Skip to main content
Pediatrics logoLink to Pediatrics
. 2015 Oct;136(Suppl 1):S10–S40. doi: 10.1542/peds.2014-3667C

Early Identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder: Recommendations for Practice and Research

Lonnie Zwaigenbaum a,, Margaret L Bauman b, Wendy L Stone c, Nurit Yirmiya d, Annette Estes e, Robin L Hansen f, James C McPartland g, Marvin R Natowicz h, Roula Choueiri i, Deborah Fein j, Connie Kasari k, Karen Pierce l, Timothy Buie m, Alice Carter n, Patricia A Davis o, Doreen Granpeesheh p, Zoe Mailloux q, Craig Newschaffer r, Diana Robins r, Susanne Smith Roley s, Sheldon Wagner t, Amy Wetherby u
PMCID: PMC9923897  PMID: 26430168

Abstract

Early identification of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is essential to ensure that children can access specialized evidence-based interventions that can help to optimize long-term outcomes. Early identification also helps shorten the stressful “diagnostic odyssey” that many families experience before diagnosis. There have been important advances in research into the early development of ASDs, incorporating prospective designs and new technologies aimed at more precisely delineating the early emergence of ASD. Thus, an updated review of the state of the science of early identification of ASD was needed to inform best practice. These issues were the focus of a multidisciplinary panel of clinical practitioners and researchers who completed a literature review and reached consensus on current evidence addressing the question “What are the earliest signs and symptoms of ASD in children aged ≤24 months that can be used for early identification?” Summary statements address current knowledge on early signs of ASD, potential contributions and limitations of prospective research with high-risk infants, and priorities for promoting the incorporation of this knowledge into clinical practice and future research.


Despite efforts to increase awareness of early signs of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and promote early screening, 1 as well as some evidence of recent trends toward diagnosing younger children (as reviewed by Daniels and Mandell 2 ), several large-scale epidemiologic studies suggest that the mean age of diagnosis in the United States remains at ∼4 to 5 years. 3 5 Given that parents of children with ASD generally report initial concerns before the child is aged 18 to 24 months, considerable opportunity exists to shorten the stressful “diagnostic odyssey” that many families experience, 6 maximize opportunities for children with ASD to benefit from early intensive interventions, and further develop evidence-based interventions for this age group. 7

For many years, much of what was known about the early signs of ASD was informed by parents’ descriptions of their initial concerns, 8 10 as well as analyses of early home videos. 11 13 Rich insights from these data (complemented by experimental work that helped delineate key foundational processes impaired in ASD, such as affect sharing and joint attention 14 , 15 ) helped to inform the development of ASD-screening tools and surveillance efforts by community health professionals. 16 Over the past decade, important advances in research have been made into the early development of ASD, incorporating prospective research designs 17 and new technologies aimed at more precisely delineating the early emergence of ASD. 18 , 19 Advances have also been made in identifying potential biomarkers (eg, genetic, neuroimaging), although there are important clinical and ethical considerations regarding their potential application. 20

These issues were the focus of an international, multidisciplinary panel of clinical practitioners and researchers with expertise in ASD and developmental disabilities that was convened in Marina del Rey, California, in October 2010. A working group (detailed in the Methods section) completed a literature review that informed the recommendations by the panel at the meeting; these recommendations were further refined by an updated review that was completed in December 2013. The panel reached consensus on the following key question: “What are the earliest signs and symptoms of ASD in children aged ≤24 months that can be used for early identification?”

Methods

The Early Identification working group comprised Drs Stone, Yirmiya (co-chairs), Chawarska, Estes, Hansen, McPartland, and Natowicz. The working group co-chairs and panel co-chairs (Drs Zwaigenbaum and Bauman) conducted a literature search on PubMed to identify relevant articles on early features of ASD. The PubMed search was conducted on June 30, 2010, and used the search terms (“child developmental disorders, pervasive” or “autistic disorder/” or autism [tw] or autistic [tw]) and (“early detection” or “early diagnosis”), with the age filter “infant, birth–23 months” and limited to English-language papers. This search yielded 341 references, which were reviewed by Drs Zwaigenbaum and Bauman, who selected articles that focused on studies examining the relationship between early behavioral or biological markers in the first 24 months of life and ASD diagnosis. The search results were complemented by additional publications identified by working group members. Hence, although the search strategy was comprehensive, selection of articles was not systematic, which is an important limitation. A scoping approach, with some discretion by the multidisciplinary expert working group, was used instead to select articles of highest relevance and methodologic quality.

Members of the working group reviewed the articles and evaluated their methodologic quality. In the absence of a standard evaluative tool for such research (eg, Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation, 21 which was used to evaluate the quality of evidence of clinical intervention trials), assessment of evidence quality focused on study design (retrospective versus prospective), measurement (eg, use of validated measures for both risk factors and diagnostic outcomes), and whether diagnostic outcomes were measured blinded to risk factor status. The working group also took into consideration whether findings were replicated across independent laboratories. Panel recommendations were based on this evaluative framework. During the conference, the working group offered draft recommendations for discussion, modification, and ratification by all attendees. Electronic voting was used to express opinions and guide consensus building. A modified nominal group technique was used to review the recommendations, with consensus reached by ≥1 round of voting. The consensus statements and discussion were summarized as draft proceedings of the conference, which were subsequently edited by all participants. Some of the statements provided here are intended to summarize the state of the literature, whereas others are in the form of recommendations for research needed to fill important gaps or aimed at addressing issues critical for clinical practice.

To ensure that the final article reflected recent literature, the search was updated by using the same strategy to add articles published to December 31, 2013; this search yielded an additional 202 references. Evidence tables and text references were updated with findings from prospective studies on early behavioral or biological markers. The working group reviewed and approved the final wording of the summary and recommendations.

Summary Statements

Statement 1: Evidence indicates substantial heterogeneity in the presentation and natural history of clinical features associated with ASDs. This heterogeneity has ramifications for the interpretation of research literature as well as for clinical practice.

There is heterogeneity not only in the etiology, neurobiology, onset, and course of core clinical ASD symptoms but also in the rates and levels of cognitive and language development, adaptive functioning, and co-morbidity with other disorders. Given the tremendous clinical diversity evident among subjects with ASD across the life span, it is not surprising to find that early manifestations and developmental course vary as well. Some children with ASDs are described as having behavioral differences (eg, in reactivity and social orienting) from the earliest months of life, whereas others present with speech delay in the second year, and still others are described as becoming withdrawn and losing skills after a period of relatively typical development into the second year of life. 22 24 There is also heterogeneity at an etiologic level, with hundreds of susceptibility genes 25 and potentially a wide range of environmental and/or epigenetic factors 26 28 implicated in ASD causation. The variability in behavioral profiles, developmental course, and underlying etiologic factors must be taken into account when synthesizing findings across studies. There are also methodologic differences that may affect comparability across studies (eg, prospective versus retrospective designs, measurement strategies, ages at which early signs and outcomes are examined).

Statement 2: There is evidence that reduced levels of social attention and social communication, as well as increased repetitive behavior with objects, are early markers of ASD between 12 and 24 months of age. Additional potential markers include abnormal body movements and temperament dysregulation.

ASD is not commonly diagnosed until 3 to 4 years of age. 29 However, many parents express concerns to their pediatrician by the time their child is aged 18 months. 30 , 31 In addition to parent reports, potential early markers have been identified according to retrospective analyses of home videos and prospective longitudinal studies of infants in the general population, as well as assessment of high-risk infants and toddlers who have an older sibling with ASD.

Studies directed toward the identification of early clinical diagnostic markers of ASD have examined atypicalities in the core domains of social communication and social interaction, as well as the presence of repetitive behaviors. There is strong evidence (ie, replication in multiple samples by independent groups) to support impairments in social attention and social communication as potential markers of ASD between the ages of 12 and 24 months (Table 1) 32 56 as well as evidence for atypical object use during this same age period. 57 , 58

TABLE 1.

Selected Studies of Potential Markers Identifying ASD in Children Aged 12 to 24 Months

