Skip to main content
Translational Psychiatry logoLink to Translational Psychiatry
. 2023 Jan 23;13:21. doi: 10.1038/s41398-022-02303-3

Approximating defense mechanisms in a national study of adults: prevalence and correlates with functioning

Carlos Blanco 1,, Leonie Kampe 2,3, Melanie M Wall 4, Shang-Min Liu 4, Shuai Wang 4, Eve Caligor 4, Mark Olfson 4
PMCID: PMC9870881  PMID: 36690611

Abstract

Despite the clinical relevance of defense mechanisms, there are no published studies in nationally representative samples of their prevalence, correlates, and association with psychosocial functioning. We sought to estimate the prevalence and correlates of 12 defense mechanisms in the general adult population by approximating from items used to assess personality traits in the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC), a representative sample of US adults (N = 36,653). We examined the associations between sociodemographic characteristics and prevalence of 3 types of defenses mechanisms (pathological, immature, and neurotic). For each defense mechanism, we used the Short-Form 12 to compare psychosocial functioning among 3 groups: those who (1) endorsed the mechanism with self-recognized impairment, (2) endorsed the mechanism without self-recognized impairment, and (3) did not endorse the defense mechanism. The prevalence of defense mechanisms ranged from 13.2% (splitting) to 44.5% (obsessive/controlling behavior). Pathological defenses were more strongly associated with immature defenses (OR = 5.4, 95% CI = 5.2–5.6) than with neurotic defenses (OR = 2.0, 95% CI = 1.9–2.0), whereas the association between immature and neurotic defenses had an intermediate value between the other two (OR = 2.2, 95% CI = 2.1–2.2). Pathological and immature defenses were associated with younger age, having been never married, lower educational attainment, and lower income. After adjusting the crude results for age and sex, individuals who did not endorse a given defense generally had higher scores on the mental health component of the SF-12 than those who endorsed the defense without self-recognized impairment who, in turn, had on average higher scores than those with self-recognized impairment. These results suggest that neurotic, immature, and pathological defense mechanisms are prevalent in the general population and associated with psychosocial impairment. Recognizing defense mechanisms may be important in clinical practice regardless of treatment modality.

Subject terms: Psychiatric disorders, Human behaviour

Introduction

Defense mechanisms, defined as mostly unconscious mental operations as an individual’s automatic psychological responses to internal or external stressors or emotional conflict [1]. They serve to keep unpleasant affects, conflicts, or mental states out of awareness and help coping with emotional distress. However, it has been shown that there are differences among the defense mechanisms in terms of their adaptiveness. The concept of defense mechanisms (also known as defenses) is central in psychoanalysis, psychodynamic psychiatry, and psychology [2]. It spans theory, clinical practice, and research, and has even been incorporated into everyday discourse. Empirical research to-date has largely relied on clinical patient studies and convenience samples, reporting the prevalence of individual defenses, defense levels, categories, and styles, as well as their association with symptom and functioning variables [24]. Treatment studies have also demonstrated the change characteristics of defenses in relationship to clinical outcome variables. Despite their clinical importance and privileged position in psychodynamic models of psychopathology [5] and development, to our knowledge, no published study has investigated defense mechanisms in a large, general population sample.

A better understanding of defense mechanisms could help advance theory, guide clinical care and improve our understanding of their function. For example, knowing whether immature or pathological defense mechanisms are prevalent in the general population or appear restricted to a small number of individuals would inform whether they are common psychological phenomena or markers of severe psychopathology. Similarly, knowing if they are associated with changes in psychological functioning could indicate whether they should be targets of therapeutic interventions. Furthermore, examining whether there are differences in the relationship between individual defense mechanisms and level of psychosocial functioning may support the existence of a hierarchy of adaptiveness, as postulated by psychoanalytic theory and studies of clinical and non-clinical samples.

An extensive review of the literature on defense mechanisms identified empirical support for the following characteristics: defenses function mostly outside of awareness, develop in a predictable order as individuals mature, are present in the healthy personality, become increasingly used in times of stress, reduce the conscious experience of negative emotions, and, when used excessively, are associated with psychopathology [6]. Several studies have shown that defense mechanisms can be arranged hierarchically based on their association with the level of functioning of the individual [4, 7], concluding that, with the exception of mature defenses, their use is generally associated with lower psychosocial functioning [2, 8, 9].

A landmark prospective cohort study of 268 Harvard sophomores observed their defensive operations over the course of a 25-year period [4, 10]. Based on the results, a hierarchy of adaptiveness of defenses was developed [4] ranging from adaptive mechanisms over neurotic mechanisms to most rigid and most rigid and maladaptive mechanism. The entire spectrum of individual defenses was assessed among healthy subjects and therefore were not necessarily pathological or exclusively found in adults with psychopathology. At the same time, adaptiveness of the individual’s predominant pattern of defense mechanisms defensive style predicted overall psychosocial adjustment [10]. Building on this work, a second study evaluated defense mechanisms in 306 inner-city men at age 47, followed as part of a 50-year longitudinal study, to validate their hierarchy of defenses [7]. Based on this and additional prior work [11], it has been proposed that defenses can be organized hierarchically in four levels [4]; the least functional are the pathological defenses, which involve gross distortion of reality [12, 13] and are strongly related conceptually to severe psychopathology; next are immature defenses, which distort interpersonal reality and have been found to be most prominent in personality disorders [14]; neurotic defenses are intrapsychic mechanisms that relate to psychological suffering when used inflexibly and with rigidity; and, finally most adaptive, mature defenses are strategies for coping and can be used flexibly and sometimes consciously. Other studies have compared the use of defense mechanisms in patients experiencing external stressors to community comparison groups. For example, Perry et al. compared defensive functioning in a sample of mothers with a recent history of breast cancer to a matched sample of healthy mothers in their local community [15].

A number of basic questions about defense mechanisms remain. First, most studies on defense mechanisms have relied on clinical or other samples for which the degree of generalization to the whole adult population is unknown [13, 1618]. Second, the use of clinical samples does not permit estimation of the prevalence and correlates of defense mechanisms in the general population. Third, while clinical work suggests that certain individuals recognize that their use of defense mechanisms is associated with impairment in their daily life, others do not or may not be aware of their uses of defenses. We are not aware of any studies that have examined these questions in a nationally representative sample. Based on previous studies, we hypothesized that: (1) the prevalence of pathological, immature, and neurotic mechanisms of defense would be each 25% or greater; and (2) use of immature and pathological defense mechanisms would be associated with lower psychosocial functioning.

Although defense mechanisms typically operate outside of awareness, their impact on an individual’s behavior, thoughts, and feelings can be observed and assessed, and used to approximate underlying defense mechanisms [24, 10, 19]. In the following analyses, we used items collected as part of the National Epidemiological Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) a large, nationally representative sample, to approximate mechanisms of defense, estimate their prevalence and correlates in the general population, examine whether defense mechanisms are associated with lower psychosocial functioning, and test whether those who recognize impairment have lower psychosocial impairment than those who did not. Although the NESARC was not designed to examine defenses and did not include any direct validation of those constructs (in contrast with studies devised to examined defense mechanisms) [2, 11, 19], we sought to examine whether our approximations had convergent validity by examining their correlation with other measures of functioning.

Methods

Sample

The 2001–2002 NESARC (Wave 1) and the 2004–2005 follow-up (Wave 2) is a nationally representative sample of the noninstitutionalized adult US population conducted by the US Census Bureau, under the direction of the National Institute on Alcoholism and Alcohol Abuse, as described elsewhere [20]. The Wave 1 response rate was 81%. Excluding ineligible respondents (e.g., deceased), the Wave 2 response rate was 86.7%, resulting in a cumulative response rate of 70.2% (n = 34,653). Wave 2 NESARC weights include a component that adjusts for non-response, demographic factors, and psychiatric diagnoses, to ensure that the Wave 2 sample approximated the target population, that is, the original sample minus attrition between the two waves [20].

Assessment

All NESARC participants were assessed with the AUDADIS-IV, the NIAAA Alcohol Use Disorder and Associated Disabilities Interview Schedule-DSM IV Version (AUDADIS-IV), a valid and reliable fully structured diagnostic interview designed for use by professional interviewers. Fully structured interviews, rather than clinical assessments, are generally used in large epidemiological studies to ensure their feasibility and decrease procedural variation across interviewers and associated potential biases. A senior psychotherapy researcher and clinician (LK) reviewed the AUDADIS-IV to identify items that could be used to assess underlying defensive operations. Candidate items were then subject to a second-level review and confirmation by a training and supervising analyst (EC) with extensive experience in psychotherapy research and psychoanalytic practice. This process resulted in extraction of items that could be used to approximate 12 defense mechanisms (psychotic distortion, delusional projection, autistic fantasy, projection, withdrawal, acting out, splitting, idealization, devaluation, omnipotence, isolation of affect, obsessive/controlling behavior, and intellectualization (see Appendix in the Supplementary Material for items). All identified items were found in the personality disorders section of the interview and were asked in binary form (yes/no). If an item was endorsed, respondents were queried as to whether the item caused impairment to them. Defense mechanisms were rated as present if one or more of their constituent items were endorsed. Individuals endorsing items assessing defense mechanisms were asked whether the items representing defense mechanisms had interfered with their relations with family or friends, or at work. Thus, for each defense mechanism, individuals were classified into three levels: no endorsement of the defense mechanism, endorsement without self-recognized impairment, and endorsement with self-recognized impairment.

Based on the classification system developed by Vaillant [4, 10], and furthermore informed by the DSM-IV Defensive Functioning Scale [3], these items were subordinated into three levels of adaptiveness (pathological, immature, and neurotic). The pathological category accounted for 2 of the 12 defense mechanisms, the immature category accounted for 8, and the neurotic category for the remaining 2. As expected, since items were extracted from the section of personality disorders, no mature defense mechanisms were assessed in the survey.

Psychosocial functioning was assessed using the Short-Form 12 version 2 (SF-12) [21], a 12-item measure that is a reliable and valid measure of disability used in population surveys [2224]. The SF-12 is normed to have a mean = 50 and SD = 10. Higher scores indicate better psychosocial functioning. In line with previous reports, we focused on the Mental Component Summary (MCS) because of its particular relevance for overall mental health and functioning, rather than focusing on specific diagnoses. The MCS applies regression weights to the all of the SF-12 items to derive a synthetic measure of mental health [22]. In addition, all NESARC respondents were asked to report their race-ethnicity, age, marital status, educational achievement, and individual income.

Statistical analyses

Weighted prevalence of mechanisms of defense were estimated for the overall sample and stratified by sociodemographic characteristics. Others Risk differences were used to compare the prevalence of defense mechanisms by sociodemographic group. They were considered to be significant if their 95% did not include 0. T-tests were used to compare scores on the SF-12 between those with and without each defense mechanism. Linear regressions, yielding adjusted means, were used to compare SF-12 scores adjusting for age, sex, and race/ethnicity. No adjustments were made for marital status, educational attainment, or income as those might be influenced by the presence of the defense mechanisms and result in collider bias [25]. Statistical significance was set at 0.05. Furthermore, for all analyses, we considered two point estimates (e.g., prevalence estimates, ORs, scores on the MCS of the SF-12) to be significantly different if their 95% CI did not overlap.

Results

There was a broad range of variation in the prevalence of the defense mechanisms. When the impairment criterion was not applied, the prevalence ranged from 13.2% (splitting) to 44.5% (obsessive/controlling behavior). Use of at least one neurotic defense was endorsed by 53.6% of respondents, while use of at least one immature defense was endorsed by 49.5%, and at least one pathological defense by 39.4%.

Applying the impairment criterion resulted in lower prevalence for all mechanisms of defense, but still considerable variability in prevalence across mechanisms from 1.2% (autistic fantasy) to 11.3% (projection). Use of at least one immature defense (25.4%) was more common than use of at least one neurotic defense (14.6%), which in turn remained more common than use of at least one pathological defense (7.3%). Figure 1 depicts the prevalence of mechanisms of defense when the impairment criterion was not applied (full bar), as well as the prevalence when the impairment criterion was applied (blue portion of the bar). The prevalence of mechanisms of defense that did not meet the impairment criterion is represented by the orange portion of the bar.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Prevalence of mechanisms of defense in the National Epidemiological Survey on alcohol and related conditions.

The OR of the association between pathological and immature defenses 5.4 (95% CI = 5.2–5.6), whereas the OR of the association of pathological and neurotic defenses was 2.0 (95% CI = 1.9–2.0), and the OR of association between immature and neurotic defenses was 2.2 (95% CI = 2.1–2.2).

Pathological defenses were associated with younger age, having been never married, lower educational attainment, and lower income. Immature defenses were associated with younger age, having been never married, and income between $10,000 and $24,999 (Table 1).

Table 1.

Association of demographic characteristics with three classes of mechanism of defense in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (N = 34,653).

Any pathological defense (n = 13,968) Any immature defense (n = 26,711) Any neurotic defense (n = 23,432)
N Row % Risk Diff 95% CI Row % Risk Diff 95% CI Row % Risk Diff 95% CI
Age, years 18–30 5471 45.1 11.2 10.2 12.2 87.1 19.3 18.3 20.2 69.0 0.7 −0.2 1.7
31–40 6866 39.4 5.4 4.7 6.1 78.8 11.0 10.1 11.9 67.0 −1.2 −2.1 −0.3
41–50 7424 38.3 4.3 3.6 5.1 75.0 7.2 6.4 8.1 68.8 0.6 −0.3 1.4
51+ 14892 34.0 Ref 67.8 Ref 68.2 Ref
Gender Female 20089 39.4 3.1 2.4 3.7 71.9 −6.4 −7.1 −5.8 68.1 −0.2 −0.9 0.5
Male 14564 36.4 Ref 78.3 Ref 68.4 Ref
Race Black 6587 52.7 17.7 16.7 18.6 87.0 14.8 14.1 15.5 67.1 −2.8 −3.8 −1.9
Native American 578 46.5 11.5 8.4 14.6 81.2 9.0 6.3 11.7 69.0 −1.0 −3.9 2.0
Asian 968 32.2 −2.8 −3.6 −1.9 71.4 −0.7 −1.9 0.5 61.4 −8.6 −9.7 −7.4
Hispanic 6359 42.7 7.7 6.7 8.6 80.7 8.5 7.8 9.3 61.2 −8.7 −9.5 −7.9
White 20161 35.0 Ref 72.2 Ref 69.9 Ref
Marital status Never married 6638 45.8 11.2 10.3 12.1 88.0 17.3 16.6 17.9 68.8 0.6 −0.3 1.4
Divorced/separated/widow 9149 42.0 7.3 6.5 8.2 77.3 6.6 5.9 7.4 67.6 −0.6 −1.4 0.1
Married 18866 34.6 Ref 70.7 Ref 68.3 Ref
Education High school or less 13699 38.8 1.4 0.7 2.1 75.4 0.7 −0.1 1.4 64.2 −6.5 −7.3 −5.8
Some college or more 20954 37.4 Ref 74.7 Ref 70.7 Ref
Income, $ 0–9999 7924 42.9 12.1 11.2 13.0 74.6 2.3 1.4 3.2 67.2 −3.9 −4.9 −3.0
10,000–24,999 10496 41.2 10.4 9.6 11.3 76.7 4.4 3.5 5.3 66.6 −4.5 −5.3 −3.6
25,000–49,999 9745 36.0 5.2 4.4 6.1 75.3 3.0 2.2 3.8 68.7 −2.4 −3.2 −1.5
≥50,000 6488 30.7 Ref 72.3 Ref 71.1 Ref

In the overall sample, the mental health component summary had a mean = 51.4 and a standard deviation = 9.5. After adjusting the crude results for age and sex, individuals who did not endorse a given defense had on average higher scores than those who endorsed the defense without impairment who, in turn, had on average higher scores than those with impairment. The only exception to this pattern was for obsessive/controlling behavior, in which individuals who did not endorse this defense had higher scores than those who endorsed it with impairment, but lower scores than those who endorse it without impairment (Table 2).

Table 2.

Mental Component Summary Score of the Short-Form 12 (SF-12) in individuals with and without mechanisms of defense in the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (N = 34,653).

N Mental Component Summary Score No mechanism of defense versus mechanism without impairment Mechanism of defense with versus without impairment
No mechanism of defense Mechanism of defense without impairment Mechanism of defense with impairment Adjusted betaa p-value Adjusted betaa p-value
Mechanism of Defense Mean 95% CI Mean 95% CI Mean 95% CI
Any Pathological 13,968 52.8 52.7 52.8 50.7 50.6 50.8 43.2 42.9 43.5 2.0 <0.0001 9.5 <0.0001
Psychotic distortion 8961 52.0 51.9 52.0 50.3 50.1 50.4 42.5 41.7 43.3 1.6 <0.0001 9.3 <0.0001
Delusional projection 9637 52.6 52.6 52.7 49.7 49.5 49.8 42.9 42.5 43.3 3.0 <0.0001 9.6 <0.0001
Any Immature 26,711 53.4 53.2 53.5 52.4 52.4 52.5 47.6 47.4 47.7 1.1 <0.0001 5.8 <0.0001
Autistic fantasy 7615 51.8 51.7 51.9 50.4 50.2 50.5 43.9 43.3 44.6 1.3 <0.0001 7.8 <0.0001
Projection 13,073 52.4 52.3 52.5 50.9 50.8 51.0 46.9 46.7 47.1 1.5 <0.0001 5.2 <0.0001
Withdrawal 6936 52.1 52.0 52.1 49.8 49.6 49.9 43.8 43.3 44.2 2.3 <0.0001 8.2 <0.0001
Acting out 9996 52.2 52.1 52.2 51.1 50.9 51.2 45.8 45.6 46.1 1.4 <0.0001 6.5 <0.0001
Splitting 8089 52.5 52.4 52.6 49.3 49.1 49.5 44.8 44.5 45.1 3.2 <0.0001 7.6 <0.0001
Idealization 13,184 52.0 51.9 52.1 51.1 51.0 51.2 45.1 44.7 45.4 0.9 <0.0001 6.8 <0.0001
Devaluation 6637 51.9 51.8 51.9 50.1 50.0 50.3 45.6 45.2 46.0 2.0 <0.0001 6.4 <0.0001
Omnipotence 12,650 51.9 51.9 52.0 51.3 51.1 51.4 47.3 47.0 47.5 0.9 <0.0001 4.9 <0.0001
Any Neurotic 23,432 52.2 52.1 52.3 52.0 52.0 52.1 47.5 47.3 47.7 0.2 0.26 4.6 <0.0001
Affect isolation 12,686 52.1 52.0 52.2 51.3 51.2 51.4 46.1 45.8 46.4 1.1 <0.0001 6.2 <0.0001
Obsessive behavior/controlling 18,293 51.7 51.6 51.8 52.0 51.9 52.1 48.0 47.8 48.3 −0.5 0.0006 3.4 <0.0001

aAdjusted for age, sex, and race/ethnicity.

Adjusted differences between individuals who did and did not endorse a given defense without self-recognized impairment ranged from obsessive/controlling behavior (−0.5) to splitting (3.2). Differences between individuals who either endorsed a defense with self-recognized impairment or did not endorse it ranged from obsessive/controlling behavior (3.4) to delusional projection (9.6).

Discussion

In a large, nationally representative sample of US adults, more than 25% used at least one of the defense mechanisms and use of any one defense mechanism increased the likelihood of using others. Although most adults endorsing a defense mechanism did not believe that it interfered with their work or relations with family or friends, there was a gradient in the level of psychosocial functioning from no endorsement of the defense to endorsement without self-recognized impairment to endorsement with self-recognized impairment. These findings provide new insights into the distribution and functioning of defense mechanisms at the population level.

To our knowledge, this is the first study to assess the prevalence of defense mechanisms in a nationally representative sample. We found that defense mechanisms were widespread. While there are no other population-based studies to compare results, our findings are consistent with clinical experience and with studies of clinical samples and smaller non-clinical samples [1, 2, 9, 26]. For example, a study that examined mechanisms of defense in women with breast cancer and a comparison group of women with no breast cancer found that, among the comparison group, all of them used at least one neurotic defense and 96.2% of them at least one immature defense. The pervasive use of defense mechanisms at all levels of adaptive functioning suggests that they constitute essential intrapsychic operations. Because defenses can become more adaptive and functional as individuals mature, and are modifiable through treatment, recognizing and learning how to work with defenses appears to be an essential skill for clinical practice [18, 26].

There was a broad range in the prevalence of individual defenses, consistent with previous findings that individuals use a variety of approaches to manage the relationship between their internal world and external reality [27]. The use of specific defenses and their level of adaptiveness probably results from a combination of life events, cultural environment, genetic predisposition, and the ability of individuals to modify these factors through maturation, insight, and the nurturing of others [26, 28]. For example, as compared to a community comparison group, women with breast cancer were more likely to use lower-level defensive styles than the comparison group, suggesting that stressful events may influence the type of defenses used by individuals.

Pathological and immature mechanisms of defense were inversely associated with age, whereas differences in the age distribution of neurotic defenses were less marked. These findings are consistent with smaller, longitudinal studies that have also found an inverse association between age and level of defenses [4, 6, 10]. Because defenses cannot influence age (i.e., reverse causation is not possible), these results suggest that as individuals age they are less likely to use lower-level defenses. An alternative and in our view less likely explanation is that as individuals age, they become less aware of their use of pathological and immature defenses.

Women were more likely than men to endorse pathological and immature defenses, but equally likely to endorse neurotic defenses. Sex differences in defenses could be due to differences in development [29, 30], cultural expectations, or accepted social norms [31], or be the result of adaptations to situations in which women are victims of discrimination of violence [32]. They may also reflect differences in expression, self-awareness, or willingness to acknowledge certain behaviors [33]. Sex differences in the expression of psychopathology have been extensively documented [3436]. Nevertheless, because we are not aware of any other studies that have examined sex differences in defenses, we believe these findings should be considered tentative and in need of replication.

Use of a given defense increased the probability of using any other defenses, yielding a potentially high number of combinations of defenses and clinical presentations. In these combinations, defenses that were closer to each other on the adaptation hierarchy (e.g., pathological and immature) were more strongly associated than those hypothesized to be more distantly related (e.g., pathological and neurotic). These findings are in accord with a hierarchy of adaptiveness of defensive operations, and with clinical experience and clinical research [36] suggesting that as individuals progress in treatment, defenses tend to shift gradually, moving up to the next higher level of adaptation. The observed patterns are also consistent with the reports that a given defensive style generally, although not always, is substituted by defenses closer in the hierarchy of adaptation than by those more distantly related [26].

As compared to individuals who used a given defense mechanism, those who did not use it had on average higher psychosocial functioning. For each defense, most individuals endorsing it did not consider that the defense interfered with their life, suggesting that even when individuals are aware of using a particular defense, they often remain unaware of the potential maladaptive nature of their behavior. This frequent lack of insight is in line with the psychodynamic view and clinical experience that defense mechanisms are generally ego syntonic, exerting a toll on psychosocial functioning that typically remains outside of the awareness of those using them. It is only as defenses become more extreme, inflexible, and pervasive that the individual is likely to become aware of their associated functional impairment and the defense becomes ego dystonic [26]. One exception to this pattern may be obsessive/controlling behavior. It is possible, for some individuals or when used at relatively low levels, that use of that defense may, may mimic some aspects of mature defenses and help increase psychosocial functioning. However, for others or when used excessively, it may reverse and begin to degrade psychosocial functioning. Future research should investigate under what circumstances or at what levels use of this defense leads to improved functioning.

From the clinical point of view, the widespread use of defense mechanisms suggests that they are likely to manifest themselves not only in psychotherapy, but also during pharmacological or other types of treatment and in interactions with other health care professionals. The clinical reality that patients may be unaware of the extent of their use of maladaptive defenses highlights the need to evaluate defensive functioning in patients who present in distress or having unexpected difficulties coping. Severity of impairment in the setting of maladaptive use of defenses might determine the need for treatment by individuals with expertise in psychodynamic psychotherapy or other approaches directed at improving defensive operations.

From the theoretical point of view, our findings are consistent with the hypothesized central role of defenses in intrapsychic operations and in determining behavior. They are also consistent with predictions that, despite variability in impairment within each level of defenses, lower-level defenses are generally associated with greater psychosocial impairment than higher level ones. Future research should investigate whether these findings hold for mature defenses in the general population and the mechanisms that lead to defense-associated functional impairment. Twin and other family-based designs could shed light on the role of genetic and environmental contributions to the development and use of defense mechanisms.

This study has several limitations. First, defense mechanisms, which are complex constructs, were approximated using items developed to assess personality disorder criteria rather than defenses per se. In some cases, such as the assessment of obsessive behavior, the boundary between personality disorder criteria and conscious derivatives of defenses may have been imperfect. Furthermore, as in most large population psychiatric epidemiological studies, items were assessed by a structured interview, rather than by a clinician. Nevertheless, the finding of a gradient in psychosocial functioning from no endorsement of defense mechanism, to use without impairment, to use with impairment supports the face validity of the assessment. Second, only 123 mechanisms could be assessed, most of which were pathological or immature, and mature mechanisms were not assessed. Because immature defenses were rated with a greater number of items, respondents may have been more likely to endorse them than pathological or neurotic defenses that had fewer items. It is possible that assessment of other mechanisms would have yielded a different pattern of results. Furthermore, because the pathological and neurotic categories had only two items, it was not possible to assess the internal consistency of this category or to use the items as indicators or a latent variable. Third, the NESARC did not assess the frequency of use of each defense mechanism. Fourth, because these data consist of cross-sectional associations, they can suggest but not prove any causal relationship between defenses and psychosocial functioning. Finally, because the NESARC did not assess individuals in jails or prisons or inpatient populations, the results may not generalize to several important institutionalized populations.

In conclusion, defense mechanisms are common in the general adult population and are associated with decreased psychosocial functioning. The high prevalence of defenses suggests the centrality of these mental processes and their importance in models of intrapsychic functioning. Recognizing and managing defenses appear essential skills for practicing clinicians regardless of their theoretical orientation.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (17.9KB, docx)

Acknowledgements

The National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions was sponsored by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism and funded, in part, by the Intramural Program, NIAAA, National Institutes of Health. The sponsors had no additional role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. The views and opinions expressed in this report are those of the authors and should not be construed to represent the views of any of the sponsoring organizations, agencies or the US government.

Author contributions

Conceptualization and design: All authors; identification of mechanisms of defense: LK and EC; data analyses: MMW, SML, and SW; initial draft of the manuscript: CB; critical revision of the manuscript: All authors; final approval of the manuscript: All authors.

Funding

Open Access funding provided by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41398-022-02303-3.

References

  • 1.Bond M, Perry JC. Long-term changes in defense styles with psychodynamic psychotherapy for depressive, anxiety, and personality disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161:1665–71. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.9.1665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Bond M, Gardner ST, Christian J, Sigal JJ. Empirical study of self-rated defense styles. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1983;40:333–8. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1983.01790030103013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Perry JC, Hoglend P, Shear K, Vaillant GE, Horowitz M, Kardos ME, et al. Field trial of a diagnostic axis for defense mechanisms for DSM-IV. J Pers Disord. 1998;12:56–68. doi: 10.1521/pedi.1998.12.1.56. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Vaillant GE. Theoretical hierarchy of adaptive ego mechanisms: a 30-year follow-up of 30 men selected for psychological health. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1971;24:107–18. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750080011003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kampe L, Bohn J, Remmers C, Hörz-Sagstetter S. It’s not that great anymore: the central role of defense mechanisms in grandiose and vulnerable narcissism. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:661948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 6.Cramer P. Seven pillars of defense mechanism theory. Soc Personal Psychol Compass. 2008;2:1963–81.. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00135.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Soldz S, Vaillant GE. A 50-year longitudinal study of defense use among inner city men: a validation of the DSM-IV defense axis. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1998;186:104–11. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199802000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Hilsenroth MJ, Callahan KL, Eudell EM. Further reliability, convergent and discriminant validity of overall defensive functioning. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2003;191:730–7. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000095125.92493.e8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Vaillant GE. The wisdom of the ego. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993.
  • 10.Vaillant GE. Adaptation to life. Boston, MA: Little, Brown; 1977.
  • 11.Prout TA, Di Giuseppe M, Zilcha-Mano S, Perry JC, Conversano C. Psychometric properties of the Defense Mechanisms Rating Scales-Self-Report-30 (DMRS-SR-30): internal consistency, validity and factor structure. J Pers Assess. 2022;104:833–43. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 12.Berney S, de Roten Y, Beretta V, Kramer U, Despland JN. Identifying psychotic defenses in a clinical interview. J Clin Psychol. 2014;70:428–39. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Constantinides P, Beck SM. Toward developing a scale to empirically measure psychotic defense mechanisms. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. 2010;58:1159–88. doi: 10.1177/0003065110396875. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Vaillant GE. Ego mechanisms of defense and personality psychopathology. J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103:44–50. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.103.1.44. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Perry JC, Metzger J, Sigal JJ. Defensive functioning among women with breast cancer and matched community controls. Psychiatry. 2015;78:156–69. doi: 10.1080/00332747.2015.1051445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Hersoug AG, Sexton HC, Hoglend P. Contribution of defensive functioning to the quality of working alliance and psychotherapy outcome. Am J Psychother. 2002;56:539–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.2002.56.4.539. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Knijnik DZ, Salum GA, Jr., Blanco C, Moraes C, Hauck S, Mombach CK, et al. Defense style changes with the addition of psychodynamic group therapy to clonazepam in social anxiety disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197:547–51. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181aac833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Kramer U, Despland JN, Michel L, Drapeau M, de Roten Y. Change in defense mechanisms and coping over the course of short-term dynamic psychotherapy for adjustment disorder. J Clin Psychol. 2010;66:1232–41. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Di Giuseppe M, Perry JC, Lucchesi M, Michelini M, Vitiello S, Piantanida A, et al. Preliminary reliability and validity of the DMRS-SR-30, a Novel Self-Report Measure Based on the Defense Mechanisms Rating Scales. Front Psychiatry. 2020;11:870. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00870. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Grant BF, Goldstein RB, Chou SP, Huang B, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, et al. Sociodemographic and psychopathologic predictors of first incidence of DSM-IV substance use, mood and anxiety disorders: results from the Wave 2 National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14:1051–66. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.41. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ware J, Jr., Kosinski M, Keller SD. A 12-Item Short-Form Health Survey: construction of scales and preliminary tests of reliability and validity. Med Care. 1996;34:220–33. doi: 10.1097/00005650-199603000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Ware J, Kosinski M, Turner-Bowker D, Gandek B. SF-12v2 How to score version 2 of the SF-12 health survey. Lincoln, RI: Quality Metric Incorporated; 2002.
  • 23.Rubio JM, Olfson M, Villegas L, Perez-Fuentes G, Wang S, Blanco C. Quality of life following remission of mental disorders: findings from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;74:e445–50. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m08269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Rubio JM, Olfson M, Perez-Fuentes G, Garcia-Toro M, Wang S, Blanco C. Effect of first episode axis I disorders on quality of life. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014;202:271–4. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Holmberg MJ, Andersen LW. Collider bias. JAMA. 2022;327:1282–3. doi: 10.1001/jama.2022.1820. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Caligor EK, Otto F, Clarkin, JF, Yeomans, FE. Psychodynamic therapy for personality pathology: treating self and interpersonal functioning. American Psychiatric Association Publishing; 2018.
  • 27.Vaillant GE, Vaillant CO. Natural history of male psychological health, XII: a 45-year study of predictors of successful aging at age 65. Am J Psychiatry. 1990;147:31–7. doi: 10.1176/ajp.147.1.31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Vaillant GE, editor. Ego mechanisms of defense: a guide for clinicians and researchers. American Psychiatric Pub; 1992.
  • 29.Smith R, Steklis HD, Steklis NG, Weihs KL, Lane RD. The evolution and development of the uniquely human capacity for emotional awareness: a synthesis of comparative anatomical, cognitive, neurocomputational, and evolutionary psychological perspectives. Biol Psychol. 2020;154:107925. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Hodes GE, Epperson CN. Sex differences in vulnerability and resilience to stress across the life span. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86:421–32.. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Ullman SE, Peter-Hagene LC, Relyea M. Coping, emotion regulation, and self-blame as mediators of sexual abuse and psychological symptoms in adult sexual assault. J Child Sex Abus. 2014;23:74–93. doi: 10.1080/10538712.2014.864747. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Blanco C, Hoertel N, Wall MM, Franco S, Peyre H, Neria Y, et al. Toward understanding sex differences in the prevalence of posttraumatic stress disorder: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. J Clin Psychiatry. 2018;79:16m11364. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 33.Else-Quest NM, Higgins A, Allison C, Morton LC. Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2012;138:947–81. doi: 10.1037/a0027930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.McMahon K, Hoertel N, Peyre H, Blanco C, Fang C, Limosin F. Age differences in DSM-IV borderline personality disorder symptom expression: results from a national study using item response theory (IRT) J Psychiatr Res. 2019;110:16–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.12.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rivollier F, Peyre H, Hoertel N, Blanco C, Limosin F, Delorme R. Sex differences in DSM-IV posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms expression using item response theory: a population-based study. J Affect Disord. 2015;187:211–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.07.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Perry JC, Bond M. Change in defense mechanisms during long-term dynamic psychotherapy and five-year outcome. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169:916–25. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (17.9KB, docx)

Articles from Translational Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES