Key Points
Question
Are subgroups derived from measures of brain function among children and adolescents with neurodevelopmental conditions replicable across independently collected data sets?
Findings
In this case-control study using resting-state data from 2 network data sets with a total of 1102 individuals aged 5 to 19 years with and without neurodevelopmental conditions, subgroups with similar biology that differed significantly in intelligence as well as hyperactivity and impulsivity problems were identified. However, these groups showed no consistent alignment with diagnostic categories.
Meaning
In this study, homogeneity in the neurobiology of neurodevelopmental conditions corresponded to behavior, not diagnostic category; these findings are replicable in independent cohorts, taking an important step toward translating neurobiological subgroups into clinical settings.
This case-control study identifies subgroups of children with and without neurodevelopmental conditions with shared brain characteristics using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data from 2 large, independent data sets.
Abstract
Importance
Neurodevelopmental conditions, such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), have highly heterogeneous and overlapping phenotypes and neurobiology. Data-driven approaches are beginning to identify homogeneous transdiagnostic subgroups of children; however, findings have yet to be replicated in independently collected data sets, a necessity for translation into clinical settings.
Objective
To identify subgroups of children with and without neurodevelopmental conditions with shared functional brain characteristics using data from 2 large, independent data sets.
Design, Setting, and Participants
This case-control study used data from the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental (POND) network (study recruitment began June 2012 and is ongoing; data were extracted April 2021) and the Healthy Brain Network (HBN; study recruitment began May 2015 and is ongoing; data were extracted November 2020). POND and HBN data are collected from institutions across Ontario and New York, respectively. Participants who had diagnoses of ASD, ADHD, and OCD or were typically developing (TD); were aged between 5 and 19 years; and successfully completed the resting-state and anatomical neuroimaging protocol were included in the current study.
Main Outcomes and Measures
The analyses consisted of a data-driven clustering procedure on measures derived from each participant’s resting-state functional connectome, performed independently on each data set. Differences between each pair of leaves in the resulting clustering decision trees in the demographic and clinical characteristics were tested.
Results
Overall, 551 children and adolescents were included from each data set. POND included 164 participants with ADHD; 217 with ASD; 60 with OCD; and 110 with TD (median [IQR] age, 11.87 [9.51-14.76] years; 393 [71.2%] male participants; 20 [3.6%] Black, 28 [5.1%] Latino, and 299 [54.2%] White participants) and HBN included 374 participants with ADHD; 66 with ASD; 11 with OCD; and 100 with TD (median [IQR] age, 11.50 [9.22-14.20] years; 390 [70.8%] male participants; 82 [14.9%] Black, 57 [10.3%] Hispanic, and 257 [46.6%] White participants). In both data sets, subgroups with similar biology that differed significantly in intelligence as well as hyperactivity and impulsivity problems were identified, yet these groups showed no consistent alignment with current diagnostic categories. For example, there was a significant difference in Strengths and Weaknesses ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behavior Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale (SWAN-HI) between 2 subgroups in the POND data (C and D), with subgroup D having increased hyperactivity and impulsivity traits compared with subgroup C (median [IQR], 2.50 [0.00-7.00] vs 1.00 [0.00-5.00]; U = 1.19 × 104; P = .01; η2 = 0.02). A significant difference in SWAN-HI scores between subgroups g and d in the HBN data was also observed (median [IQR], 1.00 [0.00-4.00] vs 0.00 [0.00-2.00]; corrected P = .02). There were no differences in the proportion of each diagnosis between the subgroups in either data set.
Conclusions and Relevance
The findings of this study suggest that homogeneity in the neurobiology of neurodevelopmental conditions transcends diagnostic boundaries and is instead associated with behavioral characteristics. This work takes an important step toward translating neurobiological subgroups into clinical settings by being the first to replicate our findings in independently collected data sets.
Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are neurodevelopmental conditions clinically defined based on distinct behavioral criteria.1 However, an increasing body of evidence suggests that these conditions are highly heterogeneous in biology and phenotype within each condition2,3,4 and significantly overlapping.5,6,7,8,9,10,11,12 These observations pose significant challenges to traditional case-control studies, especially with relatively small sample sizes,13 and have resulted in discrepant findings across various studies. For example, investigations attempting to characterize the functional connectome in individuals with ASD, ADHD, and OCD compared with typically developing (TD) populations are highly mixed.14,15,16 The seemingly contradictory findings reflect the heterogeneous nature of these conditions17,18 as well as suggest that differences in the topography of the functional connectome, such as integration and segregation between resting state networks, may better explain connectivity patterns in neurodevelopmental conditions rather than the strength of individual connections.19,20,21,22,23 These topographical differences have frequently been described in all 3 conditions,24,25,26 and there is increasing evidence that these differences are shared across conditions.9,10
To disentangle these findings, a shift from traditional case-control designs to data-driven approaches, which transcend diagnostic boundaries to identify groups that are homogeneous in their neurobiology, is necessary. An emerging body of literature using data-driven approaches supports the idea that the diagnostic categories of ASD, ADHD, and OCD are not associated with unique underlying neurobiological mechanisms27,28 and often do not predict treatment outcome.29 This motivates the need for the discovery of homogeneous groups that can accelerate the development of targeted and personalized treatment approaches, interventions, supports, and accommodations that fit the diverse profiles of strengths and needs of children with neurodevelopmental conditions.
To this end, several studies have used measures of brain function or structure to identify transdiagnostic subgroups of neurodevelopmental conditions, consistently demonstrating a misalignment between data-driven groupings and existing diagnostic categories.9,10,12,27,28,30,31,32 The first contribution of this article is to characterize the heterogeneity across neurodevelopmental conditions by identifying cross-diagnosis subgroups of children and adolescents with and without neurodevelopmental conditions using measures of integration and segregation of the functional connectome.
Despite the promise of data-driven approaches and the encouraging preliminary reports, the replicability and generalizability of these findings remains an open question in the field33 and a critical gap to clinical translation of the findings.13,34 To date, this issue has been addressed partly by using subsampling within a data set to enhance generalizability27,28; however, to our knowledge, subgroupings within neurodevelopmental conditions based on neuroimaging data have not been replicated across independently collected data sets. The second contribution of this article is to be the first, to our knowledge, to address this replication gap by examining subgroups across 2 large, independently collected, cross-condition data sets, namely the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental Disorders (POND) network and the Healthy Brains Network (HBN).
Methods
Both the POND network and HBN studies were approved by the appropriate research ethics boards, and the current study was approved by the Holland Bloorview Kids Rehabilitation Hospital’s research ethics board; written informed consent and/or verbal assent was obtained from the primary caregivers and/or participants (eMethods in Supplement 1). This study followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) reporting guideline for case-control studies.
Participants
For the primary cohort, participants were drawn from the POND Network data set (exported April 2021)35, and data from the Healthy Brain Network36,37 was used as the replication cohort (exported November 2020) (eMethods in Supplement 1). Overall, 717 POND participants (210 with ADHD; 300, ASD; 69, OCD; and 138, TD) and 966 HBN participants (672, ADHD; 111, ASD; 12, OCD; and 171, TD) aged between 5 and 19 years were included in the current study based on successful completion of the resting-state and anatomical imaging protocols and presence of phenotypic data. Details on phenotypic measures used to characterize the POND and HBN samples are provided in the eMethods in Supplement 1.
Both datasets used self- or parent-reported race and ethnicity, per the protocols of the larger POND and HBN studies. In the POND data set, racial groups were defined according to the Canadian Institute for Health Information standards and included Black, East Asian, Indigenous, Latino, Middle Eastern, other, South Asian, Southeast Asian, and White. Participants were classified into multiple categories if they were of mixed race; those who did not identify as one of the groups were categorized as other. In the HBN data set, categories were defined according to US Census guidelines and included American Indian or Alaskan Native, Asian, Black, Hispanic, 2 or more races, Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander, other, and White. Participants of mixed race were classified as such, and thus participants were only assigned to 1 category; those who did not identify as any of the census groups were categorized as other. Due to low sample size, categories for both datasets were collapsed into minoritized racial and ethnic group and White for statistical tests.
Neuroimaging Data
Five minutes of resting-state data and anatomical brain images were collected as part of the POND and HBN studies and preprocessed. Propensity scores were used to match the POND and HBN participants who passed quality control on age, sex, and motion. Full details on data acquisition, preprocessing, and propensity score matching can be found in eMethods and eTable 1 in Supplement 1.
Connectome Construction
Connectome nodes were defined using the cortical atlas from Schaefer et al,38 supplemented by the Melbourne subcortical atlas,39 as this parcellation scheme is highly representative across alternative connectome construction pipelines,40 resulting in 232 nodes. The parcels were categorized into 8 functional networks: visual, somatomotor, dorsal attention, ventral attention and salience, limbic, frontoparietal control, default mode, and subcortical (eFigure 1 in Supplement 1). Pairwise Pearson correlations between parcel-averaged preprocessed time series were computed as the edge weights between pairs of nodes. The edge weights were harmonized to account for acquisition site effects across both data sets, and the influence of scanner, age, and sex were removed (eMethods in Supplement 1). The connectomes were thresholded to remove spurious connections and produce more biologically plausible connectomes.40,41 For each participant, nodal measures of integration (betweenness centrality42,43) and segregation (clustering coefficient42) were extracted and z scored.
Clustering
Clustering was performed separately on the POND and HBN data sets, and the pipeline is presented in Figure 1. In the first section (Figure 1A), similarity network fusion (SNF44) was used to compute similarity matrices for each measure-network pair (eg, segregation of the visual network) using the Euclidean distance across all nodal measures belonging to the network and SNF’s K-nearest neighbors weighted similarity kernel. The 16 similarity matrices (8 networks × 2 measures) were then fused using SNF, from which spectral clustering can be used to identify a prespecified number of subgroups.
Due to SNF’s dependence on 2 free hyperparameters (μ and K), 10 000 clustering solutions were obtained using different combinations of hyperparameters (Figure 1B and eMethods in Supplement 1). A participant coassignment matrix was then generated by computing the percentage of times 2 participants were clustered in the same subgroup across cluster solutions that were stable across the hyperparameter space.
The clustering procedure was performed on 10 000 subsampling iterations to increase robustness of the final clustering solution, selecting 63.2% of the sample in each iteration; a final coassignment matrix was constructed by computing the median across all subsampling iterations (Figure 1C). Hierarchical clustering was performed on the final coassignment matrix (Figure 1D) to identify subgroups across the full range of number of clusters (2-10). Hierarchical clustering constructs a rooted tree, or a dendrogram, consisting of layers of nodes: the first layer contains a single cluster, representing the trivial solution, and the nth layer contains n clusters, representing the nth-cluster solution45; in each layer, a root cluster is split into 2 leaf clusters. The optimal number of clusters was identified using the Calinski-Harabasz index.46
Statistical Analysis
Differences in the demographic and behavioral measures were compared among the diagnostic groups within each data set as well as compared between data sets. For the continuous measures, Kruskal-Wallis tests or 1-way analysis of variance were used, depending on normality (eTable 2 in Supplement 1); for significant omnibus tests (P < .05), post hoc testing was carried out using the Dunn procedure with Bonferroni-corrected P values (corrected P < .05). For nominal categorical variables (sex and acquisition scanner), χ2 tests were performed with post hoc pairwise z tests of independent proportions (corrected P < .05). For ordinal categorical variables (socioeconomic variables in the POND data set), ordinal regression was performed, testing for all pairwise between-group differences (corrected P < .05).
To determine which brain measures were associated with the split from a root cluster into its 2 leaf clusters in each layer, we tested for a difference in means between each pair of leaf clusters in their network-averaged measures of segregation and integration. Given that we are using the same data to both define the groups via clustering and perform downstream testing, traditional statistical tests such as Mann-Whitney U and t tests would lead to inflated type I errors, as they only control for such error rates when groups are defined a priori.47 Thus, we used the clusterpval48 package in R version 4.2.1 (R Project for Statistical Computing) to produce test statistics and P values that are corrected for double-use of the data; the generic implementation was used, which approximates the corrected P values using Monte Carlo sampling, and the resulting P values were corrected for multiple comparisons (corrected P < .05). Mann-Whitney U or t tests and χ2 tests were used to determine differences between leaves in the demographic and behavioral measures.
For all significant tests, effect sizes were reported. For continuous measures, eta-squared (η2) effect sizes were used, using the ranked data for nonnormally distributed data. For categorical variables, Cramer V effect sizes were reported, while for ordinal variables, pseudo-R2 values were reported.49
Results
Sample Characteristics
The final data set included 551 POND participants (164 with ADHD; 217, ASD; 60, OCD; 110, TD; median [IQR] age. 11.87 [9.51-14.76] years; 393 [71.2%] male participants; 20 [3.6%] Black, 28 [5.1%] Latino, and 299 [54.2%] White participants) and 551 HBN participants (374 with ADHD; 66, ASD; 11, OCD; 100, TD; median [IQR] age, 11.50 [9.22-14.20] years; 390 [70.8%] male participants; 82 [14.9%] Black, 57 [10.3%] Hispanic, and 257 [46.6%] White participants). The final sample was reached by performing propensity matching on the individuals who passed quality control (592 POND and 756 HBN individuals) to match the data sets on age, sex, and head motion. Descriptive statistics of the POND and HBN sample characteristics are provided in Tables 1 and 2, respectively. Complete race and ethnicity data for each data set are presented in eTable 3 in Supplement 1.
Table 1. Descriptive Statistics of the Participant Demographics and Clinical Behavioral Measures for the Province of Ontario Neurodevelopmental Network Data Set, With the Corresponding Statistics Identifying Significant Differences Among the Diagnostic Groups.
Measurea | Median (IQR) | Statistics | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ADHD (n = 164) | ASD (n = 217) | OCD (n = 60) | TD (n = 110) | Test statisticb | P value | Effect sizec | Significant post hocd | |
Age, y | 11.34 (9.47 to 13.77) | 11.90 (9.51 to 15.14) | 12.98 (11.32 to 15.12) | 11.6 (8.94 to 13.35) | 9.50 | .02 | 0.02 | ADHD < OCD |
Sex, No. (%) | ||||||||
Male | 125 (76.2) | 166 (76.5) | 38 (63.3) | 64 (58.2) | 15.9 | 1.18 × 10−3 | 0.17 | Female: TD, OCD > ADHD, ASD |
Female | 39 (23.8) | 51 (23.5) | 22 (36.7) | 46 (41.8) | ||||
Primary caregiver education, No. (%)e | ||||||||
1 | 1 (0.6) | 2 (0.9) | 2 (1.8) | 8.78 | .03 | 0.03 | ASD < OCD, TD | |
2 | 10 (6.1) | 23 (10.6) | 10 (9.1) | |||||
3 | 20 (12.2) | 34 (15.7) | 2 (3.3) | 17 (15.5) | ||||
4 | 25 (15.2) | 41 (18.9) | 5 (8.3) | 44 (40) | ||||
5 | 18 (11) | 31 (14.3) | 5 (8.3) | 32 (29.1) | ||||
Household income, No. (%)f | ||||||||
Low | 17 (10.4) | 29 (13.4) | 1 (1.7) | 13 (11.8) | 11.76 | .01 | 0.04 | ADHD, ASD < TD |
Medium | 36 (22) | 60 (27.6) | 4 (6.7) | 40 (36.4) | ||||
High | 19 (11.6) | 25 (11.5) | 3 (5) | 39 (35.5) | ||||
Race and ethnicity, No. (%) | ||||||||
Minoritized race and ethnicityg | 48 (29.3) | 55 (25.3) | 15 (25) | 40 (36.4) | 0.3 | .96 | 0.03 | NA |
White | 65 (39.6) | 84 (38.7) | 20 (33.3) | 59 (53.6) | ||||
Scanner, No. (%) | ||||||||
SK-TT | 41 (25) | 90 (41.5) | 37 (61.7) | 16 (14.5) | 81.3 | 1.97 × 10−15 | 0.27 | SK-TT: OCD > ASD > ADHD, TD; QU-TT: ADHD, TD > ASD |
QU-TT | 38 (23.2) | 11 (5.1) | 0 (0) | 31 (28.2) | ||||
SK-PF | 85 (51.8) | 116 (53.5) | 23 (38.3) | 63 (57.3) | ||||
Head motion, mm | 11.34 (9.47 to 13.77) | 11.90 (9.51 to 15.14) | 12.98 (11.32 to 15.12) | 11.6 (8.94 to 13.35) | 13.31 | 4.01 × 10−3 | 0.02 | ASD > OCD, TD |
FSIQ | 11.34 (9.47 to 13.77) | 11.90 (9.51 to 15.14) | 12.98 (11.32 to 15.12) | 11.6 (8.94 to 13.35) | 79.01 | 5.01 × 10−17 | 0.16 | ASD < ADHD < TD, OCD |
SCQ | 11.34 (9.47 to 13.77) | 11.90 (9.51 to 15.14) | 12.98 (11.32 to 15.12) | 11.6 (8.94 to 13.35) | 305.01 | 8.18 × 10−66 | 0.61 | ASD > ADHD |
OCD > TD | ||||||||
RBS-R | 10.00 (4.00 to 20.00) | 26.00 (15.00 to 40.50) | 22.00 (10.75 to 36.00) | 0.00 (0.00 to 2.00) | 250.30 | 5.64 × 10−54 | 0.49 | ASD, OCD > ADHD > TD |
SWAN-I | 6.00 (1.00 to 7.00) | 5.00 (2.00 to 7.00) | 1.00 (0.00 to 3.25) | 0.00 (0.00 to 0.00) | 224.57 | 2.07 × 10−48 | 0.45 | ADHD > ASD > OCD > TD |
SWAN-HI | 3.50 (1.00 to 7.00) | 3.00 (1.00 to 7.00) | 0.00 (0.00 to 2.00) | 0.00 (0.00 to 0.00) | 174.46 | 1.38 × 10−37 | 0.35 | ADHD, ASD > OCD > TD |
TOCS | −22.00 (−48.00 to −2.00) | −5.00 (−31.50 to 6.75) | 20.00 (12.00 to 33.75) | −43.50 (−59.00 to −11.50) | 149.46 | 3.44 × 10−32 | 0.30 | OCD > ASD > ADHD > TD |
Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; FSIQ, full-scale intelligence quotient; NA, not applicable; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; QU-TT, Queen’s University TimTrio; RBS-R, Repetitive Behaviors Scale–Revised; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SK-PF, SickKids PrismaFIT; SK-TT, SickKids TimTrio; SWAN-HI, Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD–Symptoms and Normal Behavior Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale; SWAN-I, Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD–Symptoms and Normal Behavior Inattention subscale; TD, typically developing; TOCS, Toronto Obsessive-Compulsive Scale.
Descriptions and ranges of all measures appear in the eMethods in Supplement 1.
Test statistic: Shapiro-Wilk W for nonnormally distributed continuous variables, 1-way analysis of variance F statistic for normally distributed continuous variables, χ2 for categorical variables, and ordinal regression χ2 for ordinal variables.
Effect size: eta-squared (η2) for continuous variables, Cramer V for categorical variables, and pseudo-R2 for ordinal variables.
Indication of which pairwise diagnosis differences are significant according to post hoc tests, with > and < symbols indicating the directionality of the association.
Level 1 indicates caregiver did not complete high school; level 2, high school education; level 3, associate degree; level 4, undergraduate degree; level 5, graduate or professional degree. Primary caregiver education data was available for 332 of 551 participants (74 ADHD, 131 ASD, 12 OCD, and 105 TD).
Low indicates less than $74 999; medium, $75 000 to $199 999; high, greater than $200 000. Household income was available for 286 of 551 participants (72 ADHD, 114 ASD, 8 OCD, 92 TD).
Includes Black, East Asian, Indigenous, Latino, Middle Eastern, South Asian, and Southeast Asian individuals as well as those who did not identify as 1 of the Canadian Institutes of Health Information groups (ie, other). Race and ethnicity were available for 381 of 551 participants (113 ADHD, 139 ASD, 35 OCD, 99 TD).
Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of the Participant Demographics and Clinical Behavioral Measures for the Healthy Brain Network Data Set, With the Corresponding Statistics Identifying Significant Differences Among the Diagnostic Groups.
Measurea | Median (IQR) | Statistics | ||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ADHD (n = 374) | ASD (n = 66) | OCD (n = 11) | TD (n = 100) | Test statisticb | P value | Effect sizec | Significant post hocd | |
Age, y | 11.20 (9.07-13.63) | 13.64 (10.83-16.04) | 12.77 (11.09-14.87) | 11.36 (9.36-14.22) | 18.39 | 3.65 × 10−4 | 0.03 | ASD > ADHD, TD |
Sex | ASD > ADHD, OCD, TD | |||||||
Male | 273 (73.0) | 57 (86.4) | 6 (54.5) | 54 (54.0) | 23.7 | 2.95 × 10−5 | 0.21 | ADHD > TD |
Female | 101 (27.0) | 9 (13.6) | 5 (45.5) | 46 (46.0) | ||||
BSMSSe | 50.00 (40.00-59.50) | 50.25 (42.00-61.00) | 61.00 (48.88-62.88) | 53.00 (45.75-61.00) | 4.24 | .24 | 0.01 | NA |
Race and ethnicity | ||||||||
Minoritized race and ethnicityf | 182 (48.7) | 26 (39.4) | 2 (18.2) | 42 (42.0) | 4.8 | .19 | 0.10 | NA |
White | 170 (45.5) | 30 (45.5) | 8 (72.7) | 49 (49.0) | ||||
Scanner | ||||||||
CBIC | 144 (38.5) | 34 (51.5) | 4 (36.4) | 21 (21.0) | 35.1 | 4.20 × 10−6 | 0.18 | CBIC: ASD > ADHD > TD; SI: ADHD, ASD < TD |
RU | 184 (49.2) | 25 (37.9) | 5 (45.5) | 46 (46.0) | ||||
SI | 46 (12.3) | 7 (10.6) | 2 (18.2) | 33 (33.0) | ||||
Head motion, mm | 0.18 (0.12-0.25) | 0.15 (0.11-0.26) | 0.16 (0.10-0.30) | 0.14 (0.11-0.22) | 8.13 | .04 | 0.01 | ADHD > TD |
FSIQ, mean (SD) | 96.84 (15.82) | 94.03 (19.65) | 106.73 (19.37) | 106.64 (15.47) | 11.53 | 2.52 × 10−7 | 0.06 | ASD, ADHD < TD |
SCQ | 7.00 (4.00-10.00) | 13.00 (10.00-18.00) | 4.00 (3.25-6.75) | 6.00 (3.00-8.00) | 92.06 | 7.92 × 10−20 | 0.17 | ASD > ADHD, OCD, TD |
ADHD > TD | ||||||||
RBS-R | 11.00 (2.00-38.00) | 43.00 (15.00-66.00) | 24.50 (16.50-51.50) | 1.00 (1.00-7.00) | 58.20 | 1.43 × 10−12 | 0.14 | ASD > ADHD, OCD, TD |
ADHD > TD | ||||||||
SWAN-I | 4.00 (1.00-7.00) | 3.00 (1.00-5.75) | 2.00 (0.00-3.00) | 0.00 (0.00-1.00) | 96.45 | 9.00 × 10−21 | 0.18 | ADHD, ASD > TD |
SWAN-HI | 1.00 (0.00-3.00) | 1.00 (0.00-4.00) | 0.00 (0.00-0.00) | 0.00 (0.00-0.00) | 65.09 | 4.80 × 10−14 | 0.12 | ADHD > OCD, TD |
ASD > TD | ||||||||
CBCL-OCS | 3.00 (2.00-4.00) | 3.00 (2.00-5.00) | 3.00 (2.00-5.75) | 1.00 (0.00-2.00) | 91.99 | 8.19 × 10−20 | 0.17 | ADHD, ASD, OCD > TD |
Abbreviations: ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; BSMSS, Barratt Simplified Measure of Social Status; CBCL-OCS, Child Behavior Checklist obsessive-compulsive subscale; CBIC, CitiGroup Cornell Brain Imaging Center; FSIQ, full-scale intelligence quotient; NA, not applicable; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; RBS-R, Repetitive Behaviors Scale–Revised; RU, Rutgers University; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SI, Staten Island; SWAN-HI, Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD–Symptoms and Normal Behavior Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale; SWAN-I, Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD–Symptoms and Normal Behavior Inattention subscale; TD, typically developing.
Descriptions and ranges of all measures appear in the eMethods in Supplement 1.
Test statistic: Shapiro-Wilk W for nonnormally distributed continuous variables, 1-way analysis of variance F statistic for normally distributed continuous variables, χ2 for categorical variables, and ordinal regression χ2 for ordinal variables.
Effect size: eta-squared (η2) for continuous variables, Cramer V for categorical variables, and pseudo-R2 for ordinal variables.
Indication of which pairwise diagnosis differences are significant according to post hoc tests, with > and < symbols indicating the directionality of the association.
The range for BSMSS is 8 to 66, with higher scores indicating a higher social status.
Includes American Indian or Alaskan Native, Asian, Black, Hispanic, Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander, and those who selected 2 or more races/ethnicities and those who did not identify as one of the US Census guideline categories. Data were available for 509 of 511 participants (352 ADHD, 56 ASD, 10 OCD, 91 TD).
Compared with the POND sample, the HBN sample had a significantly higher proportion of ADHD and a lower proportion of ASD and OCD diagnoses (χ2 = 197.74; P < .001; V = 0.42). Furthermore, the HBN sample had significantly more participants belonging to minoritized racial and ethnic groups (χ2 = 6.50; P = .01; V = 0.09), lower full-scale IQ (U = 1.15 × 105; P = .01; η2 = 7.44 × 10−3), and fewer social communication difficulties (U = 1.26 × 105; P = .03; η2 = 1.71 × 10−3) and hyperactivity and impulsivity problems, measured by the Strengths and Weaknesses ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behavior Hyperactivity/Impulsivity subscale (SWAN-H/I; U = 1.09 × 105; P < .001; η2 = 0.03). eTable 4 in Supplement 1 includes full details.
Clustering
The Calinski-Harabasz index indicated that the 6- and 10-cluster solutions were optimal for POND and HBN, respectively (eFigure 2 in Supplement 1). Visual representations of the emergence of the 6 clusters in the POND and HBN data sets are presented in Figure 2, and statistical results appear in eTables 5 to 8 in Supplement 1. Significant differences between the leaf clusters in network-averaged measures of segregation and integration for each layer in the POND and HBN dendrograms are shown in eFigure 3 and eTables 9 to 12 in Supplement 1.
For both POND and HBN data sets, the 2-cluster solution split the sample into 2 groups (POND: subgroups A and B; HBN: subgroups a and b). Subgroups B and b had increased segregation in all resting-state networks; increased integration in the somatomotor, dorsal attention, limbic and default mode networks; and decreased integration in the frontoparietal control network and subcortical regions compared with subgroups A and a (Figure 3A and B). Although integration of the visual network was also observed to be decreased in the HBN data set, this was not replicated in the POND data set. A difference in IQ scores was observed (full-spectrum IQ, POND: U = 2.86 × 104; P = .04; η2 = 0.01; full-spectrum IQ, HBN: t = −2.37; P = .02; η2 = 0.01), with subgroups B and b having increased IQ scores compared with subgroups A and a (median [IQR] IQ scores, B vs A: 104.00 [92.00-114.00] vs 100.00 [88.00-110.00]; mean [SD] IQ scores, b vs a: 100.63 [16.98] vs 97.08 [16.55]) (eFigure 4 in Supplement 1). In both data sets, there were no differences in the proportion of each diagnosis between the leaves.
In subsequent layers of the dendrogram, consistent results were also observed with respect to cluster splits resulting in differences in hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms as measured by SWAN-HI. In the third layer of the POND dendrogram, a significant difference in SWAN-HI (U = 1.19 × 104; P = .01; η2 = 0.02) was identified between the 2 leaf subgroups (C and D), with subgroup D having increased hyperactivity and impulsivity traits compared with subgroup C (median [IQR], 2.50 [0.00-7.00] vs 1.00 [0.00-5.00]). In the HBN dendrogram, the leaves in the third layer (subgroups c and d) also showed a difference in hyperactivity and impulsivity, but the difference was not statistically significant (U = 1.13 × 104; P = .06; η2 = 0.01). A significant difference in symptoms was observed in the fifth layer (subgroups g and h: U = 2.68 × 103; P = .02; η2 = 0.03). To contrast with POND, the 4 HBN subgroups (c, d, g, and h) were compared, and a difference in hyperactivity and impulsivity problems was identified (W = 10.81; P = .01; η2 = 0.02), with post hoc tests identifying that subgroup g had significantly higher hyperactivity and impulsivity compared with subgroup d (median [IQR], 1.00 [0.00-4.00] vs 0.00 [0.00-2.00]; corrected P = .02). Thus, POND subgroup D and HBN subgroup g were identified as having increased SWAN-HI scores compared with POND subgroup C and HBN subgroup d (eFigure 4 in Supplement 1). Differences in the brain measures were compared between the 2 subgroups for both POND and HBN (Figure 3C and D; eTable 13 in Supplement 1). In both data sets, we observed increased segregation in all networks in the subgroup with higher hyperactivity and impulsivity, with the largest effect sizes occurring in the somatomotor and default mode networks in both data sets, along with the dorsal attention network in HBN. Increased integration in the motor and default mode networks and decreased integration in the frontoparietal and subcortical networks were also observed in both data sets. Despite these subgroups differing in symptoms associated with ADHD, we observed no differences in the proportion of each diagnosis between the subgroups in either data set.
In subsequent layers of the POND dendrogram, we observed differences in the proportion of diagnoses between the leaf clusters in the 4- and 5-cluster solutions. However, these diagnostic differences were not replicated in the HBN data set.
Discussion
In this study, we used measures derived from the brain’s functional networks to identify data-driven transdiagnostic subgroups of children and adolescents with and without neurodevelopmental conditions to characterize the heterogeneity across the conditions; these data-driven subgroups were then described using demographic and clinical indices. We identified subgroups in 2 independently collected data sets—POND and HBN—and, focusing on findings that were present in both cohorts, found subgroups that differed in intelligence and hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms but not diagnosis.
Research on neurodevelopmental conditions has classically operated under the assumption that diagnostic labels are the ground truth. However, there is increasing awareness of the biological and symptom heterogeneity within conditions and overlap across conditions, which has raised concerns about the appropriateness of service provision systems in health care that are based on diagnostic labels.50,51 Our work joins the growing body of literature supporting transdiagnostic approaches for accommodating the variability and complexity of these conditions and provides support for categorizing individuals on biology to identify better targets for treatments and interventions.
Furthermore, to our knowledge, we are the first to replicate our discovered transdiagnostic subgroups across 2 independently collected data sets, showing that similar subgroups with specific brain signatures can be identified that are accompanied by replicable phenotypic differences. With a mounting body of work challenging the reproducibility of brain-behavior relations13,34 and brain-based subtyping,52 replicating clustering results is essential to establish the robustness necessary to translate the groupings to clinical settings, particularly given the heterogeneity in both brain and behavior in neurodevelopmental conditions. While previous studies have established transdiagnostic subgroupings using neuroimaging,9,12,27,28,30,31,32,53 these results should be interpreted cautiously in the absence of replication. We explicitly address their shortcomings by evaluating subgroup replicability in 2 independent data sets.
Our discovered brain-based subgroups spanned the spectrum of neurodiversity, including typical development, and do not align with existing categorical boundaries. Specifically, we identified subgroups with similar biology that differed significantly in intelligence and hyperactivity and impulsivity problems yet showed no consistent alignment with the current diagnostic categories. The identification of neurobiologically defined subgroups that align poorly with the current behavior-based diagnostic categories contributes to the work criticizing the lack of correspondence of these categorical descriptors with biology.9,11,27,28,30,32,53 The TD individuals were also spread across all identified brain-based subgroups, emphasizing that an overlap in neurobiology exists not only across conditions, but also across typical development, aligning with emerging studies highlighting the similarities, rather than differences, in resting-state brain function between populations with neurodevelopmental conditions and TD populations.9,10,54
We identified replicable subgroups differing in intelligence, between which no differences in diagnosis (including those with no diagnosis) were observed. The distribution of each diagnosis across the subgroups aligned with the diversity in traits observed in neurodevelopmental conditions. For example, although neurodevelopmental conditions demonstrate overall reduced intelligence compared with their TD peers,55,56,57 those with ASD have been shown to also have a higher probability of scoring in the superior intelligence range.55 We also identified subgroups in both data sets who differed in hyperactivity and impulsivity traits. Even though these traits are traditionally considered characteristic of ADHD, we did not observe a difference in proportion of diagnostic categorization between these subgroups in either data set. This is consistent with the finding that hyperactivity and impulsivity are shared characteristics across neurodevelopmental conditions.58,59,60 The identification of subgroups that differ in these behavioral characteristics, rather than by diagnosis, supports using continuous measures of behavior to study neurodevelopmental differences rather than relying on the current discrete categorical categories.
These differences in intelligence and hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms were accompanied by pervasive differences in the brain’s functional segregation and integration. Networks involved in intelligence are distributed throughout the brain to support distinct information processing stages.61 Similarly, there was no convergence in the spatial patterns of functional brain connectivity associated with ADHD in the literature,17 and ADHD traits have been reported to be more associated with brainwide connectivity than with local connectivity.62 Thus, our findings support the distributed involvement of brain regions in intelligence and hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms.
We observed increased segregation in all brain networks coupled with predominantly increased integration in the subgroups with increased intelligence. Simultaneous increases in both segregation and integration occurs throughout development as hubs in the brain’s network shift from primary to cognitive brain regions to support cognitive development.63,64 Opposite to the other networks, the integration of the frontoparietal control and subcortical networks was decreased in the subgroup with increased intelligence. The specific pattern of involvement in these brain networks in intelligence aligns with the parieto-frontal integration theory of intelligence61 that has been extended to include subcortical structures.65,66
The subgroup with increased hyperactivity and impulsivity demonstrated widespread increased segregation and patterns of both increased (eg, motor) and decreased (eg, subcortical) integration. To our knowledge, no study has identified alterations in the topography of the brain’s resting-state functional network that are specific to hyperactivity and impulsivity in ADHD. The few studies examining the associations between hyperactivity and impulsivity and functional connectivity have implicated connections between striatal regions and regions in the motor network.67,68,69 The significant and opposite associations of integration we observed in these networks reinforces their specific involvement to hyperactivity and impulsivity symptoms.
Limitations
This study has limitations. Our study focused on brain function; however, differences between individuals with and without neurodevelopmental conditions have been observed in measures of both brain function and structure as well as in other domains. Thus, our study is limited by focusing on only one aspect of the brain, and our identified subgroups may not be homogeneous in other measures. There was a limited number of participants with OCD compared with the other diagnostic groups, and most behavioral measures used to characterize the data-driven subgroups were parent-rated reports, which may not be impartial. Furthermore, our age range is restricted to children and adolescents, a developmental period when significant changes are occurring; future work should extend the age range into adulthood and examine how the identified subgroups change throughout development. We have also only used a single 5-minute resting-state scan from the HBN data set when 2 were available and passed quality control; future work could evaluate the within-participant stability of our subgroups between multiple resting-state scans. The removal of nuisance covariates (age, sex, head motion, and acquisition scanner) also may inadvertently remove signal of interest yet was necessary to ensure subgroups were not defined by these covariates. Additionally, the study design was cross-sectional, and future studies should incorporate longitudinal data to examine the stability of the clusters over time. It is important to note that the findings of this study are based on neurobiological profiles quantified through measurements of brain function. As such, these results do not reflect broader considerations for existing diagnostic categories including issues of self-identity and service provision. Consultations and partnerships with neurodiverse populations are needed to appropriately contextualize and translate these findings into clinical practice. We recognize the different language preferences for referring to autistic identity (identity-first language and person-first language). We use both in this paper to reflect the diversity of perspectives.
Conclusions
To our knowledge, this is the first study to identify transdiagnostic subgroups replicated across 2 independent data sets. With the reliability of associations between brain and behavior being increasingly questioned in the literature, stratification techniques are a useful way of increasing power by identifying more homogeneous subgroups within the sample to target treatments and interventions. The replication of exact subgroups across different samples with varying diagnostic and behavioral characteristics is an essential step in ensuring robustness prior to implementing the groupings into clinical settings. Finally, our study suggests that homogeneity in neurobiology transcends diagnostic boundaries, promoting a shift in the research community away from classic case-control designs that rely on diagnostic categories, which have increasingly been shown not to reflect distinct biological and phenotypic constructs.
References
- 1.American Psychiatric Association . Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Jacob S, Wolff JJ, Steinbach MS, Doyle CB, Kumar V, Elison JT. Neurodevelopmental heterogeneity and computational approaches for understanding autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):63. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0390-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wåhlstedt C, Thorell LB, Bohlin G. Heterogeneity in ADHD: neuropsychological pathways, comorbidity and symptom domains. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(4):551-564. doi: 10.1007/s10802-008-9286-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Lochner C, Stein DJ. Heterogeneity of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a literature review. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2003;11(3):113-132. doi: 10.1080/10673220303949 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Lai MC, Kassee C, Besney R, et al. Prevalence of co-occurring mental health diagnoses in the autism population: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(10):819-829. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30289-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hollingdale J, Woodhouse E, Young S, Fridman A, Mandy W. Autistic spectrum disorder symptoms in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytical review. Psychol Med. 2020;50(13):2240-2253. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719002873 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ruzzano L, Borsboom D, Geurts HM. Repetitive behaviors in autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder: new perspectives from a network analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(1):192-202. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2204-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Baribeau DA, Doyle-Thomas KAR, Dupuis A, et al. Examining and comparing social perception abilities across childhood-onset neurodevelopmental disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(6):479-86.e1. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.03.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Choi EJ, Vandewouw MM, Taylor MJ, et al. Beyond diagnosis: cross-diagnostic features in canonical resting-state networks in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 2020;28:102476. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102476 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Spronk M, Keane BP, Ito T, et al. A whole-brain and cross-diagnostic perspective on functional brain network dysfunction. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(1):547-561. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa242 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Dajani DR, Burrows CA, Odriozola P, et al. Investigating functional brain network integrity using a traditional and novel categorical scheme for neurodevelopmental disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;21:101678. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101678 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dajani DR, Burrows CA, Nebel MB, Mostofsky SH, Gates KM, Uddin LQ. Parsing heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with individual connectome mapping. Brain Connect. 2019;9(9):673-691. doi: 10.1089/brain.2019.0669 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Marek S, Tervo-Clemmens B, Calabro FJ, et al. Reproducible brain-wide association studies require thousands of individuals. Nature. 2022;603(7902):654-660. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-04492-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Di Martino A, Yan CG, Li Q, et al. The autism brain imaging data exchange: towards a large-scale evaluation of the intrinsic brain architecture in autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(6):659-667. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.78 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Gao Y, Shuai D, Bu X, et al. Impairments of large-scale functional networks in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Psychol Med. 2019;49(15):2475-2485. doi: 10.1017/S003329171900237X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Gürsel DA, Avram M, Sorg C, Brandl F, Koch K. Frontoparietal areas link impairments of large-scale intrinsic brain networks with aberrant fronto-striatal interactions in OCD: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2018;87:151-160. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cortese S, Aoki YY, Itahashi T, Castellanos FX, Eickhoff SB. Systematic review and meta-analysis: resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;60(1):61-75. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.08.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hull JV, Dokovna LB, Jacokes ZJ, Torgerson CM, Irimia A, Van Horn JD. Resting-state functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders: a review. Front Psychiatry. 2017;7(JAN):205. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hahamy A, Behrmann M, Malach R. The idiosyncratic brain: distortion of spontaneous connectivity patterns in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(2):302-309. doi: 10.1038/nn.3919 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Henry TR, Cohen JR. Dysfunctional brain network organization in neurodevelopmental disorders. In Munsell BC, Wu G, Bonilha L, Laurienti PJ, eds. Connectomics: Applications to Neuroimaging. Academic Press; 2018:83-100. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-813838-0.00005-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cohen JR, D’Esposito M. The segregation and integration of distinct brain networks and their relationship to cognition. J Neurosci. 2016;36(48):12083-12094. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2965-15.2016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shine JM, Poldrack RA. Principles of dynamic network reconfiguration across diverse brain states. Neuroimage. 2018;180(Pt B):396-405. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.08.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Deco G, Tononi G, Boly M, Kringelbach ML. Rethinking segregation and integration: contributions of whole-brain modelling. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(7):430-439. doi: 10.1038/nrn3963 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Armstrong CC, Moody TD, Feusner JD, et al. Graph-theoretical analysis of resting-state fMRI in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2016;193:175-184. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.12.071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rudie JD, Brown JA, Beck-Pancer D, et al. Altered functional and structural brain network organization in autism. Neuroimage Clin. 2012;2(1):79-94. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2012.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Wang Y, Zuo C, Xu Q, Liao S, Kanji M, Wang D. Altered resting functional network topology assessed using graph theory in youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2020;98:109796. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Kushki A, Cardy RE, Panahandeh S, et al. Cross-diagnosis structural correlates of autistic-like social communication differences. Cereb Cortex. 2021;31(11):5067-5076. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab142 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kushki A, Anagnostou E, Hammill C, et al. Examining overlap and homogeneity in ASD, ADHD, and OCD: a data-driven, diagnosis-agnostic approach. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):318. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0631-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(7):748-751. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kernbach JM, Satterthwaite TD, Bassett DS, et al. Shared endo-phenotypes of default mode dysfunction in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and autism spectrum disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 2018;8(1):133. doi: 10.1038/s41398-018-0179-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Safar K, Vandewouw MM, Pang EW, et al. Shared and distinct patterns of functional connectivity to emotional faces in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder children. Front Psychol. 2022;13:826527. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.826527 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Itahashi T, Fujino J, Sato T, et al. Neural correlates of shared sensory symptoms in autism and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Commun. 2020;2(2):fcaa186. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa186 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fröhlich H, Balling R, Beerenwinkel N, et al. From hype to reality: data science enabling personalized medicine. BMC Med. 2018;16(1):150. doi: 10.1186/s12916-018-1122-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Klapwijk ET, van den Bos W, Tamnes CK, Raschle NM, Mills KL. Opportunities for increased reproducibility and replicability of developmental neuroimaging. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;47:100902. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100902 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Brain-CODE. Accessed February 1, 2023. https://www.braincode.ca/
- 36.Alexander LM, Escalera J, Ai L, et al. An open resource for transdiagnostic research in pediatric mental health and learning disorders. Sci Data. 2017;4(1):170181. doi: 10.1038/sdata.2017.181 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Child Mind Institute. Healthy Brain Network. Accessed February 1, 2023. http://fcon_1000.projects.nitrc.org/indi/cmi_healthy_brain_network/
- 38.Schaefer A, Kong R, Gordon EM, et al. Local-global parcellation of the human cerebral cortex from intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Cereb Cortex. 2018;28(9):3095-3114. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Tian Y, Margulies DS, Breakspear M, Zalesky A. Topographic organization of the human subcortex unveiled with functional connectivity gradients. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(11):1421-1432. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-00711-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Luppi AI, Stamatakis EA. Combining network topology and information theory to construct representative brain networks. Netw Neurosci. 2021;5(1):96-124. doi: 10.1162/netn_a_00170 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Dimitriadis SI, Antonakakis M, Simos P, Fletcher JM, Papanicolaou AC. Data-driven topological filtering based on orthogonal minimal spanning trees: application to multigroup magnetoencephalography resting-state connectivity. Brain Connect. 2017;7(10):661-670. doi: 10.1089/brain.2017.0512 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage. 2010;52(3):1059-1069. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Brandes U. A faster algorithm for betweenness centrality. J Math Sociol. 2001;25(2):163-177. doi: 10.1080/0022250X.2001.9990249 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Wang B, Mezlini AM, Demir F, et al. Similarity network fusion for aggregating data types on a genomic scale. Nat Methods. 2014;11(3):333-337. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2810 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Jain AK. Dubes RC. Algorithms for Clustering Data. Prentice Hall; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Calinski T, Harabasz J. A dendrite method for cluster analysis. Commun Stat Simul Comput. 1974;3(1):1-27. doi: 10.1080/03610927408827101 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Gao LL, Bien J, Witten D. Selective inference for hierarchical clustering. J Am Stat Assoc. Published online October 11, 2022. doi: 10.1080/01621459.2022.2116331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gao L. Clusterpval: P-values for differences in means after clustering. Accessed February 1, 2023. https://www.lucylgao.com/clusterpval/
- 49.Cox DR, Snell EJ. Analysis of Binary Data. Second edition. Chapman & Hall; 1989. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Astle DE, Holmes J, Kievit R, Gathercole SE. Annual research review: the transdiagnostic revolution in neurodevelopmental disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2022;63(4):397-417. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13481 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Miller A, Shen J, Mâsse LC. Child functional characteristics explain child and family outcomes better than diagnosis: population-based study of children with autism or other neurodevelopmental disorders/disabilities. Health Rep. 2016;27(6):9-18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Dinga R, Schmaal L, Penninx BWJH, et al. Evaluating the evidence for biotypes of depression: methodological replication and extension of. Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101796. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101796 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Jacobs GR, Voineskos AN, Hawco C, et al. Integration of brain and behavior measures for identification of data-driven groups cutting across children with ASD, ADHD, or OCD. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2021;46(3):643-653. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00902-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.He Y, Byrge L, Kennedy DP. Nonreplication of functional connectivity differences in autism spectrum disorder across multiple sites and denoising strategies. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41(5):1334-1350. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24879 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Billeiter KB, Froiland JM. Diversity of intelligence is the norm within the autism spectrum: full scale intelligence scores among children with ASD. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. Published online January 27, 2022. doi: 10.1007/S10578-021-01300-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Frazier TW, Demaree HA, Youngstrom EA. Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. 2004;18(3):543-555. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.543 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Abramovitch A, Anholt G, Raveh-Gottfried S, Hamo N, Abramowitz JS. Meta-analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ) in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28(1):111-120. doi: 10.1007/s11065-017-9358-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Krakowski AD, Cost KT, Anagnostou E, et al. Inattention and hyperactive/impulsive component scores do not differentiate between autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinical sample. Mol Autism. 2020;11(1):28. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00338-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.van der Plas E, Dupuis A, Arnold P, Crosbie J, Schachar R. Association of autism spectrum disorder with obsessive-compulsive and attention-deficit/hyperactivity traits and response inhibition in a community sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(9):3115-3125. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2853-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Geller DA, Biederman J, Faraone SV, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder: fact or artifact? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2002;41(1):52-58. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200201000-00011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Jung RE, Haier RJ. The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence. Behav Brain Sci. 2007;30(2):135-154. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X07001185 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mooney MA, Hermosillo RJM, Feczko E, et al. Cumulative effects of resting-state connectivity across all brain networks significantly correlate with ADHD symptoms. medRxiv. Preprint posted online November 20, 2021. doi: 10.1101/2021.11.16.21266121 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 63.Cao M, Huang H, He Y. Developmental connectomics from infancy through early childhood. Trends Neurosci. 2017;40(8):494-506. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2017.06.003 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Luna B, Marek S, Larsen B, Tervo-Clemmens B, Chahal R. An integrative model of the maturation of cognitive control. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2015;38:151-170. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071714-034054 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Basten U, Stelzel C, Fiebach CJ. Intelligence is differentially related to neural effort in the task-positive and the task-negative brain network. Intelligence. 2013;41(5):517-528. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2013.07.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Basten U, Hilger K, Fiebach CJ. Where smart brains are different: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional and structural brain imaging studies on intelligence. Intelligence. 2015;51:10-27. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2015.04.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Oldehinkel M, Beckmann CF, Pruim RHR, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms coincide with altered striatal connectivity. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2016;1(4):353-363. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.03.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Sörös P, Hoxhaj E, Borel P, et al. Hyperactivity/restlessness is associated with increased functional connectivity in adults with ADHD: a dimensional analysis of resting state fMRI. BMC Psychiatry. 2019;19(1):43. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2031-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Chen Y, Li G, Ide JS, Luo X, Li CSR. Sex differences in attention deficit hyperactivity symptom severity and functional connectivity of the dorsal striatum in young adults. Neuroimage Rep. 2021;1(2):100025. doi: 10.1016/j.ynirp.2021.100025 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.