Buckley TA, Oldham JR, Caccese JB. Postural control deficits identify lingering post-concussion neurological deficits. Journal of Sport and Health Science. 2016;5(1):61–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2016.01.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wing AM, Kristofferson AB. Response delays and the timing of discrete motor responses. Perception & Psychophysics. 1973;14(1):5–12. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Repp BH, Su Y-H. Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2013;20(3):403–52. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Mioni G, Grondin S, Stablum F. Temporal dysfunction in traumatic brain injury patients: primary or secondary impairment? Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:269. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00269 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Breck J, Bohr A, Poddar S, McQueen MB, Casault T. Characteristics and incidence of concussion among a US collegiate undergraduate population. JAMA network open. 2019;2(12):e1917626–e. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2019.17626 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Helmick KM, Spells CA, Malik SZ, Davies CA, Marion DW, Hinds SR. Traumatic brain injury in the US military: Epidemiology and key clinical and research programs. Brain Imaging and Behavior. 2015;9(3):358–66. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9399-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Sosnoff JJ, Broglio SP, Shin S, Ferrara MS. Previous mild traumatic brain injury and postural-control dynamics. Journal of Athletic Training. 2011;46(1):85–91. doi: 10.4085/1062-6050-46.1.85 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wright WG, Handy JD, Avcu P, Ortiz A, Haran FJ, Doria M, et al. Healthy active duty military with lifetime experience of mild traumatic brain injury exhibits subtle deficits in sensory reactivity and sensory integration during static balance. Military medicine. 2018;183(suppl_1):313–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Giza CC, Hovda DA. The new neurometabolic cascade of concussion. Neurosurgery. 2014;75(suppl_4):S24–S33. doi: 10.1227/NEU.0000000000000505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Guskiewicz KM. Balance assessment in the management of sport-related concussion. Clinics in Sports Medicine. 2011;30(1):89–102. doi: 10.1016/j.csm.2010.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cavanaugh JT, Guskiewicz KM, Giuliani C, Marshall SW, Mercer VS, Stergiou N. Recovery of postural control after cerebral concussion: New insights using approximate entropy. Journal of Athletic Training. 2006;41(3):305–13. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Gao J, Hu J, Buckley T, White K, Hass C. Shannon and Renyi entropies to classify effects of mild traumatic brain injury on postural sway. PLOS ONE. 2011;6(9):e24446. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0024446 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Cavanaugh JT, Guskiewicz KM, Giuliani C, Marshall SW, Mercer VS, Stergiou N. Detecting altered postural control after cerebral concussion in athletes with normal postural stability. British Journal of Sports Medicine. 2005;39(11):805–11. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2004.015909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Richards J, Pramanik A, Sykesand L, Pomeroy V. A comparison of knee kinematic characteristics of stroke patients and age-matched healthy volunteers. Clinical rehabilitation. 2003;17(5):565–71. doi: 10.1191/0269215503cr651oa [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Roeing KL, Hsieh KL, Sosnoff JJ. A systematic review of balance and fall risk assessments with mobile phone technology. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics. 2017;73:222–6. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2017.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Abou L, Peters J, Wong E, Akers R, Dossou MS, Sosnoff JJ, et al. Gait and Balance Assessments using Smartphone Applications in Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Journal of Medical Systems. 2021;45(9):1–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Goble DJ, Khan E, Baweja HS, O’Connor SM. A point of application study to determine the accuracy, precision and reliability of a low-cost balance plate for center of pressure measurement. Journal of Biomechanics. 2018;71:277–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.01.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.O’Connor SM, Baweja HS, Goble DJ. Validating the BTrackS Balance Plate as a low cost alternative for the measurement of sway-induced center of pressure. Journal of Biomechanics. 2016;49(16):4142–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2016.10.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Goldberger AL, Peng CK, Lipsitz LA. What is physiologic complexity and how does it change with aging and disease? Neurobiology of Aging 2002;23(1):23–6. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(01)00266-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Kaplan DT, Furman MI, Pincus SM, Ryan SM, Lipsitz LA, Goldberger AL. Aging and the complexity of cardiovascular dynamics. Biophysical Journal. 1991;59:945–9. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(91)82309-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Costa M, Priplata A, Lipsitz L, Wu Z, Huang N, Goldberger AL, et al. Noise and poise: Enhancement of postural complexity in the elderly with a stochastic-resonance–based therapy. Europhysics Letters. 2007;77(6):68008. doi: 10.1209/0295-5075/77/68008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Kang HG, Costa MD, Priplata AA, Starobinets OV, Goldberger AL, Peng C-K, et al. Frailty and the degradation of complex balance dynamics during a dual-task protocol. The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences. 2009;64(12):1304–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Manor B, Costa MD, Hu K, Newton E, Starobinets O, Kang HG, et al. Physiological complexity and system adaptability: evidence from postural control dynamics of older adults. Journal of Applied Physiology. 2010;109(6):1786–91. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00390.2010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Manor B, Lipsitz LA. Physiologic complexity and aging: Implications for physical function and rehabilitation. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 2013;45(0):287–93. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.08.020 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Wayne PM, Gow BJ, Costa MD, Peng C-K, Lipsitz LA, Hausdorff JM, et al. Complexity-based measures inform effects of Tai Chi training on standing postural control: cross-sectional and randomized trial studies. PLOS ONE. 2014;9(12):e114731. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0114731 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Zhou J, Habtemariam D, Iloputaife I, Lipsitz LA, Manor B. The complexity of standing postural sway associates with future falls in community-dwelling older adults: The MOBILIZE Boston study. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:2924. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03422-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Chen Y-C, Hung T-H, Tseng T-C, Hsieh CC, Chen F-C, Stoffregen TA. Pre-bout standing body sway differs between adult boxers who do and do not report post-bout motion sickness. 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Chen Y-C, Hung T-H, Tseng T-C, Stoffregen TA. Postural Precursors of Postboxing Motion Sickness in a Manual Aiming Task. Ecological Psychology. 2015;27(1):26–42. [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Chen Y-C, Tseng T-C, Hung T-H, Stoffregen TA. Precursors of post-bout motion sickness in adolescent female boxers. Experimental brain research. 2014;232(8):2571–9. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3910-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Abrahams S, Mc Fie S, Patricios J, Posthumus M, September AV. Risk factors for sports concussion: an evidence-based systematic review. British journal of sports medicine. 2014;48(2):91–7. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2013-092734 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Emery CA, Black AM, Kolstad A, Martinez G, Nettel-Aguirre A, Engebretsen L, et al. What strategies can be used to effectively reduce the risk of concussion in sport? A systematic review. British journal of sports medicine. 2017;51(12):978–84. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-097452 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Kim JW, Eom GM, Kim CS, Kim DH, Lee JH, Park BK, et al. Sex differences in the postural sway characteristics of young and elderly subjects during quiet natural standing. Geriatrics & gerontology international. 2010;10(2):191–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1447-0594.2009.00582.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rhea CK, Schleich KN, Washington L, Glass SM, Ross SE, Etnier JL, et al. Neuromotor and neurocognitive performance in female American football players. Athletic Training & Sports Health Care. 2019;11(5):224–33. [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Covassin T, Elbin R, Harris W, Parker T, Kontos A. The role of age and sex in symptoms, neurocognitive performance, and postural stability in athletes after concussion. The American Journal of Sports Medicine. 2012;40(6):1303–12. doi: 10.1177/0363546512444554 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Arnegard ME, Whitten LA, Hunter C, Clayton JA. Sex as a biological variable: a 5-year progress report and call to action. Journal of Women’s Health. 2020;29(6):858–64. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2019.8247 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Thomas A Stoffregen