Reference Findings Type of Study Sample Ascertainment Outcome Diagnosis Comments
Social/emotional behavioral markers (from studies with outcome assessments)
 Bryson et al, 32 2007 At ages 12–24 mo, 2 broadly defined ASD subgroups: Prospective case series 9 SIBS-A later diagnosed with ASD Recruited from multidisciplinary autism diagnostic and treatment centers Gold standard diagnostic assessment for ASD at age 36 mo using ADI-R, ADOS, and DSM-IV-TR criteria
Clinical diagnosis of ASD for 4 children at 24 mo and for 3 children at 30 mo Assessments every 6 mo from age 6 to 24 mo, including:
• Decrease in IQ from average/near average to severe cognitive impairment, with ASD signs emerging earlier or more striking earlier • AOSI and/or ADOS to assess for ASD symptoms
• Continued average or near average IQ • BSID-II to assess cognition
In all 9 subjects, social-communicative impairments coexisted with atypical sensory or motor behaviors and temperament profile of irritability/distress and difficulties with self- or other-regulation of state • CDI-WG to assess gestural and early language development
• Infant Temperament Scale or Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire to assess temperament
• Semi-structured interviews regarding parental concerns
 Fodstad et al, 33 2009 At ages 17–37 mo: Prospective • 161 children with AD Enrollees in state-funded early intervention program for children with development delay or medical condition likely to result in a developmental delay Diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS based on DSM-IV-TR criteria, M-CHAT, and Battelle Developmental Inventory–2nd edition • BISCUIT–Part 1 (20-item social/nonverbal communication subscale and 7-item communication subscale) to assess autism symptoms in one-to-one parent interviews along with child observation
• Deficits in communications and social skills more obvious and pronounced in AD versus other groups • 140 children with PDD-NOS
• Greater deficits in PPD-NOS versus controls • 585 controls: children at risk for other developmental delays or to have disorder such as Down syndrome and cerebral palsy
• 5 of 20 items on socialization/nonverbal communication subscale discriminated between groups a
• 5 of 7 items on communication subscale were most predictive of diagnostic group b Aged 17–37 mo
 Garon et al, 34 2009 At 24 mo, children with later diagnosis of ASD were distinguished from non-ASD siblings and controls by: Prospective • 138 high-risk infants (SIBS-A) Enrolled in larger study; recruited through autism diagnostic and treatment centers Gold standard diagnostic assessment for ASD at 36 mo based on ADI-R, ADOS, DSM-IV-TR criteria • TBAQ-R to assess temperament (parent report) at 24 mo
• Temperament profile (“effortful emotion regulation”) of lower positive affect, higher negative affect, and difficulty controlling attention and behavior Ongoing (N = 211 with data collected at age 24 mo) • 73 low-risk infants (no family history of ASD) 34 of 138 SIBS-A (24.6%) diagnosed with ASD • MSEL at 24 and 36 mo
• Lower sensitivity to social reward cues (low “behavioral approach”) 84% and 80%, respectively, enrolled in study at age 6 mo; rest by age 12 mo
Two ASD subgroups distinguished by number of ASD symptoms, IQ, age at diagnosis, Behavioral Approach profile
 Goldberg et al, 35 2005 At ages 14–19 mo, significant group differences in 3 of 4 social and communication behaviors between ASD and TD children but not between ASD and SIBS-A children: Prospective • 8 children diagnosed with AD or PPD-NOS aged 21.0–33.0 mo From larger sample of families participating in autism study at university medical center; controls from community volunteer sample For subjects recruited as part of larger study, ADI-R and ADOS-G were administered; controls were screened by using CARS ESCS (abridged version) structured interactions videotaped and coded to assess social interaction, joint attention, and behavioral regulation
• Responses to social interaction bids: less frequent eye contact, gestures, and turn-taking (P < .05) • 8 SIBS-A aged 14.0–19.0 mo
• Initiation of joint attention: fewer nonverbal behaviors to initiate shared experiences of objects or events (P < .001) • 9 children with TD from families without ASD aged 10.0–19.0 mo
• Differences in frequency of requesting behaviors (P < .05)
• No significant differences in responses to JA
 Mosconi et al, 36 2009 In 2-y-olds with AD: Longitudinal MRI study • 50 children with AD Children with AD recruited after receiving clinical diagnosis or while awaiting clinical evaluation Diagnosis of AD confirmed at 2 y and reassessed at 4 y by using ADI-R, ADOS, DSM-IV criteria; controls screened with CARS • ADOS sessions videotaped and coded by using a new scale (SOC-RS) to rate social orienting and communications behaviors, including IJA, RJA, and nonverbal gestures
• Amygdala enlargement that persisted through age 4 y (16% larger volumes than in controls) Ongoing • 33 controls (11 with developmental delay and 22 with TD) Details of subject ascertainment described in Hazlett et al, 37 2005 • Repetitive Behavior Scale–Revised to assess specificity of hypothesized relationship between amygdala and JA, investigated only in children with AD
Aged 2 y (18–35 mo)
 Nadig et al, 38 2007 At age 12 mo, failure to respond to name is highly suggestive of developmental abnormality Prospective longitudinal study • 101 at-risk infants (SIBS-A) Enrollees in university-based study Clinical best diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS at 24 mo based on clinical observation, ADOS, and DSM-IV criteria • Subjects followed up to age 36 mo
• 86% of at-risk infants responded to first or second name call vs 100% of controls Ongoing • 46 infants at no known risk (SIBS-TD) 12 children who failed to respond to name at 12 mo have outcome data: AD and PDD-NOS in 5, other delays in 4, TD in 3 • MSEL to assess development
• Failure to respond to name at age 12 mo highly specific for 24-mo outcome of developmental delay, including ASD Aged 12 mo • ADOS to measure social and communicative behaviors
• Response-to-name experimental task videotaped at 6 and 12 mo and coded for number of calls it took for response to child’s name
 Osterling et al, 39 2002 At 1 y, infants with ASD: Retrospective video study • 20 infants with later diagnosis of AD (35% of sample) or PPD-NOS Recruited from university subject pools, state autism society, Division of Developmental Disabilities, local newspaper and local radio advertisements, local schools AD or PPD-NOS diagnosis confirmed at study entry on basis of DSM-III-R plus score of ≥30 on CARS Home videos of first birthday parties coded by blind raters on frequencies of specific social and communicative behaviors and repetitive motor actions
• Look at others less frequently • 14 infants later diagnosed with MR (without ASD and without distinguishing physical anomalies)
• Orient to their names less frequently than infants with mental retardation: • 20 infants with TD
At 1 y, infants with ASD or MR: Aged 2.5–10 y at time of study
• Use gestures less frequently
• Look to objects held by others less frequently
• Engage in repetitive motor actions more frequently than infants with TD
 Ozonoff et al, 22 2010 By age 12 mo, significant differences between ASD and TD groups in frequency of gaze to faces and directed vocalizations (although not at 6 mo) Prospective longitudinal study • 25 high-risk infants with later diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS (22 were SIBS-A) Sample drawn from larger longitudinal study Classification as ASD or TD at 36 mo by using Baby Siblings Research Consortium definitions (ADOS and DSM-IV-TR criteria for AD or PDD-NOS) Assessments at ages 6, 12, 18, 24, and 36 mo of:
By 18 mo, significant group differences on all social communication variables (see Comments) • 25 gender-matched SIBS-TD determined later to have TD • Frequencies of 6 social communication behaviors (gaze to faces, gaze to objects, smiles, nonverbal vocalizations, single-word verbalizations, phrase vocalizations), recorded onto DVDs and coded during MSEL visual reception subtest
Between 6 and 18 mo: • Frequency of infant social engagement rated by blind examiners
• Declining trajectories of social communication behavior and loss of skills in most infants with later ASD diagnosis • MSEL to assess cognitive functioning
• Increase in cognitive and language skills (MSEL raw scores) over time in both groups, with significantly slower growth in ASD starting at 12 mo • Symptom onset by parent reports
• Discrepancy in autism symptom onset with parent reports
 Pierce et al, 40 2011 • Toddlers with an ASD as young as 14 mo spent significantly more time fixating on geometric images than on social images Prospective longitudinal study • 37 with ASD Sample recruited from general population screening approach starting at 12 mo and community referral Clinical best diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS at ≥24 mo based on clinical observation, ADOS, and DSM-IV criteria • 1-min eye-tracking test used to measure visual attention
• Individual visual fixation levels could be used to diagnostically classify a toddler as having ASD at 100% accuracy level • 22 with DD • Diagnosis confirmed by experienced diagnostician blinded to eye-tracking test results
• Toddlers with ASD who visually examined geometric images exhibited significantly fewer saccades when looking at geometric images than other toddlers • 51 with TD • MSEL and VABS used to measure cognitive and adaptive behavior
 Sullivan et al, 41 2007 By age 14 mo, children later diagnosed with ASD or broader autism phenotype had deficits in RJA Prospective 51 high-risk children (SIBS-A) who received outcome assessments: Subsample of subjects of large study; recruited through ASD advocacy group, schools, word of mouth Outcome assessment at 30 (n = 7) or 36 (n = 44) mo; diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS based on ADOS, DSM-IV criteria; BAP = language and/or social delays without ASD diagnosis RJA bids were coded from videotapes of 3 measures administered at ages 14 and 24 mo:
Between 14 and 24 mo, children with ASD had minimal improvement in RJA, unlike with BAP and non-BAP • AD (n = 13) or PDD-NOS (n = 3) • “Look only” trials adapted from Butterworth and Jarrett, 1991 97
Significant (P < .01) group differences in RJA at 24 mo • BAP (n = 8) • Look + point trials from CSBS DP
RJA at 14 mo predicted ASD outcome • Non-BAP (n = 27) • RJA item from ADOS
 Wetherby et al, 42 2004 During second year of life, 9 prelinguistic behaviors serve as red flags to distinguish ASD from DD and TD: Prospective • 18 children with later diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS Recruited to larger study Best-estimate diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS made between ages 30 mo and 5 y based on MSEL; VABS, Interview Edition, Survey Form; and ADOS • Direct observation within standardized Behavior Sample, videotaped and scored by using standard CSBS DP procedures
• 7 related to social and communication symptoms c Ongoing • 18 with DD without ASD • Behavior Sample videotape recoded by using SORF scoring
• 2 related to repetitive movements with objects and/or body parts • 18 with TD
4 additional “red flags” differentiated ASD from TD but not from DD d Aged 13.0–26.9 mo at Behavior Sample
 Yoder et al, 43 2009 Later social impairment and later ASD diagnosis predicted by: Prospective longitudinal correlational design • 43 SIBS-A Participants of another study; SIBS-A recruited from university-based autism and speech-language programs and community agencies; SIBS-TD through birth record database and word of mouth ASD diagnosis assessed ∼1.5 y after study entry (ie, after age 30 mo) using ADOS, ADI-R, and DSM-IV-TR criteria • Hierarchical linear modeling to assess growth in early social skills (ie, RJA and WTC) as predictors of social impairment in SIBS-A
• Initial level of RJA (at age 15 mo) • 24 age-matched SIBS-TD who provided social outcome benchmarks ASD diagnosed in 6 (AD in 3 and PDD-NOS in 3) • RJA and WTC assessed at 4 time points 4 mo apart; RJA, WTC, and SBC assessed at time 5, 6 mo after fourth measurement (ie, after age 30 mo)
• Growth rate of weighted triadic communication (from ages 15–34 mo) Aged 12–23 mo at study entry • Weighted frequency of unprompted triadic communications derived from STAT
At 30 mo, delay in RJA and more general social skills in SIBS-A but large variation in social outcome scores • Experimental task described in Presmanes et al, 44 2007, to assess RJA
• SBC to assess social behaviors by parent report
Zwaigenbaum et al, 45 2005 By age 12 mo, infants with later diagnosis of autism may be distinguished from other siblings and controls by: Prospective longitudinal study • 65 SIBS-A Recruited mainly at age ≤6 mo from autism diagnostic and treatment programs; low-risk infants recruited from nurseries in same regions Formal independent diagnostic assessment at 36 mo based on DSM-IV criteria, ADI-R, and ADOS • AOSI at 6 and 12 mo to assess autism-specific behaviors
• Behavior markers e Ongoing (N = 88 followed up to age 24 mo) • 23 low-risk infants (no first- or second-degree relatives with ASD) Clinical diagnosis of ASD made at 24 mo in up to 7 SIBS-A who met DSM-IV criteria (confirmed by using ADI-R and ADOS) • ADIS (24 mo) to assess autism-related social communication impairments
• Prolonged latency to disengage visual attention (starting after 6 mo) “Roughly” matched by gender, birth order, and age; N values varied by assessment • Computerized visual orienting task (6 and 12 mo) to assess ability to disengage from 1 of 2 competing visual stimuli (attentional disengagement)
• Temperament profile f Available 12-mo outcome data for 65 SIBS-A: • IBQ (6 and 12 mo) and TBAQ (24 mo) to measure infant temperament
• Delayed expressive and receptive language • 7 with AD • MSEL-ABS edition and CDI-WG (12 mo) to assess language and cognitive development
• 12 with ASD classification on ADOS
• 46 without ASD
Social/emotional behavioral markers (from studies without outcome assessments)
 Macari et al, 46 2012 At age 12 mo, lack of attention-sharing behaviors, including: Prospective • 53 SIBS-A Recruited from university research programs, Web site, advertising, word of mouth Provisional CBE diagnosis at age 24 mo • ADOS-T administered at 12 and 24 mo and analyzed by using nonparametric decision-tree learning algorithms
• Showing • 31 low-risk infants (no ASD history in first- or second-degree relative) Diagnosis not done at 36 mo • MSEL to assess developmental status
• IJA 64% male, enrolled by age 6 mo
Can predict ASD or other atypical development at age 24 mo
Various combinations of social communicative features associated with ASD presentation at 24 mo
 Presmanes et al, 44 2007 In second year of life, SIBS-A showed impaired RJA relative to SIBS-TD across range of prompt types Prospective • 46 SIBS-A SIBS-A recruited from regional multidisciplinary evaluation and speech language centers, state birth-to-3 service network, autism parent groups, and university-based autism service and outreach program; SIBS-TD recruited from birth records “Not yet available” • RJA task administered with different combinations of verbal and nonverbal cues; when number of attentional cues was increased; and where multiple objects or events competed for child’s attention
Attention disengagement similar for 2 groups Ongoing • 35 SIBS-TD • 20 RJA trials per subject were videotaped and coded
Aged 12–23 mo • MSEL cognitive scales administered
• STAT to assess social communicative behaviors
 Stone et al, 47 2007 At ages 12–23 mo, weaker performance by SIBS-A in: Prospective • 64 SIBS-A SIBS-A recruited from university-based autism and speech-language programs and community agencies; SIBS-TD recruited from birth record database, university-based research programs, and community agencies Not known Child-based and parent report measures:
• Nonverbal problem solving (ie, visual reception) • 42 SIBS-TD • MSEL to assess cognitive function
• Directing attention (ie, IJA) Aged 12–23 mo (mean: 16 mo) • CARS to assess autism symptoms
• Understanding words and phrases • STAT to assess play, requesting, directing attention, and motor imitation
• Gesture use • MCDI questionnaire to assess verbal and nonverbal understanding and expression
• Social communicative interactions with parents • DAISI to assess social engagement behaviors
Increased autism symptoms in SIBS-A
Significant correlations between child-based measures and parent reports
 Yirmiya et al, 48 2006 At age 14 mo, SIBS-A: Prospective (N = 61 with 14-mo assessments) • 30 SIBS-A Sibs-A recruited from treatment centers, special schools, and contacts with families of children with ASD At age 14 mo, 1 subject was suspected of having autism, a diagnosis confirmed at ages 24 and 36 mo by using ADI-R and ADOS-G Measures at age 4 and 14 mo:
• Initiated fewer nonverbal requesting gestures • 31 SIBS-TD Comparison group recruited from hospital maternity wards • BSID-II to assess general development and language
• Achieved lower language scores (accounted for by 5-mo language delay in 6 SIBS-A) than SIBS-TD Matched on 1-to-1 basis according to chronologic age, gender, birth order, number of children in family, and Bayley mental and motor scales • ICQ to assess maternal perception of infant temperament
Communication and cognition measures at age 14 mo:
In general, most SIBS-A functioned well • ESCS to assess nonverbal communication skills
• CHAT to assess JA behavior
Language/communication behavioral markers (from studies with outcome assessments)
 Landa and Garrett-Mayer, 49 2006 By 14 mo of age, significantly worse performance in fine motor, gross motor, and receptive and expressive language (ASD versus unaffected) Prospective longitudinal • 24 children with later diagnosis of ASD SIBS-A recruited through Autism Society of America local chapters and university-based center for autism; children at low autism risk recruited through local physician offices and caregiver/child play groups ASD classified at 24 mo based on PLS, ADOS, and MCDI • MSEL to assess general and language development across 5 domains of nonsocial development (gross motor, fine motor, visual reception, receptive and expressive language); administered as close as possible to ages 6, 14, and 24 mo
By 24 mo, significantly worse performance in all 5 domains (ASD versus unaffected) and significantly worse performance in gross motor, fine motor, and receptive language (ASD versus LD) • 11 with later diagnosis of language delay • PLS-III or PLS-IV at 24 mo for receptive and expressive standard scores and age equivalences
Slower developmental trajectory in ASD group versus other groups, with significant decline between first and second birthdays • 52 classified as unaffected (at 24 mo) • CDI at 14 and 24 mo to assess child’s understanding and production of language
Enrollees entered study as SIBS-A and infants with no family history of idiopathic autism
 Oller et al, 50 2010 From ∼1 y of age, vocalizations from children with autism or language delay can be differentiated from children with TD by automated analysis of selected acoustic features Prospective • 77 children aged 16–48 mo with AD or PDD-NOS Recruited through advertisements Phase I: children with reported diagnosis of language delay evaluated by speech-language clinician. Phase II: documentation of diagnosis from outside clinician, plus failure on M-CHAT • Phase I (2006–2008) and Phase II (2009) studies to assess automated method to determine presence or absence of 12 acoustic parameters on child vocalizations; parameters reflect rhythmic/syllabic articulation and voice
• 49 children aged 10–44 mo with language delay without autism • Analysis of 1486 all-day recordings
• 106 children aged 10–48 mo with TD
Language/communication behavioral markers (from studies without outcome assessments)
 Gamliel et al, 51 2007 At 14 and 24 mo, cognitive and/or language delays (≥2 SDs below average on measures) in SIBS-A subsets, compared with other SIBS-A and SIBS-TD Prospective • 39 SIBS-A Recruited through treatment centers, special schools, national autism organization, families of children with autism; comparison group recruited from hospital maternity wards Not known • BSID-II at 4, 14, and 24 mo to assess development
At ages 14–36 mo, cognitive and/or language difficulties in 11 of 39 SIBS-A vs 2 SIBS-TD Ongoing (N = 78 with developmental trajectories to age 54 mo) • 39 SIBS-TD • RDLS (24 mo) to assess expressive and verbal comprehension
By 54 mo, cognitive differences gone, but some differences in language ability (receptive and expressive) remained Matched at age 4 mo on 1-to-1 basis according to chronologic age, gender, birth order, number of children in family, temperament profile, and mental and motor scales • K-ABC (36 and 54 mo) to assess intelligence
Most siblings with language impairments at age 14 mo functioning well at 54 mo without intervention • Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–Preschool (36 and 54 mo)
• Parent questionnaire regarding clinical and educational services (24, 36, and 54 mo)
• This is a follow-up report to Yirmiya et al, 48 2006
Markers of motor dysfunction (from studies with outcome assessments)
 Esposito and Venuti, 52 2008 At age ∼20 mo, boys with AD had: Retrospective video study • 16 boys mean age 20.2 mo with AD Recruited from referrals to university-based center for developmental disabilities AD diagnosis before study confirmed by using DSM-IV criteria and ADOS • Analysis of home videos taken after 6 mo of independent walking
• Different distributions in WOS scores from other groups • 10 boys mean age 21 mo with mental retardation • WOS used to code videos by assessing gait on 3 axes: foot, arm, and global movements
• Different gait patterns: problems with heel-to-toe pattern, more asymmetric posture of arm while walking, higher frequency of general movement anomalies (eg, “waddling walk”) • 16 boys mean age 20.5 mo with TD
AD and MR groups matched by chronological and developmental ages
 Esposito et al, 53 2011 Aged <2 y, significant (P ≤ .001) differences in gait pattern in toddlers with AD versus TD and DD: Retrospective video study • 20 toddlers with AD Recruited from 2 nationwide referral centers for developmental disabilities Clinical diagnosis of AD based on DSM-IV-R and confirmed by using ADI-R, ADOS-G, and CARS Analysis when toddlers were first walking without assistance
• Higher levels of postural asymmetry during walking • 15 toddlers with nonautistic DD • Home videos taken when child was aged <2 y (plus all footage within 2-h window)
• Some atypical arm and foot movements during walking • 20 toddlers with TD • WOS to code videos by assessing gait on 3 axes: foot, arm, and global movements
Mean ages 12.9–14.2 mo when first walking without assistance • PPSW to assess static and dynamical symmetry during gait
 Matson et al, 54 2009 At ages 17–37 mo: Prospective • 140 infants with AD Enrollees of state-funded early intervention program for DDs AD and PDD-NOS diagnoses based on DSM-IV-TR criteria, M-CHAT, and Battelle Developmental Inventory–Second Edition developmental profile • BISCUIT Part 1—specifically the repetitive behavior/ restricted interests subscale—to assess for ASD symptoms
• Stereotypies and repetitive/ritualistic behaviors were most common in AD, followed by PDD-NOS and then other DDS • 121 with PDD-NOS • Child observation and 1-to-1 parent assessment; administered as part of battery of assessments of physical and social development
• Striking differences across groups in limited number of interests, engages in repetitive motor movements for no reason, eye-to-eye gaze, maintains eye contact • 499 at risk for DD but no ASD diagnosis
• BISCUIT subset could accurately predict diagnostic group membership Aged 17–37 mo (mean: 26.63 mo)
 Ozonoff et al, 57 2008 At age 12 mo: Prospective • 35 SIBS-A Recruited from SIBS-A and SIBS-TD Diagnosis at 24 (n = 29) or 36 (n = 37) mo based on ADOS, SCQ, and DSM-IV criteria Manner in which infants explore objects was examined in task that afforded range of repetitive uses
• Atypical uses of objects (spinning, rotating, and, especially, unusual visual exploration of objects) significantly (P ≤ .005) more frequent in infants with ASD compared with other groups • 31 in SIBS-TD For 37 with outcome data at 24 and 36 mo, 31 (83.8%) had the same classifications at both ages; the later classification was used in analyses • Four objects presented to the infant, 1 at a time, for 30 s each
• Repetitive behaviors significantly related to cognitive and symptom status at 36 mo Aged 12 mo, with following outcomes: • Behavior recorded on DVD
• 9 with ASD diagnosis • 8 object uses coded by blinded raters as either frequency or duration; 4 hypothesized as typical and age-appropriate and 4 as atypical h
• 10 with later diagnosis of other delays g
• 47 with no concerns (did not meet criteria for other groups)
 Ozonoff et al, 55 2008 During first 2 y of age: Retrospective video study • 26 children aged 26–61 mo with nonregression-type AD 82 recruited from university-based recruitment core and local agencies serving individuals with developmental disabilities; 21 recruited in another city from ongoing studies and university subject pool Diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS in the community confirmed at study entry; AD diagnosis based on ADI-R plus ADOS; regression versus no regression subgroups based on ADI-R • Participants seen at initial enrollment (when all home videos of child from birth to age 2 y were collected) and 1–2 y later for assessment battery that was part of another study
• Neither regression nor nonregression types of AD differed from TD in rates of movement abnormalities or lack of protective responses • 28 children aged 26–61 mo with regression-type AD • Videos coded for motor maturity, protective responses, and movement abnormalities by using Infant Motor Maturity and Atypicality Coding Scales
• Toddlers with nonautistic DD displayed higher rates of movement abnormalities in sitting and prone and fewer protective responses in crawling than other groups • 25 with nonautistic DD matched for chronologic and developmental age
• 24 with TD
 Provost et al, 56 2007 At ages 21–41 mo: Prospective • 19 children aged 21–41 mo with ASD Recruited from referrals to university-based early childhood evaluation program for developmental disabilities ASD diagnosis made by study team evaluation • BSID-II Motor Scale to assess motor skills
• Motor dysfunction to some degree in all children with ASD (delay in gross and/or fine motor skills) • 19 with other DD including motor delay Diagnosis of AD in 18, PDD-NOS in 1 • PDMS-2 also to assess presence and degree of delay in motor skills
• Significant motor impairments versus those with developmental concerns without motor delay • 18 with developmental concerns (eg, speech and emotional issues) but no motor delay
• No significant difference in motor scores versus children with DD ASD and DD groups matched on gender, cognitive abilities within 3 mo; ASD matched to DD and no motor delay groups on chronologic age within 3 mo
 Watt et al, 58 2008 At ages 18–24 mo, children with ASD demonstrated significantly higher frequency and longer duration of: Prospective • 50 children with ASD (AD or PDD-NOS) Recruited through screening project involving general population sample; ASD and DD participants were in bottom 10th percentile on ≥1 composite of CSBS Behavior Sample during second year of life Best-estimate assessment of ASD or DD diagnosis at age ≥30 mo based on battery of tests including ADOS In second year of life:
• RSB with objects • 25 with developmental delay without ASD • CSBS Behavior Sample to assess communication behaviors; RSB coded from Behavior Sample videotapes
• RSB with body • 50 with TD • Symbolic composite of Behavior Sample to measure developmental level
• Sensory behaviors than DD and TD groups Aged 18–24 mo; DD group matched groupwise to ASD group on age and developmental level; TD group matched individually to ASD group on gender and chronologic age • Social composite of Behavior Sample to assess social competence
In fourth year:
In children with ASD and those with other DDs, RSB with objects predicted developmental outcomes and severity of ASD symptoms at 3 y • MSEL to measure developmental level
• VABS to assess adaptive behavior
• ADOS to measure autism symptoms

Studies may evaluate markers in >1 category (see “Comments”). AD, autistic disorder; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ADOS-G, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic; ADOS-T, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Toddler Module; AOSI, Autism Observation Scale for Infants; BAP, broader autism phenotype; BISCUIT, Baby and Infant Scale for Children with Autism Traits; BSID-II, Bayley Scales of Infant Development–2nd Edition; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; CBE, clinical best estimate; CDI-WG, MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories–Words and Gestures; CSBS DP, Communication and Behavior Scales Developmental Profile; CHAT, Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; DAISI, Detection of Autism by Infant Sociability Interview; DD, developmental delay; DSM-III-R, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition; DSM-IV-TR, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision; ESCS, Early Social Communication Scales; IBQ, Infant Behavior Questionnaire; JA, joint attention; K-ABC, Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children; LD, language delay; MR, mental retardation; PDD-NOS, pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified; PDMS-2, Peabody Development Motor Scales, Second Edition; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; PPSW, Positional Pattern for Symmetry during Walking; RDLS, Reynell Developmental Language Scales; RSB, repetitive and stereotyped behavior; SBC, Social Behavior Checklist; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SIBS-A, siblings of children with ASD; SIBS-TD, siblings of children with typical development; SOC-RS, Social Orienting Continuum and Response Scale; SORF, Systematic Observation of Red Flags for ASD in Young Children; STAT, Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year-Olds; TBAQ, Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire; TBAQ-R, Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire–Revised; TD, typical development; VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales; WOS, Walking Observation Scale; WTC, weighted triadic communications (child-initiated communication of message about an object or event to another person, also called IJA but considered to have imperative as well as declarative functions).

a

Fodstad et al 2009 33 : Socialization/nonverbal communication subscale items that discriminated: intellectual abilities; shares enjoyment, interests, or achievement with others; interest in participating in social games, sports, and activities; use of too few or too many social gestures; and development of social relationships.

b

Fodstad et al 2009 33 : Communication subscale items that were predictive: age-appropriate self-help and adaptive skills; use of language to communicate; use of language in conversations with others; communicates effectively; and language development.

c

Wetherby et al 2004 42 : Red flags differentiating ASD from both DD and TD: lack of appropriate gaze; lack of warm, joyful expressions with gaze; lack of sharing enjoyment or interest; lack of response to name; lack of coordination of gaze, facial expression, gesture, and sound; lack of showing; and unusual prosody.

d

Wetherby et al 2004 42 : Red flags differentiating ASD from TD but not DD were lack of response to contextual cues, lack of pointing, lack of vocalizations with consonants, and lack of playing with a variety of toys conventionally.

e

Zwaigenbaum et al 2005 45 : Behavioral markers distinguishing autism by age 12 months were atypical eye contact, visual tracking, disengagement of visual attention, orienting to name, imitation, social smiling, reactivity, social interest and affect, and sensory-oriented behaviors.

f

Zwaigenbaum et al 2005 45 : Temperament marked by marked passivity and decreased activity level at 6 mo, followed by extreme stress reactions, tendency to fixate on particular objects in environment, and decreased expression of positive affect by 12 mo.

g

Ozonoff et al 2008 57 : Other delays were general developmental delay, speech/language delay, marked hyperactivity, and marked anxiety.

h

Ozonoff et al 2008 57 : Behaviors hypothesized as typical: shaking, banging, mouthing, throwing; behaviors hypothesized as atypical: spinning, rolling, rotating, unusual visual exploration (eg, engages in prolonged visual inspection, examines object from odd angles or peripheral vision).

When concern about any of these behaviors is conveyed by parents or observed by other care professionals (eg, a health care provider such as community physician or nurse, developmental service provider, or early childhood educator), it is recommended that the child be referred for further autism screening and, as appropriate, for a more comprehensive developmental and diagnostic evaluation.

Early marker: reduced levels of social attention and social communication

Social attention and social communication behaviors indicative of ASD include decreased response to one’s name being called (ie, “orienting to name”), reduced visual attention to socially meaningful stimuli, and less frequent use of joint attention and communicative gestures.

Reduced orienting to name is frequently identified by parents of children with ASD as 1 of their earliest concerns, 8 , 59 and it has been identified in several prospective studies of at-risk infants as a robust early marker of the diagnosis. 32 , 38 , 42 , 45 There is some evidence to suggest that decreased orienting to name can differentiate children with a later ASD diagnosis not only from typically developing children but also from children with other developmental delays/disabilities. 42 , 60

Toddlers with ASD also exhibit a reduced tendency to visually examine socially meaningful stimuli. Eye-tracking technology provides a unique opportunity to understand visual attention in ASD and can accurately measure point of gaze with <1 degree of error. Studies of toddlers with ASD report reduced monitoring of social scenes even with an explicit dyadic cue (ages 13–25 months) 61 and a preference for visually examining geometric shapes rather than images of children (ages 14–42 months). 40

Attention in ASD is often abnormal not just in terms of what toddlers with ASD prefer to look at but also how they attend to their world. “Joint attention” refers to the development of specific skills that enable sharing attention with others through pointing, showing, and coordinating looks between objects and people; joint attention skills are associated with language acquisition. 47 Some studies have reported group differences in both responding to joint attention (RJA) and initiating joint attention (IJA) between infants who receive a later diagnosis of ASD and children who are not diagnosed. RJA, also called attention following, is indicated by the child’s shifting of attention in response to a cue, such as someone’s gaze, head turn, point, or attention-directing utterance. Lower levels of RJA have been reported in children with ASD as early as 14 months in 2 prospective studies 41 , 43 but not in a third. 35 In 1 study, 41 group differences in RJA between high-risk siblings with a later ASD diagnosis and high-risk siblings with later outcomes of broader autism phenotype or non–broader autism phenotype became apparent at 24 months of age.

IJA behaviors, or directing attention, refer to a child’s integration of gestures, gaze shifts, utterances, and other cues to initiate a shared experience of objects or events with others. As early as age 14 months, IJA behaviors have been found to be impaired in children with ASD 35 and younger siblings of children with ASD 47 compared with typically developing children. Reduced IJA (at 18 months) also distinguishes younger siblings who subsequently develop ASD from those who do not, 62 as does a slower growth trajectory of IJA-related communication from age 15 months. 43 Other studies have assessed the use of gestures more generally. During the second year of life, a lower frequency of gesture use differentiated children with ASD from typically developing children 35 , 63 and from children with other developmental disorders. 33 , 39

Early marker: repetitive behavior with objects

As early as 12 months of age, infants with a later diagnosis of ASD were found to exhibit atypical use of objects, such as the spinning, lining up, rotating, and especially visual exploration of objects, compared with infants with a later diagnosis of other developmental or language delays or no developmental concerns. 57 These findings are consistent with other reports of repetitive behaviors associated with object use 42 , 58 , 64 and prolonged visual fixation on objects 32 or repetitive geometric shapes 40 in infants who subsequently develop ASD. In 2 samples, such repetitive behaviors correlated with subsequent diagnostic outcomes and other ASD symptoms. 57 , 58 , 64

Potential early marker: atypical body movements and motor development

Evidence in this domain is less well established, but research suggests that atypicalities in body movements, which can encompass repetitive actions or posturing of the body, arms, hands, or fingers (including hand flapping, finger flicking, and atypical arm and foot movements during walking), may emerge as important early markers. Whether these atypical behaviors are noted to emerge early or late during the second year of life seems to vary depending on the design of the study.

Prospective studies in children with a later diagnosis of ASD have shown a higher frequency and longer duration of repetitive stereotyped movements 58 , 64 compared with typically developing or “unaffected” children, respectively. Similar findings have been reported in other prospective studies 32 , 42 , 54 , 65 as well as in retrospective studies. 39 , 52 , 53 In contrast, 1 retrospective video study of children with autistic disorder found no differences from typically developing children in rates of movement abnormalities. 55

There is a growing interest as to whether atypicalities in developmental motor patterns may appear very early and possibly predate social and communication markers. General delays in gross and/or fine motor skills have been reported in high-risk infants, 49 , 66 and more recent research has suggested very early emerging abnormalities in motor control. For example, in a preliminary study of 40 high-risk infant siblings, Flanagan et al 67 reported that head lag at 6 months was predictive of a subsequent diagnosis of ASD at 30 to 36 months. In a related study, 68 motor delays at 6 months were predictive of social communication delays across the high-risk cohort. Bolton et al 69 reported that fine motor behaviors were among a larger set of parent-report items on a general developmental screening tool that was informative for risk of ASD at 6 months of age. Although these studies suggest that, in some cases, delayed or atypical motor patterns may be predictive of ASD, definitive markers are not yet available. Certainly, children with atypical motor development should be closely monitored and followed up, not only for ASD but also for other developmental disorders. Further studies of the association between infant motor development and ASD risk are warranted.

Potential early marker: temperamental profile

It has been reported that by 24 months of age, temperament profiles can distinguish high-risk siblings with a later diagnosis of ASD from high-risk siblings who do not receive an ASD diagnosis and siblings without a family history of ASD. 34 One profile is characterized by lower sensitivity to social reward cues. A second profile, marked by negative affect and difficulty in controlling attention and behavior, can differentiate siblings diagnosed later with ASD from infants with no family history of ASD (low-risk infants). Two smaller case series by the same group identified temperamental differences in children with ASD as early as age 6 months. 32 , 45 Clifford et al 70 reported on the presence of reduced positive affect and increased perceptual sensitivity at 7 months of age, as well as a pervasive pattern of emotional dysregulation and reduced attentional flexibility (consistent with findings from Garon et al 34 ), features that were predictive of ASD within a cohort of 54 high-risk infants. Measures of temperament have not yet been reported by other groups investigating infants at risk for autism, suggesting that these observations need further study. In addition, temperamental features in low-risk populations have not been investigated as a potential risk marker for ASD.

Statement 3: Reliable behavioral markers for ASD in children aged <12 months have not yet been consistently identified.

Many factors limit investigations into the earliest age at which markers for ASD can be identified, including: (1) the presence of considerable individual differences and variability in cognitive and social development in young infants; (2) the use of study designs that limit conclusions about whether differences are predictive of an ASD diagnosis and/or are specific to ASD; (3) the possibility that behavioral symptoms used in diagnosis are associated with neuronal circuitry that develops after 12 months of age; and (4) the possibility that early, prodromal symptoms at the time of ASD diagnosis may differ from behaviors observed and measured later in development.

Table 2 summarizes studies in which emerging markers over the first 12 months of life were assessed. 22 , 32 , 38 , 45 , 46 , 48 , 49 , 71 78 Some researchers reported no behavioral differences at the age of 6 months in social communication behaviors 22 or in language or motor development 49 , 66 between infants who were later diagnosed with ASD and those with a later diagnosis of typical development. Other studies, which have also included outcome measures, suggest that there may be differences during the age range of 6 to 12 months in social attention (social gaze or orienting to name being called), 32 , 74 atypical sensory behaviors, 32 repetitive or otherwise atypical motor behaviors, and nonverbal communication (differences in gesture use). 77 Additional similar studies during the first 6 months of life have suggested differences in responding to social stimuli 72 and “at least some suggestion of” more difficult temperaments, characterized by marked irritability, intolerance to intrusions, and being prone to distress/negative affect. 32

TABLE 2.

Selected Studies of Potential Markers Identifying ASD in Infants Aged 12 Months

First Author and Year of Publication Findings Type of Study Sample Ascertainment Outcome Diagnosis Comments
Social/emotional behavioral markers (from studies with outcome assessment)
 Bryson et al, 32 2007 Between ages 6 and 12 mo, in subset of siblings with change in cognitive development between 12 and 24 mo, 5 of 6 infants were “more difficult to engage socially” (“less … eye contact, no or very little social smiling, and little interest or pleasure in interacting with others”); minimal exploration of toys; atypical sensory behavior (striking visual fixation); repetitive/atypical motor behaviors Prospective case series 9 SIBS-A later diagnosed with ASD Recruited from multidisciplinary autism diagnostic and treatment centers Gold standard diagnostic assessment for ASD at age 36 mo by using ADI-R, ADOS, and DSM-IV-TR criteria Assessments every 6 mo from age 6–24 mo, including:
Clinical diagnosis of ASD for 4 children at 24 mo and for 3 at 30 mo • AOSI and/or ADOS to assess for ASD symptoms
• BSID-II to assess cognition
• CDI-WG to assess gestural and early language development
• Infant Temperament Scale or Toddler Behavior Assessment Questionnaire to assess temperament
Semi-structured interviews regarding parental concerns
 Maestro et al, 71 2005 • Between ages 0 and 6 mo, significant group differences in social attention (high scores in social versus nonsocial stimuli in “typical” infants) Retrospective video study • 15 children aged 3.5–5.2 y with AD diagnosis Subjects with AD recruited from community sources referred to public academic hospital; controls were kindergarten attendees Diagnosis made at study entry through symptom checklist based on DSM-IV plus ≥30 score on CARS • From each group, home movies lasting at least 10 min coded by blinded observers for frequency of behaviors via an 8-item “grid” for assessment of social and nonsocial attention
• Between ages 7 and 12 mo, no group differences in social or nonsocial attention; but behaviors regarding attention to nonsocial stimuli increased in both AD and typical groups but “more evident” in the former • 13 “typical” children with mean age of 4.7 y • Social attention behaviors assessed: looking at people, orienting toward people, smiling at people, vocalizing to people
• Nonsocial attention behaviors assessed: looking at objects, orienting toward objects, smiling at objects, vocalizing to objects
 Maestro et al, 72 2002 Between ages 0 and 6 mo, significant group differences in social attention and social behavior, including: Retrospective video study • 15 children aged 3.5–5.6 (mean: 4.1) y with diagnosis of AD (n = 7) or PPD-NOS (n = 8) Controls were kindergarten attendees Diagnosis made at study entry through symptom checklist based on DSM-IV plus score of ≥30 on CARS • Home movies lasting at least 10 min for each subject during age 0–6 mo were rated by blinded observers for frequency of behaviors by using 13-item “grid” covering 3 developmental areas of social attention (eg, looking at people), social behavior (eg, anticipating the other’s aim), and nonsocial attention (eg, “explorative activity with object”)
• Less frequent looking at people (P < .001) • 15 “typical” “normal” children with mean age of 4.7 y
• Less frequent vocalizing to people (P < .001) Matched for gender and age in home videos
• Less frequent orienting toward people (P > .01)
No group differences in items referring to interest and attention versus nonsocial stimuli
 Macari et al, 46 2012 At 12 mo, 7 ADOS-T items optimized classification of children with and without ASD at 24 mo Prospective longitudinal study • 53 at-risk infants (SIBS-A); 13 diagnosed with ASD at 24 mo Recruited from multiple sources (existing research programs, Web site, advertising, and word of mouth) Clinical best diagnosis of ASD at 24 mo based on developmental and medical history, developmental and language assessments and ADOS, and DSM-IV criteria • Subjects assessed at 12 mo and followed up to 24 mo
• 11 of 13 children with ASD and 68 of 71 children without ASD correctly classified Ongoing • 31 infants at no known risk (SIBS-TD) • MSEL to assess development
These items included: level of engagement, amount of requesting, imitation, fussiness, showing, gestures, and intonation • ADOS-T to measure social and communicative behaviors
• Item-level analysis of ADOS-T, “decision tree” procedures to optimize prediction of ASD
 Nadig et al, 38 2007 At 6 mo, nonsignificant trend for controls to require fewer number of calls to respond to name Prospective longitudinal study • 55 at-risk infants (SIBS-A) Enrolled in university-based study Clinical best diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS at 24 mo based on clinical observation, ADOS, DSM-IV criteria • Subjects followed up to age 36 mo
• 82% of controls responded on first or second call of name vs. 66% of SIBS-A Ongoing • 43 infants at no known risk (SIBS-TD) • MSEL to assess development
Aged 6 mo • ADOS to measure social and communicative behaviors
• Response-to-name experimental task videotaped at 6 and 12 mo and coded for number of calls it took for response to child’s name
• Same sample as in Merin et al, 73 2007
 Ozonoff et al, 22 2010 At 6 mo: Prospective longitudinal study • 25 high-risk infants with later diagnosis of AD or PDD-NOS (22 were SIBS-A) Sample drawn from larger longitudinal study Classification as ASD or TD at 36 mo using Baby Siblings Research Consortium definitions (ADOS and DSM-IV-TR criteria for AD or PDD-NOS) Assessments at ages 6, 12, 18, 24, and 36 mo of:
• No group differences in social communication behaviors (including frequency of gaze to faces, shared smiles, and vocalizations to others) • 25 gender-matched SIBS-TD determined later to have TD • Frequencies of 6 social communication behaviors (gaze to faces, gaze to objects, smiles, nonverbal vocalizations, single-word verbalizations, phrase vocalizations), recorded onto DVDs and coded during MSEL Visual Reception subtest
• Better (NS) social communication behaviors for ASD versus TD outcome group “on all variables” • Frequency of infant social engagement rated by blind examiners
• MSEL to assess cognitive functioning
• Symptom onset by parent reports
 Werner et al, 74 2000 At 8–10 mo, significant (P < .05) main effect of diagnostic group for social behaviors, after children with late-onset ASD (n = 3) were removed from analysis Retrospective video study • 15 infants later diagnosed with AD (n = 8) or PPD-NOS (n = 7) Participants of earlier study plus additional recruits from university infant research pool Confirmation of AD or PPD-NOS based on DSM-III-R plus ≥30 score on CARS • Home videos between ages 8–10 mo coded for presence or absence of behaviors categorized as social (e.g., looking at others, orienting to name being called), communication (vocalizations), and repetitive
• Infants with ASD “much less likely” (P < .005) than infants with TD to orient when their name was called • 15 children with TD
 Young et al, 75 2009 • No infant in Merin et al, 73 2007, who showed abnormal gaze behavior (decreased eye contact) at 6 mo had any signs of autism at outcome Prospective • 33 high-risk infants (SIBS-A) Refer to Merin et al, 73 2007 (below) Clinical diagnosis of autism at 18 and/or 24 mo based on ADOS-G supplemented by M-CHAT and MSEL • Longitudinal follow-up for sample in Merin et al, 73 2007
• The 3 infants in sample who were diagnosed with autism by 24 mo did not exhibit abnormal gaze patterns at 6 mo and had typical affective responses at 6 mo • 25 infant SIBS-TD Clinical outcome data available on 49 infants • Assessment at 6 mo of eye-tracking data and behavioral data during live mother–infant interaction
• MSEL to assess development at 6, 12, 18, and 24 mo
• VABS to assess social, communication, and motor skills at 12, 18, and 24 mo
• CDI to assess language development at 18 and 24 mo
 Zwaigenbaum et al, 45 2005 At 6 mo, siblings with later diagnosis of ASD showed: Prospective longitudinal study • 44 SIBS-A Recruited mainly at age ≤6 mo from autism diagnostic and treatment programs; low-risk infants recruited from nurseries in same regions Formal independent diagnostic assessment at 36 mo based on DSM-IV criteria, ADI-R, and ADOS • AOSI at 6 and 12 mo to assess autism-specific behaviors
• No difference in number of risk markers measured by using AOSI Ongoing (N = 88 followed up to age 24 mo; 6-mo data available on 44 SIBS-A) • 15 low-risk infants (no first- or second- degree relatives with ASD) Clinical diagnosis of ASD made at 24 mo in up to 7 SIBS-A who met DSM-IV criteria (confirmed by using ADI-R and ADOS) • Computerized visual orienting task at 6 and 12 mo to assess ability to disengage from 1 of 2 competing visual stimuli (attentional disengagement)
• No difference (P = .12) in disengagement of visual attention “Roughly” matched according to gender, birth order, and age; N values varied by assessment • IBQ at 6 and 12 mo to measure infant temperament
• Temperament characterized by marked passivity and decreased activity level (P = .019) Available 6-mo outcome data for 44 SIBS-A: • MSEL and CDI-WG at 12 mo to assess language and cognitive development
Compared with non-ASD siblings and controls • 4 with AD
Behavioral observations at 6 mo do not predict later diagnosis • 8 with ASD classification on ADOS
• 32 without ASD
Social/emotional behavioral markers (from studies without outcome assessments)
 Merin et al, 73 2007 At 6 mo, diminished gaze to mother’s eyes relative to mouth (10 of 11 infants with this finding were in at-risk group) Prospective • 31 at-risk infants (SIBS-A) Recruited by using research institute database and word of mouth Not done • Visual fixation assessment during reciprocal social interaction, conducted at age 6 mo via eye tracking during modified still-face paradigm
• 24 with an older sibling without autism • Same sample as in Nadig et al, 38 2007
Noland et al, 76 2010 At 6.5–9 mo, higher working memory scores for SIBS-A versus SIBS-TD for nonsocial stimuli; no group difference for social stimuli Prospective • 25 SIBS-A SIBS-A recruited primarily through university-based service and ASD outreach program; SIBS-TD recruited from telephone contacts by using state birth record database Not done • Trials at age 6.5 mo and/or 9 mo involving tasks relating to orienting toward social and nonsocial targets (stimuli); correct response was infant gaze toward location where target most recently appeared
• 30 SIBS-TD • Trials videotaped, coded for correct first looks
Yirmiya et al, 48 2006 At 4 mo: Prospective • 21 dyads of mothers and infants who were SIBS-A Comparison group recruited from hospital maternity wards At age 14 mo, 1 subject was suspected of having autism, a diagnosis confirmed at ages 24 and 36 mo by using ADI-R and ADOS-G Measures at age 4 and 14 mo:
• No significant group difference in mother–infant synchrony, although SIBS-A exhibited weaker synchrony during infant-led interactions Ongoing (N = 42 with 4-mo assessments) • 21 dyads and infants who were SIBS-TD • BSID-II to assess general development and language
• No significant group difference in infant gaze behavior during still-face procedure but more neutral affect and less upset with SIBS-A Matched on 1-to-1 basis according to chronologic age, gender, birth order, number of children in family, and Bayley mental and motor scales • ICQ to assess maternal perception of infant temperament
• Significantly more SIBS-A responded to name being called by mother than SIBS-TD Social engagement measures at 4 mo:
• Synchrony during mother–child free play interaction
• Infant gaze and affect during still-face paradigm
• Procedure to assess responsiveness to name being called
Language/communication behavioral markers (from studies with outcome assessment)
 Colgan et al, 77 2006 At 9–12 mo, decreased variety in types of social interaction gestures used was significantly associated with autism status Retrospective video study • 21 children later diagnosed with AD Recruited through mailings to child care centers and developmental evaluation centers; parent advocacy group meetings; hospital clinics; university-based autism subject registry Clinical diagnosis of AD made according to time of study recruitment (preschool age), by using DSM-III-R or DSM-IV criteria and, for 11 subjects, score >30 on CARS Home videotapes from ages 9–12 mo collected; edited footage totaling 5 min and including social scenes, comparable across groups, were coded for use of gestures
• Other measures were not associated with an AD diagnosis: total number of social interaction gestures, number of child-initiated social interaction gestures • 14 children with TD • Only gestures defined as social interaction were used in this study (“gestural act used to attract or maintain attention of another for social purposes”; eg, waving hello or good-bye, shaking head yes or no)
Landa and Garrett-Mayer, 49 2006 At 6 mo, no significant group differences in domains of motor, visual reception, and language development Prospective longitudinal • 24 children with later diagnosis of ASD SIBS-A recruited through Autism Society of America local chapters and university-based center for autism; children at low autism risk recruited through local physician offices and caregiver-child play groups ASD classified at 24 mo based on PLS, ADOS, and CDI MSEL to assess general and language development across 5 domains of nonsocial development (gross motor, fine motor, visual reception, receptive and expressive language); administered as close as possible to ages 6, 14, and 24 mo
• 11 with later diagnosis of language delay
• 52 classified as unaffected (at 24 mo)
58 SIBS-A and infants with no family history of autism evaluated at 6 mo
Paul et al, 78 2011 At age 6–12 mo, group differences for certain prelinguistic vocal behaviors Prospective cross-sectional design • 28–38 high-risk infants (SIB-A) Recruited from university research pool; also referrals from local pediatric practices, local autism advocacy groups, word of mouth, advertising in parenting media Provisional diagnoses at 24 mo based on clinical observations, ADOS-T, and MSEL • Vocalization samples collected at age 6, 9, and 12 mo during play with mother and standard set of toys
• Significantly fewer speech-like vocalizations and more nonspeech vocalization Ongoing (N = 43 who have participated in 24-mo follow-up) • 20–31 low-risk infants (no sibling with ASD diagnosis) Of the 24 high-risk subjects who made a 24-mo visit: • Detailed analysis of vocal production (eg, for consonant inventory, presence of canonical syllables) and development of prespeech vocalization
• Significantly fewer consonant types • 7 with ASD • Discriminant function analyses included only children at high risk
• Significantly fewer canonical syllable shapes • 6 with symptoms without meeting full BAP criteria
Differences in vocal production in first year of life associated with “outcomes in terms of autistic symptoms” in second year for children at high risk • 1 with nonautistic developmental delay
• 10 without a clinical diagnosis

Studies may evaluate markers in >1 category (see “Comments”). AD, autistic disorder; ADI-R, Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised; ADOS, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule; ADOS-G, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic; ADOS-T, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Toddler Module; AOSI, Autism Observation Scale for Infants; BAP, broader autism phenotype; BSID-II, Bayley Scales of Infant Development–2nd edition; CARS, Childhood Autism Rating Scale; CDI-WG, MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories–Words and Gestures; DSM-III-R, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised; DSM-IV, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition; DSM-IV-TR, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision; DVD, digital video disc (high-capacity optical disk); IBQ, Infant Behavior Questionnaire; ICQ, Infant Characteristics Questionnaire; M-CHAT, Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers; PLS, Preschool Language Scale; PDD-NOS, pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified; SIBS-A, younger siblings of children with ASD; SIBS-TD, younger siblings of children with typical development; TD, typical development; VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales.

Jones and Klin 18 recently completed a landmark study that incorporated eye-tracking technology to assess a high-risk sibling sample. They reported that infants later diagnosed with ASD exhibited diminished orienting to the eye region of the face over time, specifically from 2 to 6 months of age. Cross-sectional group differences emerged later in the first year. However, these differences in orienting of visual attention, as measured by using the eye tracker, did not have straightforward behavioral correlates that were detected by either clinicians or parents.

Studies of younger siblings of children with ASD without a known diagnostic outcome have reported either no differences in specific social behaviors 48 or differences in visual fixation 73 ; orienting to nonsocial versus social stimuli 76 ; and prespeech vocalizations. 78 However, the predictive validity of these differences cannot be interpreted in the absence of outcome data.

To summarize, no definitive behavioral or diagnostic markers for ASD have yet been identified in infants aged <12 months. Replication of findings across research groups is needed. Nevertheless, caregivers are encouraged to be mindful of early developmental milestones (in social and emotional development, as well as motor, language, and problem-solving skills) and to raise questions if they have concerns that developmental goals are not being met. 79

Statement 4: Developmental trajectories may also serve as risk indicators of ASD.

The term “trajectory” encompasses the degree, rate, and direction of changes in the behaviors and/or developmental milestones being studied. An assessment of the time course of specific behaviors and patterns of development may be more sensitive than single-point, or “snapshot,” measures. Specifically, there is evidence that both early development (eg, language, nonverbal cognition) and social communication behaviors may follow atypical trajectories in children with ASD ascertained from high-risk infant sibling cohorts.

Atypical trajectory of early language and nonverbal development in ASD

Scores on standardized measures of early development reflect the slowing in acquisition of new skills over the first 2 years of life. Prospective studies have reported atypical trajectories of early verbal and nonverbal skills, with relatively typical development during the first year followed by declining standard scores corresponding with slowing of the acquisition of new skills during the second year of life. In a consecutive case series, 32 7 of 9 high-risk infants with a later diagnosis of ASD had average cognitive scores at the age of 12 months based on either the Bayley Scales of Infant Development or the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL). However, over the next 12 to 24 months, 4 of these 7 subjects (all boys) exhibited dramatic declines in IQ into the severe impairment range. In a second study comparing 50 infants aged 6 to 36 months who received a later diagnosis of ASD or typical development, 22 MSEL scores increased over time for all, but starting at 12 months of age, the ASD group exhibited slower nonverbal cognitive/language growth. Similarly, Landa and Garrett-Mayer 49 reported that 24 children with ASD, ascertained from a group of 87 high-risk infants, demonstrated reduced language and motor skills by age 14 months and reduced skills in all areas at age 24 months on the MSEL, compared with high-risk siblings who were developing typically. More recently, Landa et al 66 , 80 have provided evidence that declining trajectories in language and nonverbal cognitive skills (as indexed by using the MSEL) in high-risk infants are strongly associated with a later diagnosis of ASD. Although there may be uncertainty with regard to what is being measured by “cognitive” assessments at young ages, the measurement equivalence of verbal and nonverbal indices over the first few years of life (ie, whether measures are tapping the same developmental constructs at different ages), and the extent to which social and attentional abilities may be contributing to children’s performance, the findings of declining trajectories on standardized measures may nevertheless provide important predictive information.

Atypical trajectory of early social communication skills in ASD

The atypical trajectory of early social communication skills in ASD include decreased social gaze, social smiles, and vocalizations between 6 and 12 to 18 months of age. Ozonoff et al 22 reported on the emergence of social behavioral signs in 25 high-risk siblings with a later diagnosis of ASD and 25 low-risk children matched according to gender and later determined to have typical development. There were no group differences at age 6 months, but a declining trajectory of specific behaviors over the next 12 months was noted in the high-risk group. Group differences in gaze to faces and directed vocalizations were significant by age 12 months and in social smiling by 18 months; these findings persisted through to 36 months. Similarly, in a cohort of 125 high-risk infants, reduced social smiling, eye contact, social interest, affect, and response to name at 12 months (but not at 6 months) were predictive of diagnostic outcomes at 24 months. 45 Declining trajectories of social communicative behaviors associated with subsequent ASD diagnoses have also been reported in another cohort of 204 high-risk infants. 80 Yoder et al 43 also reported declining rates of joint attention behaviors in high-risk infants subsequently diagnosed with ASD. As previously noted, Jones and Klin 18 reported that a decline in the relative amount of time that high-risk infants aged 2 to 6 months spent orienting to the eyes versus mouth of a highly engaging adult shown on video was predictive of ASD.

Thus, the monitoring of development over time may prove important in assessing ASD risk, consistent with the American Academy of Pediatrics’ recommendations for systematic surveillance during well-child visits. 79

Statement 5: Caution should be exercised in drawing conclusions about early risk markers of ASD from studies that do not include individual-level outcome data.

Studies comparing behavior profiles across high- and low-risk groups can contribute to our understanding of early emerging features as well as the extent of the broader ASD phenotype (milder constellation of behavioral, cognitive, and other developmental characteristics that present in some relatives of individuals with ASD). However, group-level correlations do not always reflect individual-level correlations. Although some high-risk siblings will go on to receive a diagnosis of ASD, others will be diagnosed with other disorders, and most will not. Therefore, prevalence of an early behavioral marker in a group known to have elevated ASD risk should not be taken as evidence that the marker predicts risk at the individual level without knowing the outcome status of individuals.

Statement 6: Caution should be exercised in generalizing findings from studies of high-risk infants.

Even when individual-level data on risk markers and ASD outcomes are available in high-risk samples and markers predictive of ASD are reported, such findings might not generalize to the general population. High-risk sibling cohorts are unique in that their outcome risk is many times greater than other populations. In light of this finding and the accepted substantive involvement of genetic susceptibility factors in ASD etiology, it is plausible to suspect that unique risk mechanisms could be operating in this group. For example, initial reports suggested that abnormalities in DNA copy number variation in children with ASD were more common in simplex families than in multiplex families. 81 More recent array- and exome-based studies resulting from more advanced sequencing methods have not confirmed a higher overall burden of genetic variants in simplex families, although these studies continue to highlight the tremendous genetic diversity among and within families. 82 , 83 Variations in genetic mechanisms and the brain networks to which they map might also correlate with variation in early behavior profiles, 83 thus potentially limiting the extent to which risk markers seen in high-risk cohorts apply to other samples. Although it is premature to assume that findings from high-risk groups do not generalize more broadly, until more is known about underlying causal mechanisms and their relationship to phenotypic profiles, ample caution should be exercised.

Statement 7: Research about early markers of ASD should include diverse high- and low-risk samples.

Studies that examine cohorts at higher risk for ASD extending beyond infant sibling cohorts may offer some additional advantages in ASD research. First, these groups (eg, infants born prematurely or infants born to older parents) might be easier to assemble in large sample sizes. Moreover, such cohorts will also prove useful for assessing the generalizability of early risk marker profiles because they will have a mix of genetic susceptibility factors different from high-risk sibling cohorts yet still have elevated outcome rates compared with general population samples. Follow-up studies involving these cohorts may also create opportunities to study whether early behavioral markers for ASD are ASD-specific or also predict other developmental end points that occur (eg, intellectual disabilities). This point is particularly critical because in the absence of such comparison groups, we cannot conclude that behavioral markers associated with later diagnosis in high-risk infant sibling samples would be specific to ASDs in community samples that include the full spectrum of developmental and mental health disorders of early onset.

Statement 8: Future efforts should aim to identify: (1) early markers that can be measured in routine clinical practice, involving direct observation and parental report; (2) early biological processes measurable concurrently with, or before, overt behavioral markers; and (3) combined approaches.

Markers measurable in routine clinical practice

Many measures currently used in early identification research involve video coding of discrete behaviors, eye tracking, and/or the development of study-specific cutoffs that are of limited utility for present-day clinical practice. Efforts should be directed toward the development and validation of easy-to-administer, reliable tools for measuring potential behavioral markers within the context of routine clinical assessments; examples include coding smiling during cognitive assessment 22 or the assessment of head lag at 6 months, 67 especially in high-risk infants. Methods should be developed for gathering information from caregivers and from direct observation and interaction with the child, and for integrating these sources of information to inform clinical judgment. In a recent study, 2 prospective measures of emerging symptoms of ASD were found to correlate highly: (1) frequency of specific social behaviors as coded from videotape; and (2) independent examiner ratings of the frequency of social engagement behaviors in a different setting. 22 This type of study design may accelerate the development of measures that would be valid as well as more easily integrated into everyday practice.

Early neurobiological processes

Progress has also been made in studying and integrating biological data, such as brain volume and functional imaging (eg, from electrophysiological measurements) indices, with behavioral measures of ASD. For example, enlarged brain volumes including both gray and white matter have been reported in MRI studies of toddlers with ASD. 37 , 84 89 Enlargement has been noted in the frontal and temporal lobes 37 , 90 and in specific subcortical structures, such as the amygdala. 36 , 91 Enlargement has been observed in children as young as age 12 months and may be accompanied by an increase in extra-axial fluid. 87 Head circumference, which is a crude proxy for brain size, is generally consistent with brain enlargement in ASD, 84 although a recent review has raised questions as to whether ASD-related increases in head circumference have been largely driven by comparison with outdated population-based norms. 92 As such, MRI is the gold standard for indexing structural brain development in ASD.

In some MRI studies, brain overgrowth was found to correlate with behavioral markers at later ages. For example, amygdala size was correlated with joint attention ability measured at age 4 years 36 and with severity of social and communicative impairments measured at age 5 years. 91 In another recent study, 93 aberrant development of white matter pathways was found between 6 and 24 months of age in high-risk infants symptomatic for ASD at 24 months. Atypical neural responses, as indexed by event-related potentials, at age 6 to 10 months to viewing faces (specifically, the contrast between viewing faces whose eye gaze was directed toward, versus away from, the infant) have also been reported to relate to risk of ASD among high-risk infants. 19 Using functional MRI during natural sleep, a new study showed that the superior temporal gyrus (known to be involved in language processing) was less activated in toddlers with ASD relative to typically developing peers while listening to a simple bedtime story. Notably, these toddlers with ASD were referred from the community rather than being identified from a high-risk sibling sample. 94 Another functional MRI study, also with ASD toddlers from the general population, found reduced correlation between the right and left hemispheres in brain regions key for language and social processing. 95 Moreover, the levels of abnormal interhemispheric correlation could be used to distinguish toddlers with ASD from control subjects at an individual level, with a sensitivity of 72% and a specificity of 84%. These reports encourage the search for neurologic biomarkers or others that may reflect underlying pathologic processes in ASD and possibly precede and/or predict behavioral changes.

Cumulative risk indices

Researchers have not found a single behavioral sign or a single developmental trajectory that is predictive of all diagnoses of ASD. Given the heterogeneity of ASD expression, it is unlikely that a single behavior will be found universally across all children or will serve as the defining marker for a later emerging ASD. Future research may improve ASD risk prediction by examining combinations of symptomatic abnormalities (both in a cross-sectional manner and over time) that constitute cumulative risk indices. 96 Moreover, such a risk-profiling approach could incorporate both behavioral and biological markers 24 and thus offer the possibility of more reliable identification of infants at very high risk who could benefit from early intervention and/or preventive approaches to mitigate symptom development. It is also essential that future studies report individual-level data and adopt more consistent measures of relevant constructs to allow for accurate estimates of sensitivity and specificity of precise risk markers, as well as meta-analysis across studies.

Acknowledgments

The conference chairs and working groups acknowledge the preconference contributions of Katarzyna Chawarska, PhD, who was unable to attend the conference. We also acknowledge the efforts of Katherine F. Murray, BSN, RN, Massachusetts General Hospital for Children, in coordinating the forum and subsequent conference report process, and Sifor Ng in the conference report process.

The meeting and consensus report were sponsored by the Autism Forum. An important goal of the forum is to identify early indicators of ASDs that may lead to effective health care services. Autism Forum programs are developed under the guidance of its parent organization, the Northwest Autism Foundation. For this project, the Autism Research Institute provided financial support.

Glossary

ASD

autism spectrum disorder

IJA

initiating joint attention

MSEL

Mullen Scales of Early Learning

RJA

responding to joint attention

Footnotes

Drs Zwaigenbaum and Bauman initiated a literature review, co-chaired the meeting that generated the consensus recommendations outlined in this article, and drafted the initial manuscript; Drs Stone and Yirmiya co-chaired the working group that conducted the detailed literature review, generated initial recommendations that were discussed at the consensus meeting, and provided critical input to subsequent drafts of the manuscript; Drs Estes, Hansen, McPartland, and Natowicz were members of the working group that reviewed selected publications, contributed to initial recommendations that were reviewed at the consensus meeting, and critically reviewed the manuscript; Drs Choueiri, Fein, Kasari, Pierce, Buie, Carter, Davis, Granpeesheh, Mailloux, Newschaffer, Robins, Smith Roley, and Wetherby contributed to the consensus meeting that formed the basis for the manuscript and critically reviewed the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

FUNDING: Sponsored by the Autism Forum under the guidance of the Northwest Autism Foundation and with the support of the Autism Research Institute.

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

References

  • 1. Myers SM , Johnson CP American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Children With Disabilities . Management of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):1162–1182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Daniels AM , Mandell DS . Explaining differences in age at autism spectrum disorder diagnosis: a critical review. Autism. 2014;18(5):583–597 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2010 Principal Investigators. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) . Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2014;63(2):1–21 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Fountain C , King MD , Bearman PS . Age of diagnosis for autism: individual and community factors across 10 birth cohorts. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2011;65(6):503–510 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Mandell DS , Morales KH , Xie M , Lawer LJ , Stahmer AC , Marcus SC . Age of diagnosis among Medicaid-enrolled children with autism, 2001-2004. Psychiatr Serv. 2010;61(8):822–829 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Shattuck PT , Grosse SD . Issues related to the diagnosis and treatment of autism spectrum disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(2):129–135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Wallace KS , Rogers SJ . Intervening in infancy: implications for autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(12):1300–1320 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. De Giacomo A , Fombonne E . Parental recognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;7(3):131–136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Gillberg C , Ehlers S , Schaumann H , et al. Autism under age 3 years: a clinical study of 28 cases referred for autistic symptoms in infancy. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;31(6):921–934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Rogers SJ , DiLalla DL . Age of symptom onset in young children with pervasive developmental disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990;29(6):863–872 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Adrien JL , Faure M , Perrot A , et al. Autism and family home movies: preliminary findings. J Autism Dev Disord. 1991;21(1):43–49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Mars AE , Mauk JE , Dowrick PW . Symptoms of pervasive developmental disorders as observed in prediagnostic home videos of infants and toddlers. J Pediatr. 1998;132(3 pt 1):500–504 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Maestro S , Casella C , Milone A , Muratori F , Palacio-Espasa F . Study of the onset of autism through home movies. Psychopathology. 1999;32(6):292–300 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Loveland KA , Landry SH . Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. J Autism Dev Disord. 1986;16(3):335–349 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Kasari C , Sigman M , Mundy P , Yirmiya N . Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic, and mentally retarded children. J Autism Dev Disord. 1990;20(1):87–100 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Filipek PA , Accardo PJ , Ashwal S , et al. Practice parameter: screening and diagnosis of autism: report of the quality standards subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology and the Child Neurology Society. Neurology. 2000;55(4):468–479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Zwaigenbaum L , Thurm A , Stone W , et al. Studying the emergence of autism spectrum disorders in high-risk infants: methodological and practical issues. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(3):466–480 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Jones W , Klin A . Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature. 2013;504(7480):427–431 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Elsabbagh M , Fernandes J , Jane Webb S , Dawson G , Charman T , Johnson MH British Autism Study of Infant Siblings Team . Disengagement of visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in toddlerhood. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):189–194 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Walsh P , Elsabbagh M , Bolton P , Singh I . In search of biomarkers for autism: scientific, social and ethical challenges. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2011;12(10):603–612 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Guyatt GH , Oxman AD , Vist GE , et al. GRADE Working Group . GRADE: an emerging consensus on rating quality of evidence and strength of recommendations. BMJ. 2008;336(7650):924–926 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Ozonoff S , Iosif AM , Baguio F , et al. A prospective study of the emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(3):256–66.e1, 2 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Zwaigenbaum L , Bryson S , Lord C , et al. Clinical assessment and management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123(5):1383–1391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Zwaigenbaum L , Bryson S , Garon N . Early identification of autism spectrum disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2013;251:133–146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Coe BP , Girirajan S , Eichler EE . The genetic variability and commonality of neurodevelopmental disease. Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet. 2012;160C(2):118–129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Carayol J , Schellenberg GD , Tores F , Hager J , Ziegler A , Dawson G . Assessing the impact of a combined analysis of four common low-risk genetic variants on autism risk. Mol Autism. 2010;1(1):4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Hallmayer J , Cleveland S , Torres A , et al. Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(11):1095–1102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Pierce K , Glatt SJ , Liptak GS , McIntyre LL . The power and promise of identifying autism early: insights from the search for clinical and biological markers. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 2009;21(3):132–147 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders—Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2012;61(3):1–19 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Howlin P , Moore A . Diagnosis of autism: a survey of over 1200 patients in the UK. Autism. 1997;1(2):135–162 [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Siegel B , Pliner C , Eschler J , Elliott GR . How children with autism are diagnosed: difficulties in identification of children with multiple developmental delays. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1988;9(4):199–204 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Bryson SE , Zwaigenbaum L , Brian J , et al. A prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):12–24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Fodstad JC , Matson JL , Hess J , Neal D . Social and communication behaviours in infants and toddlers with autism and pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified. Dev Neurorehabil. 2009;12(3):152–157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Garon N , Bryson SE , Zwaigenbaum L , et al. Temperament and its relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(1):59–78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Goldberg WA , Jarvis KL , Osann K , et al. Brief report: early social communication behaviors in the younger siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35(5):657–664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Mosconi MW , Cody-Hazlett H , Poe MD , Gerig G , Gimpel-Smith R , Piven J . Longitudinal study of amygdala volume and joint attention in 2- to 4-year-old children with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(5):509–516 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Hazlett HC , Poe M , Gerig G , et al. Magnetic resonance imaging and head circumference study of brain size in autism: birth through age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(12):1366–1376 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Nadig AS , Ozonoff S , Young GS , Rozga A , Sigman M , Rogers SJ . A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):378–383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Osterling JA , Dawson G , Munson JA . Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(2):239–251 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Pierce K , Conant D , Hazin R , Stoner R , Desmond J . Preference for geometric patterns early in life as a risk factor for autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(1):101–109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Sullivan M , Finelli J , Marvin A , Garrett-Mayer E , Bauman M , Landa R . Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a prospective study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):37–48 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Wetherby AM , Woods J , Allen L , Cleary J , Dickinson H , Lord C . Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(5):473–493 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Yoder P , Stone WL , Walden T , Malesa E . Predicting social impairment and ASD diagnosis in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(10):1381–1391 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Presmanes AG , Walden TA , Stone WL , Yoder PJ . Effects of different attentional cues on responding to joint attention in younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):133–144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Zwaigenbaum L , Bryson S , Rogers T , Roberts W , Brian J , Szatmari P . Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23(2–3):143–152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Macari SL , Campbell D , Gengoux GW , Saulnier CA , Klin AJ , Chawarska K . Predicting developmental status from 12 to 24 months in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: a preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(12):2636–2647 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Stone WL , McMahon CR , Yoder PJ , Walden TA . Early social-communicative and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):384–390 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Yirmiya N , Gamliel I , Pilowsky T , Feldman R , Baron-Cohen S , Sigman M . The development of siblings of children with autism at 4 and 14 months: social engagement, communication, and cognition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(5):511–523 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Landa R , Garrett-Mayer E . Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):629–638 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Oller DK , Niyogi P , Gray S , et al. Automated vocal analysis of naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language delay, and typical development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2010;107(30):13354–13359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Gamliel I , Yirmiya N , Sigman M . The development of young siblings of children with autism from 4 to 54 months. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):171–183 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Esposito G , Venuti P . Analysis of toddlers’ gait after six months of independent walking to identify autism: a preliminary study. Percept Mot Skills. 2008;106(1):259–269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Esposito G , Venuti P , Apicella F , Muratori F . Analysis of unsupported gait in toddlers with autism. Brain Dev. 2011;33(5):367–373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Matson JL , Dempsey T , Fodstad JC . Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Dev Neurorehabil. 2009;12(3):122–127 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Ozonoff S , Young GS , Goldring S , et al. Gross motor development, movement abnormalities, and early identification of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(4):644–656 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Provost B , Lopez BR , Heimerl S . A comparison of motor delays in young children: autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(2):321–328 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Ozonoff S , Macari S , Young GS , Goldring S , Thompson M , Rogers SJ . Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a prospective sample. Autism. 2008;12(5):457–472 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Watt N , Wetherby AM , Barber A , Morgan L . Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(8):1518–1533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Coonrod EE , Stone WL . Early concerns of parents of children with autistic and nonautistic disorders. Infants Young Child. 2004;17(3):258–268 [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Baranek GT , Barnett CR , Adams EM , Wolcott NA , Watson LR , Crais ER . Object play in infants with autism: methodological issues in retrospective video analysis. Am J Occup Ther. 2005;59(1):20–30 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Chawarska K , Macari S , Shic F . Context modulates attention to social scenes in toddlers with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(8):903–913 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Cornew L , Dobkins KR , Akshoomoff N , McCleery JP , Carver LJ . Atypical social referencing in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(12):2611–2621 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Mitchell S , Brian J , Zwaigenbaum L , et al. Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27(2 suppl):S69–S78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Barber AB , Wetherby AM , Chambers NW . Brief report: repetitive behaviors in young children with autism spectrum disorder and developmentally similar peers: a follow up to Watt et al. (2008). J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(9):2006–2012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Loh A , Soman T , Brian J , et al. Stereotyped motor behaviors associated with autism in high-risk infants: a pilot videotape analysis of a sibling sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):25–36 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Landa RJ , Gross AL , Stuart EA , Bauman M . Latent class analysis of early developmental trajectory in baby siblings of children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012;53(9):986–996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Flanagan JE , Landa R , Bhat A , Bauman M . Head lag in infants at risk for autism: a preliminary study. Am J Occup Ther. 2012;66(5):577–585 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Bhat AN , Galloway JC , Landa RJ . Relation between early motor delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for autism. Infant Behav Dev. 2012;35(4):838–846 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Bolton PF , Golding J , Emond A , Steer CD . Autism spectrum disorder and autistic traits in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: precursors and early signs. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;51(3):249–260.e25 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Clifford SM , Hudry K , Elsabbagh M , Charman T , Johnson MH BASIS Team . Temperament in the first 2 years of life in infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(3):673–686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Maestro S , Muratori F , Cavallaro MC , et al. How young children treat objects and people: an empirical study of the first year of life in autism. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2005;35(4):383–396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Maestro S , Muratori F , Cavallaro MC , et al. Attentional skills during the first 6 months of age in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(10):1239–1245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Merin N , Young GS , Ozonoff S , Rogers SJ . Visual fixation patterns during reciprocal social interaction distinguish a subgroup of 6-month-old infants at-risk for autism from comparison infants. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(1):108–121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Werner E , Dawson G , Osterling J , Dinno N . Brief report: recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: a retrospective study based on home videotapes. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(2):157–162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Young GS , Merin N , Rogers SJ , Ozonoff S . Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months: predicting clinical outcomes and language development in typically developing infants and infants at risk for autism. Dev Sci. 2009;12(5):798–814 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Noland JS , Steven Reznick J , Stone WL , Walden T , Sheridan EH . Better working memory for non-social targets in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev Sci. 2010;13(1):244–251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Colgan SE , Lanter E , McComish C , Watson LR , Crais ER , Baranek GT . Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12(4-5):307–319 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Paul R , Fuerst Y , Ramsay G , Chawarska K , Klin A . Out of the mouths of babes: vocal production in infant siblings of children with ASD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52(5):588–598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Johnson CP , Myers SM American Academy of Pediatrics Council on Children With Disabilities . Identification and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):1183–1215 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Landa RJ , Gross AL , Stuart EA , Faherty A . Developmental trajectories in children with and without autism spectrum disorders: the first 3 years. Child Dev. 2013;84(2):429–442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Sebat J , Lakshmi B , Malhotra D , et al. Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. 2007;316(5823):445–449 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Griswold AJ , Ma D , Cukier HN , et al. Evaluation of copy number variations reveals novel candidate genes in autism spectrum disorder-associated pathways. Hum Mol Genet. 2012;21(15):3513–3523 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Pinto D , Delaby E , Merico D , et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94(5):677–694 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Courchesne E , Mouton PR , Calhoun ME , et al. Neuron number and size in prefrontal cortex of children with autism. JAMA. 2011;306(18):2001–2010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Carper RA , Moses P , Tigue ZD , Courchesne E . Cerebral lobes in autism: early hyperplasia and abnormal age effects. Neuroimage. 2002;16(4):1038–1051 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Sparks BF , Friedman SD , Shaw DW , et al. Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology. 2002;59(2):184–192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Shen MD , Nordahl CW , Young GS , et al. Early brain enlargement and elevated extra-axial fluid in infants who develop autism spectrum disorder. Brain. 2013;136(pt 9):2825–2835 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Hazlett HC , Poe MD , Gerig G , et al. Early brain overgrowth in autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(5):467–476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Schumann CM , Bloss CS , Barnes CC , et al. Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical development through early childhood in autism. J Neurosci. 2010;30(12):4419–4427 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Carper RA , Courchesne E . Localized enlargement of the frontal cortex in early autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(2):126–133 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Schumann CM , Barnes CC , Lord C , Courchesne E . Amygdala enlargement in toddlers with autism related to severity of social and communication impairments. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(10):942–949 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Raznahan A , Wallace GL , Antezana L , et al. Compared to what? Early brain overgrowth in autism and the perils of population norms. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(8):563–575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Wolff JJ , Gu H , Gerig G , et al. IBIS Network . Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(6):589–600 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Eyler LT , Pierce K , Courchesne E . A failure of left temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging and fundamental property of autism. Brain. 2012;135(pt 3):949–960 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Dinstein I , Pierce K , Eyler L , et al. Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers with autism. Neuron. 2011;70(6):1218–1225 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Yirmiya N , Charman T . The prodrome of autism: early behavioral and biological signs, regression, peri- and post-natal development and genetics. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(4):432–458 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Butterworth G , Jarrett N . What minds have in common is space: Spatial mechanisms serving joint visual attention in infancy. Br J Dev Psychol. 1991;9:55–72 20085609 [Google Scholar]

Articles from Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of American Academy of Pediatrics

RESOURCES