14 Apr 2022

PONE-D-22-03586Neuromotor changes after a concussion can be detected with a custom smartphone appPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Rhea,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Both Reviewers see merit in the work, but note significant problems with the submitted manuscript. Reviewer 2 recommends rejection, but I feel it may be possible to address the concerns raised in his/her review in a substantial revision. In revising, please pay careful attention to the issues raised by each of the Reviewers.

Please submit your revised manuscript by May 29 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Thomas A Stoffregen, PhD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf  and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please amend your current ethics statement to address the following concerns:

a) Did participants provide their written or verbal informed consent to participate in this study?

b) If consent was verbal, please explain i) why written consent was not obtained, ii) how you documented participant consent, and iii) whether the ethics committees/IRB approved this consent procedure

3. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

   a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

   b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide."

4. We note that Figure 1 includes an image of a participant  in the study.

As per the PLOS ONE policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-human-subjects-research) on papers that include identifying, or potentially identifying, information, the individual(s) or parent(s)/guardian(s) must be informed of the terms of the PLOS open-access (CC-BY) license and provide specific permission for publication of these details under the terms of this license. Please download the Consent Form for Publication in a PLOS Journal (http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=8ce6/plos-consent-form-english.pdf). The signed consent form should not be submitted with the manuscript, but should be securely filed in the individual's case notes. Please amend the methods section and ethics statement of the manuscript to explicitly state that the patient/participant has provided consent for publication: “The individual in this manuscript has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details”.

If you are unable to obtain consent from the subject of the photograph, you will need to remove the figure and any other textual identifying information or case descriptions for this individual

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors used a smartphone to measure the accelerative consequences of a stepping in place task that was applied to a large sample of adults, with vs. without recent concussion. One parameter of measured movement differed between groups. The authors propose their method as a cheap, effective way to obtain objective (i.e., non-observational) data on the movement consequences of concussion. The idea is excellent and the approach (generally) is good. With some important changes in analysis, the value of the study can be greatly increased.

It is necessary to report the numbers of women and men in your sample. It also is relevant: Posture and movement differ by sex (e.g., Kim et al., 2010), and aspects of concussion differ by sex (e.g., Alsalaheen et al., 2019). In revising, please conduct new analyses in which sex is an independent variable.

Much of the recent research relating concussion to quantitative measures of sway has focused on measures of temporal dynamics; something that is prominent in formulation and testing of the optimal movement variability hypothesis. The data obtained from the cell phones might be analyzed in any of a number of ways. In revising, please either 1) compute and include some measure of temporal dynamics or 2) provide an explicit motivation for not using the available data to assess temporal dynamics.

I understand that ANOVA was not appropriate, but the reversion to t-tests weakens the analysis. You can do better. Please consider conducting new analyses using mixed methods analysis (aka mixed effects modelling), which (in effect) is a form of ANOVA that is robust to the sorts of issues that exist in your data set (Brown & Prescott, 2006; Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998; Putt & Chinchilli, 1999).

The quantitative kinematics of the body are changed by concussion. However, recent evidence suggests that the quantitative kinematics of the body also may predict the risk of concussion in individuals (Chen et al., 2012, 2014, 2015). It would be helpful, in your Discussion, to address these effects. Might your cell phone approach be useful in a predictive context?

Alsalaheen, Johns, Bean, Almeida, Eckner, & Lorincz (2019). Women and men use different strategies to stabilize the head in response to impulsive loads: Implications for concussion injury risk. Journal of Orthopaedic & Sports Physical Therapy, 49, 779-786.

Brown H, Prescott R. Applied mixed models in medicine. New York: Wiley; 2006:215–44.

Chen, Y.-C., Hung, T.-H., Tseng, T.-C., Hsieh, C. C., Chen, F.-C., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2012). Pre-bout standing body sway differs between adult boxers who do and do not report post-bout motion sickness. PLOS ONE, 7(10): e46136. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046136.

Chen, Y.-C., Hung, T.-H., Tseng, T.-C., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2015). Postural precursors of post-boxing motion sickness in a manual aiming task. Ecological Psychology, 27, 26-42.

Chen, Y.-C., Tseng, T.-C., Hung, T.-H., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2014). Precursors of post-bout motion sickness in adolescent female boxers. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 2571-2579. DOI 10.1007/s00221-014-3910-4

Kim JW, Eom GM, Kim CS, Kim DH, Lee JH, Park BK, Hong J (2010) Sex differences in the postural sway characteristics of young and elderly subjects during quiet natural standing. Geriat Geront Int 10:191–198

Kreft, I. G., de Leeuw, J. Introducing multilevel modeling. NewYork: Sage Publications; 1998.

Putt M, Chinchilli VM. A mixed effects model for the analysis of repeated measures cross-over studies. Stat Med 1999; 18: 3037–58.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript. I commend the author for considering the need for novel and affordable methodologies which seek to identify neuromotor alterations in civilian and military individuals with a history of concussion. While the methodologies of the study are novel, the prepared manuscript is absent of pertinent information and critical data that are necessary for publication consideration. Thus, I recommend rejection for publication in PLOS ONE. Below I have summarized major concerns to support the authors with the next phases of their work.

1) The authors should consider reframing their manuscript to align with current literature regarding history of concussion. The authors refer to their participants as ‘concussed,’ which does not appear to be accurate given the length of time since last reported concussion. The participants’ current symptom status and whether they have been cleared to return to sport (or duty) should be included for reader interpretation. Likewise, I recommend authors include more detail on how history of concussion was quantified (e.g., was the ‘definition of concussion’ provided when asking participants to self-report? What about patients with a history of symptoms but no medical diagnosis?)

2) More clarification regarding the phone app as being an ‘objective outcome.’ While the smartphone app is objective, they are discriminating against a highly subjective diagnosis (concussion diagnosis varies greatly across medical professionals – was the criteria standardized?).

3) Clearer presentation of inclusion and exclusion criteria and demographics more generally (e.g., limb dominance, age, and entire group missing demographics). For instance, the inclusion of individuals with an ambulatory device or prosthetic would present a separate group of individuals with differences in gait or neuromotor control.

4) Lack of clarity in analyses. Specifically, the total n used in analyses differed without explanation (e.g., 154 healthy in table 1, but that number is not the n in any table 2 analysis).

5) While I appreciate the consideration of the OMVH, the data presented does not fully support the authors’ discussion points. The authors did not perform any nonlinear (e.g., complexity) analyses. Likewise, the optimal movement variability for healthy controls does not seem to be established in the literature.

In summary, while we do think the novelty of devices to measure neuromotor changes in the field are important to gain a greater understanding of individual deficits following concussive injury, the concerns stated above and other minor issues within the manuscript do not meet the current standards for publication in PLOS ONE.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2022 Dec 15;17(12):e0278994. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0278994.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


3 Jun 2022

The authors would like to thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments, as well as thanking the editor for the opportunity to resubmit our paper for publication consideration in PLOS ONE. We believe the reviewers raised some significant and high-quality questions. We have attended to all of the reviewer’s comments/suggestions in the manuscript and/or in this document and have provided a point-by-point response to all comments/suggestions below. The reviewer’s comments are in bold, our responses are in normal typeface, and the text that has been added to the manuscript is in red (please see separate Response to Reviewers document).

REVIEWER #1 COMMENTS

The authors used a smartphone to measure the accelerative consequences of a stepping in place task that was applied to a large sample of adults, with vs. without recent concussion. One parameter of measured movement differed between groups. The authors propose their method as a cheap, effective way to obtain objective (i.e., non-observational) data on the movement consequences of concussion. The idea is excellent and the approach (generally) is good. With some important changes in analysis, the value of the study can be greatly increased.

Thank you for your encouraging comments. We have heeded your and the other reviewer’s comments to strengthen the manuscript.

It is necessary to report the numbers of women and men in your sample. It also is relevant: Posture and movement differ by sex (e.g., Kim et al., 2010), and aspects of concussion differ by sex (e.g., Alsalaheen et al., 2019). In revising, please conduct new analyses in which sex is an independent variable.

We appreciate your comment, especially since we have focused our related work on the very sex differences you mention (Rhea et al., 2019). Unfortunately, we do not have sex data for all our participants. The reason for this is a large portion of our sample (all health Service Members, N=54) were collected as part of a larger previous study at a Department of Defense site and sex was not recorded as part of that dataset. Nevertheless, we agree that it is important to report sex for the participants for whom we have those data, so we have added the following text to lines 122-126:

“The demographics of N = 54 healthy Service Members were not obtained. Sex of the participants for whom demographics were obtained are as follows: (1) for the 100 healthy civilians, 51 were female and 49 were male, (2) for the 44 civilians with a concussion history, 26 were female and 18 were male, (3) and for the 18 Service Members with a concussion history, 4 were female and 14 were male.”

Since we do not have sex for all of our participants, we believe that re-running the analyses with sex as an independent variable would provide an impartial picture. Sex differences in the healthy civilians wouldn’t be that interesting, as it is the sex differences after a concussion that researchers typically focus on. With our relatively small and unbalanced concussed sample that would need to be further separated between Service Members and civilians, it is likely such sex-dependent analyses would be underpowered. A key feature of our study as presented is the large sample size (N=216 participants) and focusing on an analysis that splinters the groups to such small sub-samples would likely detract from this manuscript. However, we agree that your suggestion warrants serious consideration for future work. Accordingly, we have added the following text to lines 415-419:

“Fifth, we acknowledge that sex can moderate the control of balance [63, 64], as well as the effect of a concussion on neuromotor control [65]. Unfortunately, sex was not reported for our entire dataset, so we were not able to include this variable in our statistical analyses. Thus, we recommend future work makes it a priority to include sex as a biological variable, congruent with current NIH policy [66].”

Rhea, C.K., Schleich, K.N., Washington. L., Glass, S.M., Ross, S.E., Etnier, J.L., Wright, W.G., Goble, D.J., & Duffy, D.M. (2019). Neuromotor and neurocognitive performance in female American football players. Athletic Training & Sports Health Care, 11(5), 224-233. https://doi.org/10.3928/19425864-20181101-01

Much of the recent research relating concussion to quantitative measures of sway has focused on measures of temporal dynamics; something that is prominent in formulation and testing of the optimal movement variability hypothesis. The data obtained from the cell phones might be analyzed in any of a number of ways. In revising, please either 1) compute and include some measure of temporal dynamics or 2) provide an explicit motivation for not using the available data to assess temporal dynamics.

Thank you for bringing up this important point. We agree with your comment that temporal dynamics have been shown to be important quantitative measures. We did include temporal metrics (i.e., stride time, variability of maximum velocity, variability of stride time), but we suspect your reference to temporal dynamics refers more to measures that quantify the evolution of the dynamics over time, such as nonlinear and/or complexity analyses (e.g., sample entropy). Due to our stepping-in-place task being confined to 60 seconds, such nonlinear analyses are not good candidates for our dataset. To acknowledge our data limitation, but with a nod to your point, we have added the following text to lines 385-394:

“It should be noted that temporal dynamics have significantly contributed to our knowledge of the optimal movement variability hypothesis. Under this framework, temporal dynamics are typically examined via a metric(s) that quantify the structure of the variability (i.e., sample entropy). However, a limitation of this approach is that enough data points (and more importantly, cyclic events) must be present in order for repeating patterns to naturally emerge. Short datasets typically do not allow for an emergence of such repeating patterns, and thus are not good candidates for structure of variability metrics. Our task was confined to 60 seconds of stepping-in-place per trial, leading to ~52 steps on average. While nonlinear dynamics could be run on such a short dataset, it would open up questions about validity and reliability of such an approach. Thus, we elected to focus on magnitude of variability metrics (i.e., coefficient of variation), which is more appropriate to quantify variability in short time series.”

I understand that ANOVA was not appropriate, but the reversion to t-tests weakens the analysis. You can do better. Please consider conducting new analyses using mixed methods analysis (aka mixed effects modelling), which (in effect) is a form of ANOVA that is robust to the sorts of issues that exist in your data set (Brown & Prescott, 2006; Kreft & de Leeuw, 1998; Putt & Chinchilli, 1999).

Thank you for this consideration. To address your comment, we reanalyzed our data using a linear mixed effect model. Our revised statistical analysis is reported in lines 226-239:

“For each dependent variable, a linear mixed-effect model analysis was adopted. The original model specification was three fixed effects and one random effect. The condition (EC, HS), group (Healthy, Concussed), and the interaction between condition and group were fixed effects and the individual intercept variance was the random effect (Fig 2B). The model specification process was predetermined as follows: (a) the original model was tested, if necessary, with a model with different variance structures; (b) the condition x group interaction term was omitted if the interaction term was not significant; and (c) the final model was determined based on the results of model comparison with Likelihood Ratio Test and AIC (Akaike Information Criterion) (Fig 2A). A model with parsimonious parameters was chosen if the two models were not statistically different. Since the task used in the present study constrained the temporal characteristics (i.e., stride time) and measured spatial characteristics, Stride Time was fitted first. The healthy group was a reference for the group variable, and the EC condition was a reference for the condition variable. The results were interpreted based on the output of the final model, with the coefficient value representing the magnitude change between the conditions or groups. Alpha was set at 0.05.”

Also, the analysis process has been revised and presented in a figure (see Fig 2B below).

Fig 2. Data processing and model specification flow chart. Abbreviations: HS = head shake, EC = Eyes-closed conditions. (A) Flow chart of the data processing. The two boxes in the bottom are the sample size submitted for the statistical analyses for all variables except CV Stride time *2 for CV stride time, n = 138 healthy and n = 61 concussed participants for the EC condition and n = 141 healthy and n = 60 concussed participants for the HS condition were submitted for analyses. (B) the model specification process: Fixed effects coefficients are B0, B1, B2, and B3, j = j-th group of i-th individual. u0i represents the random effect of the individual intercept, and e0i represents the residuals, where both are assumed to be normally distributed.

Finally, we have added the new statistical results in Tables 2 and 3 to lines 293 and 309 (also pasted below).

Stride Time

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 1.1503 0.006 200 208.716 < 0.001 StrideTime_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition -0.005 0.003 200 -1.703 0.0901

Group -0.017 0.001 200 -1.718 0.0873

Mean Maximum Knee Flexion Velocity (Max Vel) in deg/s

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 170.314 3.309 200 51.476 < 0.001 MaxVel_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition -5.944 1.565 200 -3.798 0.0002

Group 8.301 5.847 200 1.421 0.157

Mean Peak Knee Flexion Angle (Peak Flex) in deg

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 41.663 0.842 200 49.472 < 0.001 PeakFlex_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition -1.639 0.379 200 -4.326 < 0.001

Group 0.810 1.492 200 0.543 0.588

Mean of Range of Motion (ROM) in deg

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 37.782 0.782 200 48.323 < 0.001 ROM_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition -1.502 0.381 200 -3.941 0.0001

Group 1.261 1.379 200 0.914 0.362

Table 2. Final model and statistical results of each general performance kinematic dependent variable.

Variability (SD) of Maximum Veloicity (SD_MaxVel)

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 12.110 0.283 200 42,828 < 0.001 〖SD_MaxVel〗_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition 0.400 0.185 200 2.167 0.0314

Group 2.001 0.486 200 4,120 0.0001

Variability (CV) of Peak knee flexion (CV_PeakFlex)

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 6.432 0.189 199 34.091 < 0.001 CVPeakFlex_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition 0.370 0.135 199 2.738 0.0067

Group 0.268 0.319 199 0.839 0.4023

Variability (CV) of Stride time (CV_Stride Time)

Variable Coefficient SE DF t-value p-value Final model

Intercept 2.510 0.085 200 29.356 < 0.001 〖CV_StrideTime〗_ji=β_0+ B_1* Con_ji+B_2*Grp_ji++u_0i+ ε_ji

Condition 0.177 0.083 197 2.128 0.0346

Group 0.241 0.135 200 1.785 0.0758

Table 3. Final model and statistical results of each movement variability dependent variable.

The quantitative kinematics of the body are changed by concussion. However, recent evidence suggests that the quantitative kinematics of the body also may predict the risk of concussion in individuals (Chen et al., 2012, 2014, 2015). It would be helpful, in your Discussion, to address these effects. Might your cell phone approach be useful in a predictive context?

Thank you for connecting the predictive work in this space with our current effort. To address this connection, we have added the following text to lines 395-401:

“While the current study was a retrospective design that tested participants with a concussion history, it is important to note that work has been done to predict the risk of motion sickness after head trauma [58-60]. This approach aligns with the general approach to identify risk factors for a concussion [61], which has led to questions related to what strategies can be used to modify the sport and/or player care to reduce concussion risk [62]. As these approaches advance, it is plausible that such predictive analytics could be part of a smartphone app similar to the one tested in this paper, which could enhance athlete care.”

REVIEWER #2 COMMENTS

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript. I commend the author for considering the need for novel and affordable methodologies which seek to identify neuromotor alterations in civilian and military individuals with a history of concussion. While the methodologies of the study are novel, the prepared manuscript is absent of pertinent information and critical data that are necessary for publication consideration. Thus, I recommend rejection for publication in PLOS ONE. Below I have summarized major concerns to support the authors with the next phases of their work.

Thank you for your critiques and comments. We have revised our manuscript based on your comments, especially by adding more pertinent information related to our study.

The authors should consider reframing their manuscript to align with current literature regarding history of concussion. The authors refer to their participants as ‘concussed,’ which does not appear to be accurate given the length of time since last reported concussion. The participants’ current symptom status and whether they have been cleared to return to sport (or duty) should be included for reader interpretation. Likewise, I recommend authors include more detail on how history of concussion was quantified (e.g., was the ‘definition of concussion’ provided when asking participants to self-report? What about patients with a history of symptoms but no medical diagnosis?)

We appreciate you brining up this important detail and we agree with your comments. Given the average number of weeks since the concussion event presented in Table 1, we agree that it is important to frame the results as those with a history of concussion, rather than a concussed group. To clarify group membership, the following information was added to lines 142-150:

“If participants had a history of a concussion, they were assigned to the concussion history group. History of concussion was initially self-reported by the participants at the time of recruitment. However, prior to the day of testing, the participants medical professional provided a medical clearance consent form verifying that their patient did have a concussion within the last 6 months and that they supported them enrolling in our study with the knowledge of the type of testing that would occur. It is important to note that given the duration of time since the concussion event presented in Table 1, our population on average was not an acutely concussed population. Rather, our population was comprised of those who had a clinically diagnosed concussion within the past 6 months, and therefore are labeled as those with a concussion history.”

Moreover, we have also changed our language that used “concussed” to “those with concussion history” in the Title, Abstract, Methods, Results, and Discussion sections as denoted by the red text throughout.

Unfortunately, we do not have data on whether those with a concussion history were cleared to return to sport and active duty at the time of our testing. As for the symptoms, that dataset is incomplete. The Military Acute Concussion Evaluation (MACE)—which includes a symptom checklist—was given to the participants at the two civilian sites and the clinical military site. However, at one of the civilian sites, participants only completed the cognitive summary section of the MACE and did not fill out the symptom reporting section. Thus, we do not have symptom reporting for the civilians at that site (n=50 healthy and n=28 participants with concussion history), nor do we have symptom reporting at the military site where the healthy Service Members (n=54) participated, as the MACE was not given at that site.

More clarification regarding the phone app as being an ‘objective outcome.’ While the smartphone app is objective, they are discriminating against a highly subjective diagnosis (concussion diagnosis varies greatly across medical professionals – was the criteria standardized?).

That is a great point. The leading developer team created a manual video clip for other co-investigators. We also had meetings to ensure investigators at all sites understood the protocols. We only had a few criteria imposed on participants to capture natural stepping patterns. Additionally, there were no subjective ratings or diagnoses involved in our protocols. Thus, we believe that differences in the variables from the smartphone app can be reasonably considered as objective measures relative to the gold standards of concussion assessment (e.g., clinical assessment and criteria/symptom-checking surveys/diagnosis). We have revised our manuscript to include this information in lines 158-182.

“The protocol for AccWalker included strapping a customized armband onto the lateral aspect of the participants’ thigh, halfway between the lateral epicondyle and the greater trochanter, and placing the smartphone in the armband holder (Fig 1A). Participants were instructed to (a) naturally step in place while synchronizing their step timing to the app’s metronome, (b) look straightforward during trials, and (c) during familiarization trials, participants were informed to raise their knees higher if they were not moving as they normally walked (e.g., feet are not leaving the floor). The pacing metronome (period = 0.575 s or 1.15 s per stride) was provided for the initial 10 s of each trial. After 10 s, the metronome turned off, and the participant was instructed to continue stepping for 60 s. No other information was provided to participants in an attempt to capture natural kinematic patterns. The general protocol of the stepping-in-place task consisted of three trials for each of the three following conditions to alter sensory input: (1) with eyes open (EO), (2) with eyes closed (EC) to remove visual information, and (3) while rotating the head left-right to perturb vestibular information (head shake; HS). A 30-second practice session was provided for the EO and HS conditions prior to data collection to familiarize the participant with the task. The manual of the protocol was recorded by the smartphone app developing team for consistency and shared the protocols with partner teams.

For the present study, participants at the civilian sites performed 3 trials per condition for the EO, EC, and HS conditions (i.e., 60 seconds per trial x 3 trials x 3 conditions). Due to time constraints, participants at one military site performed only 2 trials of all three conditions (i.e., 60 seconds per trial x 2 trials x 3 conditions), and participants at the other military site performed 2 trials per condition only the EC and HS conditions (i.e., 60 second per trial x 2 trials x 2 conditions). In both sites, all participants completed the task in the order of EO, EC, HS for civilians and EC, HS for service members, with brief standing rest between trials (20 - 30 seconds). To compare common data that were collected at all sites, the mean of all dependent variables for 2 trials (instead of 3 trials) were calculated for the EC and HS conditions, which were then submitted for statistical analysis. This is congruent with previous work showing acceptable concurrent validity when averaging 2 trials in our protocol [21].”

Clearer presentation of inclusion and exclusion criteria and demographics more generally (e.g., limb dominance, age, and entire group missing demographics). For instance, the inclusion of individuals with an ambulatory device or prosthetic would present a separate group of individuals with differences in gait or neuromotor control.

The only inclusion criteria used for our study are listed in lines 136-139:

“Inclusion criteria for both groups included: (1) 18-50 years old, (2) at least moderately active (> 3 times of physical activity per week), (3) BMI under 33, (4) non-smoker, (5) normal or corrected-to-normal vision, (6) no surgeries in the past six months, (7) ability to walk with a prosthetic or assistive device, and (8) not pregnant at the time of study data collection.”

Unfortunately, we do not have sex data for all our participants. The reason for this is a large portion of our sample (all health Service Members, N=54) were collected as part of a larger previous study at a Department of Defense site and sex was not recorded as part of that dataset (nor were their demographics collected at that site). Nevertheless, we believe that it is important to report sex for the participants for whom we have those data, so we have added the following text to lines 122-126:

“The demographics of N = 54 healthy Service Members were not obtained. Sex of the participants for whom demographics were obtained are as follows: (1) for the 100 healthy civilians, 51 were female and 49 were male, (2) for the 44 civilians with a concussion history, 26 were female and 18 were male, (3) and for the 18 Service Members with a concussion history, 4 were female and 14 were male.”

We did not collect information on limb dominance. Any individuals with an ambulatory device or prosethic would have been excluded based on the inclusion criteria #7 above.

Lack of clarity in analyses. Specifically, the total n used in analyses differed without explanation (e.g., 154 healthy in table 1, but that number is not the n in any table 2 analysis).

Thank you for the opportunity to clarify the differences in the sample size. We created a flow chart to clarify the analysis procedure (Figure 2).

Fig 2. Data processing and model specification flow chart. Abbreviations: HS = head shake, EC = Eyes-closed conditions. (A) Flow chart of the data processing. The two boxes in the bottom are the sample size submitted for the statistical analyses for all variables except CV Stride time *2 for CV stride time, n = 138 healthy and n = 61 concussed participants for the EC condition and n = 141 healthy and n = 60 concussed participants for the HS condition were submitted for analyses. (B) the model specification process: Fixed effects coefficients are B0, B1, B2, and B3, j = j-th group of i-th individual. u0i represents the random effect of the individual intercept, and e0i represents the residuals, where both are assumed to be normally distributed.

While I appreciate the consideration of the OMVH, the data presented does not fully support the authors’ discussion points. The authors did not perform any nonlinear (e.g., complexity) analyses. Likewise, the optimal movement variability for healthy controls does not seem to be established in the literature.

Thank you for bringing up this important point. We believe that the OMVH encompasses various ‘families’ of variability, not necessarily derived from the chaos, fractal, or synergy theories, but also theories related to the magnitude of variability (e.g., optimal control theory, UCM, Bernstein’s concept, dynamical system theory). Moreover, we suggest that the data derived from our task is not a good candidate for nonlinear analyses. To acknowledge our data limitation, but with a nod to your point, we have added the following text to lines 385-394:

“It should be noted that temporal dynamics have significantly contributed to our knowledge of the optimal movement variability hypothesis. Under this framework, temporal dynamics are typically examined via a metric(s) that quantify the structure of the variability (i.e., sample entropy). However, a limitation of this approach is that enough data points (and more importantly, cyclic events) must be present in order for repeating patterns to naturally emerge. Short datasets typically do not allow for an emergence of such repeating patterns, and thus are not good candidates for structure of variability metrics. Our task was confined to 60 seconds of stepping-in-place per trial, leading to ~52 steps on average. While nonlinear dynamics could be run on such a short dataset, it would open up questions about validity and reliability of such an approach. Thus, we elected to focus on magnitude of variability metrics (i.e., coefficient of variation), which is more appropriate to quantify variability in short time series.”

In summary, while we do think the novelty of devices to measure neuromotor changes in the field are important to gain a greater understanding of individual deficits following concussive injury, the concerns stated above and other minor issues within the manuscript do not meet the current standards for publication in PLOS ONE.

Thank you for pointing the limitations of the manuscript. We have revised our manuscript based on your comments, and we believe the revised manuscript meets the standards for publication in PLOS ONE.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

Callam Davidson

2 Oct 2022

PONE-D-22-03586R1Neuromotor changes in participants with a concussion history can be detected with a custom smartphone appPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Rhea,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Nov 16 2022 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the editor and reviewers. You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Callam Davidson

Editorial Office

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please define all abbreviations in Figure 1 (in the legend).

Line 260: Please correct ‘remained’ to ‘remaining’.

Table 3: Per our statistical reporting guidelines (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines.#loc-statistical-reporting), please report exact p-values for all values greater than or equal to 0.001. P-values less than 0.001 may be expressed as p < 0.001, or as exponentials in studies of genetic associations.

Line 424: Please capitalise ‘We’.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Partly

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: I Don't Know

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #3: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The revision is good. It should be published. here are some extra characters to meet the minimum character count that is required.

Reviewer #3: This is the first revision of a manuscript reporting the results of a study of military and civilian participants with and without a recent (6-18 weeks) history of concussion. The SD of the mean maximum angular velocity of the thigh was found to be higher among participants with concussion history during the eyes closed and head shake conditions, compared to those without concussion history.

Most importantly, this analysis shows a statistical association, not discriminative ability – to do that would require a classification workflow (e.g., held out test set, classification performance assessment). I would encourage clarification of the language throughout the manuscript on this point.

Although the authors are to be commended for sharing the raw data from the study, it would help the reader to see descriptive analyses of the study variables – for example, a tabulation of the central tendency and distribution _and_ a scatter plot of the SD of maximum velocity (and the other ~6 potential input variables to a discriminative tool), across concussion vs. no concussion groups.

Specific comments

Both civilians and service members were young (18-50), healthy, and fit (height/weight). These results may not generalize to different populations.

For subgroups among whom 3 trials were performed, only 2 were analyzed. How were the 2/3 selected?

Was participant history of musculoskeletal injury collected? Such injuries could affect both range of motion and balance.

Table 1 and Results (first paragraph) – it is generally useful to have key comparisons be shown between table columns, with p-values shown in the table rather than only in text.

The time from concussion to testing was quite different between the two populations (6+ weeks civilians, ~18 weeks military) in a way that could affect results. Statistical associations were not found, but a more standardized study would be necessary before any final conclusions were drawn.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #3: Yes: Tellen D. Bennett

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Ryan Thomas Roemmich

29 Nov 2022

Neuromotor changes in participants with a concussion history can be detected with a custom smartphone app

PONE-D-22-03586R2

Dear Dr. Rhea,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Ryan Thomas Roemmich

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: I am typing these words to complete the 100-character minimum for this item, so that I can submit my recommendation.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Thomas A. Stoffregen

**********

Acceptance letter

Ryan Thomas Roemmich

7 Dec 2022

PONE-D-22-03586R2

Neuromotor changes in participants with a concussion history can be detected with a custom smartphone app

Dear Dr. Rhea:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at plosone@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Ryan Thomas Roemmich

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 File. Raw data.

    Dependent variable and demographic data for each participant.

    (XLSX)

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are available within the paper and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES